Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-64p75 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-29T04:54:47.249Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 24 - Empathy and Compassion

from Section VI - Social Emotions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2025

Jorge Armony
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Patrik Vuilleumier
Affiliation:
University of Geneva
Get access

Summary

In this chapter, we focus on the neuronal networks underlying the socio-affective capacities empathy and compassion. We first provide definitions of empathy and compassion and give an overview of the historical development in social neuroscience related to empathy and compassion research, with a focus on differentiating between empathy, empathic distress, compassion, and related concepts of social understanding like Theory of Mind. We then examine the neuronal networks underlying these distinct social capacities and discuss the latest discoveries in this field. Next, we turn to the plasticity of the social brain and compare training approaches in their efficacy in improving socio-affective and socio-cognitive capacities. This is followed by the exploration of how psychopathological symptoms are differentially related to empathy, compassion, and socio-cognitive skills. Lastly, we conclude the main findings of this chapter and provide questions for future neuroscientific and psychological research on empathy and compassion.

Information

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Book purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Adolphs, R. (2002). Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: Psychological and neurological mechanisms. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 1, 21–62.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreou, M., & Skrimpa, V. (2020). Theory of mind deficits and neurophysiological operations in autism spectrum disorders: A review. Brain Sciences, 10, 393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashar, Y. K., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Dimidjian, S., & Wager, T. D. (2017). Empathic care and distress: Predictive brain markers and dissociable brain systems. Neuron, 94, 1263–1273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartels, A., & Zeki, S. (2004). The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage, 21, 1155–1166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D. (1991). The altruism question: Toward a social-psychological answer. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 3–15). Boston Review.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., Courtemanche, J., Paquette, V., & St-Pierre, É. L. (2009). The neural basis of unconditional love. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 172, 93–98.Google ScholarPubMed
Bernhardt, B. C., Valk, S. L., Silani, G., Bird, G., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2014). Selective disruption of sociocognitive structural brain networks in autism and alexithymia. Cerebral Cortex, 24, 3258–3267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, G., & Cook, R. (2013). Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Translational Psychiatry, 3, e285.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, G., Silani, G., Brindley, R., White, S., Frith, U., & Singer, T. (2010). Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism. Brain, 133, 1515–1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, G., & Viding, E. (2014). The self to other model of empathy: Providing a new framework for understanding empathy impairments in psychopathy, autism, and alexithymia. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 520–532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Böckler, A., Tusche, A., Schmidt, P., & Singer, T. (2018). Distinct mental trainings differentially affect altruistically motivated, norm motivated, and self-reported prosocial behaviour. Scientific Reports, 8, 13560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bodnar, A., & Rybakowski, J. K. (2017). Increased affective empathy in bipolar patients during a manic episode. Brazilian Journal of Psychiatry, 39, 342–345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bonfils, K. A., Lysaker, P. H., Minor, K. S., & Salyers, M. P. (2016). Affective empathy in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 175, 109–117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bora, E. (2022). Social cognition and empathy in adults with obsessive compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 316, 114752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Byrne, R. W., & Whiten, A. (1991). Computation and mindreading in primate tactical deception. In Whiten, A. (Ed.), Natural theories of mind: Evolution, development and simulation of everyday mindreading (pp. 127–141). Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bzdok, D., Schilbach, L., Vogeley, K., Schneider, K., Laird, A. R., Langner, R., & Eickhoff, S. B. (2012). Parsing the neural correlates of moral cognition: ALE meta-analysis on morality, theory of mind, and empathy. Brain Structure and Function, 217, 783–796.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T. (1994). Social neuroscience: Autonomic, neuroendocrine, and immune responses to stress. Psychophysiology, 31, 113–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., & Berntson, G. G. (1992). Social psychological contributions to the decade of the brain: Doctrine of multilevel analysis. American Psychologist, 47, 1019–1028.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carrillo, M., Han, Y., Migliorati, F., Liu, M., Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2019). Emotional mirror neurons in the rat’s anterior cingulate cortex. Current Biology, 29, 1301–1312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chierchia, G., Przyrembel, M., Lesemann, F. P., Bosworth, S., Snower, D., & Singer, T. (2021). Navigating motivation: A semantic and subjective Atlas of 7 motives. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 568064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Condon, P. (2017). Mindfulness, compassion, and prosocial behaviour. In Karremans, J. C. & Papies, E. K. (Eds.), Mindfulness in social psychology (pp. 124–138). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Condon, P., Desbordes, G., Miller, W. B., & DeSteno, D. (2013). Meditation increases compassionate responses to suffering. Psychological Science, 24, 2125–2127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corradi-Dell’Acqua, C., Tusche, A., Vuilleumier, P., & Singer, T. (2016). Cross-modal representations of first-hand and vicarious pain, disgust and fairness in insular and cingulate cortex. Nature Communications, 7, 10904.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8, 144–153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czarna, A. Z., Wróbel, M., Dufner, M., & Zeigler-Hill, V. (2015). Narcissism and emotional contagion: Do narcissists “catch” the emotions of others? Social Psychological and Personality Science, 6, 318–324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czarna, A. Z., Zajenkowski, M., & Dufner, M. (2018). How does it feel to be a narcissist? Narcissism and emotions. In Hermann, A. D., Brunell, A. B., & Foster, J. D. (Eds.), Handbook of trait narcissism (pp. 255–263). Springer.Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1983). Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 113–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M. H., Conklin, L., Smith, A., & Luce, C. (1996). Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 713–726.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J. (2010). The neurodevelopment of empathy in humans. Developmental Neuroscience, 32, 257–267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C., & Kiehl, K. (2013). An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: Imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3, 71–100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Skelly, L. R., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy. JAMA Psychiatry, 70, 638–645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derntl, B., Seidel, E.-M., Schneider, F., & Habel, U. (2012). How specific are emotional deficits? A comparison of empathic abilities in schizophrenia, bipolar and depressed patients. Schizophrenia Research, 142, 58–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 435–441.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dinsdale, N., & Crespi, B. J. (2013). The borderline empathy paradox: Evidence and conceptual models for empathic enhancements in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 27, 172–195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992). Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain Research, 91, 176–180.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donald, J. N., Sahdra, B. K., Van Zanden, B., Duineveld, J. J., Atkins, P. W. B., Marshall, S. L., & Ciarrochi, J. (2019). Does your mindfulness benefit others? A systematic review and meta-analysis of the link between mindfulness and prosocial behaviour. British Journal of Psychology, 110, 101–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development, Vol. 3, 6th ed. (pp. 646–718). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Eres, R., Decety, J., Louis, W. R., & Molenberghs, P. (2015). Individual differences in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and cognitive empathy. NeuroImage, 117, 305–310.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fan, Y., Duncan, N. W., de Greck, M., & Northoff, G. (2011). Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 903–911.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fertuck, E. A., Fischer, S., & Beeney, J. (2018). Social cognition and borderline personality disorder: Splitting and trust impairment findings. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 41, 613–632.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P. C., Happé, F., Frith, U., Baker, S. C., Dolan, R. J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Other minds in the brain: A functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cognition, 57, 109–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox, K. C., Nijeboer, S., Dixon, M. L., Floman, J. L., Ellamil, M., Rumak, S. P., … Christoff, K. (2014). Is meditation associated with altered brain structure? A systematic review and meta-analysis of morphometric neuroimaging in meditation practitioners. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 43, 48–73.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2005). Theory of mind. Current Biology, 15, R644–R645.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U., & Frith, C. D. (2003). Development and neurophysiology of mentalizing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358, 459–473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, U., Morton, J., & Leslie, A. M. (1991). The cognitive basis of a biological disorder: Autism. Trends in Neurosciences, 14, 433–438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gaigg, S. B., Cornell, A. S., & Bird, G. (2018). The psychophysiological mechanisms of alexithymia in autism spectrum disorder. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 22, 227–231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallese, V., & Goldman, A. (1998). Mirror neurons and the simulation theory of mind-reading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2, 493–501.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, P. (2017). Compassion as a social mentality: An evolutionary approach. In Gilbert, P. (Ed.), Compassion: Concepts, research and applications (pp. 31–68). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351–374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J. A., & Schwartz, R. (2022). Empathy, an important but problematic concept. The Journal of Social Psychology, 162, 1–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanh, T. N. (2002). Teachings on love. Parallax Press.Google Scholar
Harmsen, I. E. (2019). Empathy in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49, 3939–3955.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1993). Emotional contagion. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 96–100.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J. (2000). Evolutionary perspectives on human motivation. American Behavioral Scientist, 43, 1015–1029.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heckhausen, J., & Heckhausen, H. (2010). Motivation und Entwicklung. In Heckhausen, J. & Heckhausen, H. (Eds.), Motivation und Handeln (pp. 427–488). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, F., Banzhaf, C., Kanske, P., Gärtner, M., Bermpohl, F., & Singer, T. (2016). Empathy in depression: Egocentric and altercentric biases and the role of alexithymia. Journal of Affective Disorders, 199, 23–29.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffmann, F., Koehne, S., Steinbeis, N., Dziobek, I., & Singer, T. (2016). Preserved self-other distinction during empathy in autism is linked to network integrity of right supramarginal Gyrus. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 637–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 849–878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jabbi, M., Swart, M., & Keysers, C. (2007). Empathy for positive and negative emotions in the gustatory cortex. NeuroImage, 34, 1744–1753.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jauniaux, J., Khatibi, A., Rainville, P., & Jackson, P. L. (2019). A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on pain empathy: Investigating the role of visual information and observers’ perspective. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 14, 789–813.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jinpa, T. (2015). A fearless heart. Why compassion is the key to greater well-being. Little Brown.Google Scholar
Joinson, C. (1992). Coping with compassion fatigue. Nursing, 22, 116–118.Google ScholarPubMed
Kang, D.-H., Jo, H. J., Jung, W. H., Kim, S. H., Jung, Y.-H., Choi, C.-H., … Kwon, J. S. (2013). The effect of meditation on brain structure: Cortical thickness mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8, 27–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P. (2018). The social mind: Disentangling affective and cognitive routes to understanding others. Interdisciplinary Science Reviews, 43, 115–124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., & Singer, T. (2015). Models, mechanisms and moderators dissociating empathy and theory of mind. In Wöhr, M. & Krach, S. (Eds.), Social behavior from rodents to humans: Neural foundations and clinical implications (pp. 193–206). Springer International Publishing.Google Scholar
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F. M., & Singer, T. (2015). Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain-behavior relations for empathy and Theory of Mind. NeuroImage, 122, 6–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanske, P., Böckler, A., Trautwein, F.-M., Parianen Lesemann, F. H., & Singer, T. (2016). Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11, 1383–1392.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kerr-Gaffney, J., Harrison, A., & Tchanturia, K. (2019). Cognitive and affective empathy in eating disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 102.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2021). Emotional contagion: Improving survival by preparing for socially sensed threats. Current Biology, 31, R728–R730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keysers, C., Knapska, E., Moita, M. A., & Gazzola, V. (2022). Emotional contagion and prosocial behavior in rodents. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 26, 688–706.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiehl, K. A. (2006). A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: Evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction. Psychiatry Research, 142, 107–128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2013). Functional neural plasticity and associated changes in positive affect after compassion training. Cerebral Cortex, 23, 1552–1561.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klimecki, O. M., Leiberg, S., Ricard, M., & Singer, T. (2013). Differential pattern of functional brain plasticity after compassion and empathy training. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9, 873–879.Google ScholarPubMed
Klimecki, O. M., & Singer, T. (2012). Empathic distress fatigue rather than compassion fatigue? Integrating findings from empathy research in psychology and social neuroscience. In Oakley, B., Knafo, A., Madhavan, G., & Wilson, D. S. (Eds.), Pathological altruism (pp. 368–383). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Krishnan, A., Woo, C.-W., Chang, L. J., Ruzic, L., Gu, X., López-Solà, M., … Wager, T. D. (2016). Somatic and vicarious pain are represented by dissociable multivariate brain patterns. eLife, 5, e15166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupferberg, A., Bicks, L., & Hasler, G. (2016). Social functioning in major depressive disorder. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 69, 313–332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Decety, J., & Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage, 54, 2492–2502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Rütgen, M., & Wagner, I. C. (2019). Imaging empathy and prosocial emotions. Functional Neuroimaging of the Emotional Brain, 693, 49–53.Google ScholarPubMed
Langford, D. J., Crager, S. E., Shehzad, Z., Smith, S. B., Sotocinal, S. G., Levenstadt, J. S., … Mogil, J. S. (2006). Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice. Science, 312, 1967–1970.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leiberg, S., Klimecki, O. M., & Singer, T. (2011). Short-term compassion training increases prosocial behavior in a newly developed prosocial game. PLoS ONE, 6, e17798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liakakis, G., Nickel, J., & Seitz, R. (2011). Diversity of the inferior frontal gyrus – A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Behavioural Brain Research, 225, 341–347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
López, A., Sanderman, R., Ranchor, A. V., & Schroevers, M. J. (2018). Compassion for others and self-compassion: Levels, correlates, and relationship with psychological well-being. Mindfulness, 9, 325–331.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luberto, C. M., Shinday, N., Song, R., Philpotts, L. L., Park, E. R., Fricchione, G. L., & Yeh, G. Y. (2018). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the effects of meditation on empathy, compassion, and prosocial behaviors. Mindfulness, 9, 708–724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lutz, A., Brefczynski-Lewis, J., Johnstone, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2008). Regulation of the neural circuitry of emotion by compassion meditation: Effects of meditative expertise. PLoS ONE, 3, e1897.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacBeth, A., & Gumley, A. (2012). Exploring compassion: A meta-analysis of the association between self-compassion and psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 32, 545–552.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, C. L., Morelli, S. A., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2011). An fMRI investigation of empathy for “social pain” and subsequent prosocial behavior. NeuroImage, 55, 381–388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meffert, H., Gazzola, V., den Boer, J. A., Bartels, A. A. J., & Keysers, C. (2013). Reduced spontaneous but relatively normal deliberate vicarious representations in psychopathy. Brain, 136, 2550–2562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, D. T. (1999). The norm of self-interest. American Psychologist, 54, 1053–1060.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, P. A., & Eisenberg, N. (1988). The relation of empathy to aggressive and externalizing/antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 324–344.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mohnke, S., Erk, S., Schnell, K., Schütz, C., Romanczuk-Seiferth, N., Grimm, O., … Schmitgen, M. M. (2014). Further evidence for the impact of a genome-wide-supported psychosis risk variant in ZNF804A on the theory of mind network. Neuropsychopharmacology, 39, 1196–1205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller-Pinzler, L., Gazzola, V., Keysers, C., Sommer, J., Jansen, A., Frässle, S., … Krach, S. (2015). Neural pathways of embarrassment and their modulation by social anxiety. NeuroImage, 119, 252–261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, H. A. (1938). Explorations in personality: A clinical and experimental study of fifty men of college age. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Nazeri, M., Nezhadi, A., & Shabani, M. (2019). Role of opioid system in empathy-like behaviours in rats. Addiction & Health, 11, 216–222.Google ScholarPubMed
Neff, K. (2003). Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward oneself. Self and Identity, 2, 85–101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. E., & Panksepp, J. (1998). Brain substrates of infant–mother attachment: Contributions of opioids, oxytocin, and norepinephrine. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 437–452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novak, L., Malinakova, K., Mikoska, P., van Dijk, J. P., & Tavel, P. (2022). Neural correlates of compassion – An integrative systematic review. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 172, 46–59.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56, 717–734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Panksepp, J. (2011). The basic emotional circuits of mammalian brains: Do animals have affective lives? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35, 1791–1804.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradiso, E., Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2021). Neural mechanisms necessary for empathy-related phenomena across species. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 68, 107–115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittelkow, M.-M., Aan Het Rot, M., Seidel, L. J., Feyel, N., & Roest, A. M. (2021). Social anxiety and empathy: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 78, 102357.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1, 515–526.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preston, S. D. (2013). The origins of altruism in offspring care. Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1305–1341.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preston, S. D., & De Waal, F. B. (2002). Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 25, 1–20.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Prinz, W. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 9, 129–154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quaglia, J. T., Soisson, A., & Simmer-Brown, J. (2021). Compassion for self versus other: A critical review of compassion training research. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 16, 675–690.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ricard, M. (2015). Altruism: The power of compassion to change itself and the world. London Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
Ritter, K., Dziobek, I., Preißler, S., Rüter, A., Vater, A., Fydrich, T., … Roepke, S. (2011). Lack of empathy in patients with narcissistic personality disorder. Psychiatry Research, 187, 241–247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron system. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rizzolatti, G., & Luppino, G. (2001). The cortical motor system. Neuron, 31, 889–901.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rogers, K., Dziobek, I., Hassenstab, J., Wolf, O. T., & Convit, A. (2007). Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 709–715.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rütgen, M., Seidel, E.-M., Pletti, C., Riěanský, I., Gartus, A., Eisenegger, C., & Lamm, C. (2018). Psychopharmacological modulation of event-related potentials suggests that first-hand pain and empathy for pain rely on similar opioidergic processes. Neuropsychologia, 116, 5–14.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “Theory of Mind.” In Cacioppo, J. T. & Berntson, G. G. (Eds.), Social Neuroscience: Key readings (pp. 171–182). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Schreiter, S., Pijnenborg, G., & Aan Het Rot, M. (2013). Empathy in adults with clinical or subclinical depressive symptoms. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 1–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schurz, M., Maliske, L., & Kanske, P. (2020). Cross-network interactions in social cognition: A review of findings on task related brain activation and connectivity. Cortex, 130, 142–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9–34.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Silani, G., Bird, G., Brindley, R., Singer, T., Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2008). Levels of emotional awareness and autism: An fMRI study. Social Neuroscience, 3, 97–112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T. (2006). The neuronal basis and ontogeny of empathy and mind reading: Review of literature and implications for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 855–863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T. (2012). The past, present and future of social neuroscience: A European perspective. NeuroImage, 61, 437–449.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., & Engert, V. (2019). It matters what you practice: Differential training effects on subjective experience, behavior, brain and body in the ReSource Project. Current Opinion in Psychology, 28, 151–158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., & Klimecki, O. M. (2014). Empathy and compassion. Current Biology, 24, R875–R878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., Kok, B. E., Bornemann, B., Bolz, M., & Bochow, C. (2016). The ReSource project: Background, design, samples, and measurements, 2nd ed. Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences.Google Scholar
Singer, T., & Lamm, C. (2009). The social neuroscience of empathy. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 81–96.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004). Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain. Science, 303, 1157–1162.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, T., & Steinbeis, N. (2009). Differential roles of fairness‐and compassion‐based motivations for cooperation, defection, and punishment. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 41–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, M. L., Asada, N., & Malenka, R. C. (2021). Anterior cingulate inputs to nucleus accumbens control the social transfer of pain and analgesia. Science, 371, 153–159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soyman, E., Bruls, R., Ioumpa, K., Müller-Pinzler, L., Gallo, S., Qin, C., … Gazzola, V. (2022). Intracranial human recordings reveal association between neural activity and perceived intensity for the pain of others in the insula. eLife, 11, e75197.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stevens, F., & Taber, K. (2021). The neuroscience of empathy and compassion in pro-social behavior. Neuropsychologia, 159, 107925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stietz, J., Jauk, E., Krach, S., & Kanske, P. (2019). Dissociating empathy from perspective-taking: Evidence from intra- and inter-individual differences research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, 126.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strauss, C., Taylor, B. L., Gu, J., Kuyken, W., Baer, R., Jones, F., & Cavanagh, K. (2016). What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures. Clinical Psychology Review, 47, 15–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoma, P., Friedmann, C., & Suchan, B. (2013). Empathy and social problem solving in alcohol dependence, mood disorders and selected personality disorders. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 448–470.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timmers, I., Park, A. L., Fischer, M. D., Kronman, C. A., Heathcote, L. C., Hernandez, J. M., & Simons, L. E. (2018). Is empathy for pain unique in its neural correlates? A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of empathy. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 12, 289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tully, E. C., Ames, A. M., Garcia, S. E., & Donohue, M. R. (2016). Quadratic associations between empathy and depression as moderated by emotion dysregulation. The Journal of Psychology, 150, 15–35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trautwein, F., Kanske, P., Böckler-Raettig, A., & Singer, T. (2020). Differential benefits of mental training types for attention, compassion, and theory of mind. Cognition, 194, 104039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valk, S. L., Bernhardt, B. C., Trautwein, F. M., Böckler, A., Kanske, P., Guizard, N., … Singer, T. (2017). Structural plasticity of the social brain: Differential change after socio-affective and cognitive mental training. Science Advances, 3, e1700489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wai, M., & Tiliopoulos, N. (2012). The affective and cognitive empathic nature of the dark triad of personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 52, 794–799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinberger, J., Cotler, T., & Fishman, D. (2010). The duality of affiliative motivation. In Schultheiss, O. & Brunstein, J. (Eds.), Implicit motives (p. 71–88). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J.-P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron, 40, 655–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winter, K., Spengler, S., Bermpohl, F., Singer, T., & Kanske, P. (2017). Social cognition in aggressive offenders: Impaired empathy, but intact theory of mind. Scientific Reports, 7, 670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolkenstein, L., Schönenberg, M., Schirm, E., & Hautzinger, M. (2011). I can see what you feel, but I can’t deal with it: Impaired theory of mind in depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 132, 104–111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yan, Z., Hong, S., Liu, F., & Su, Y. (2020). A meta-analysis of the relationship between empathy and executive function. PsyCh Journal, 9, 34–43.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, F., Li, J., Zhao, W., Xu, L., Zheng, X., Fu, M., … Becker, B. (2020). Empathic pain evoked by sensory and emotional-communicative cues share common and process-specific neural representations. eLife, 9, e56929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Accessibility standard: WCAG 2.0 A

The PDF of this book conforms to version 2.0 of the Web Content Accessibility Guidelines (WCAG), ensuring core accessibility principles are addressed and meets the basic (A) level of WCAG compliance, addressing essential accessibility barriers.

Content Navigation

Table of contents navigation
Allows you to navigate directly to chapters, sections, or non‐text items through a linked table of contents, reducing the need for extensive scrolling.
Index navigation
Provides an interactive index, letting you go straight to where a term or subject appears in the text without manual searching.

Reading Order & Textual Equivalents

Single logical reading order
You will encounter all content (including footnotes, captions, etc.) in a clear, sequential flow, making it easier to follow with assistive tools like screen readers.
Short alternative textual descriptions
You get concise descriptions (for images, charts, or media clips), ensuring you do not miss crucial information when visual or audio elements are not accessible.
Full alternative textual descriptions
You get more than just short alt text: you have comprehensive text equivalents, transcripts, captions, or audio descriptions for substantial non‐text content, which is especially helpful for complex visuals or multimedia.

Visual Accessibility

Use of high contrast between text and background colour
You benefit from high‐contrast text, which improves legibility if you have low vision or if you are reading in less‐than‐ideal lighting conditions.

Structural and Technical Features

ARIA roles provided
You gain clarity from ARIA (Accessible Rich Internet Applications) roles and attributes, as they help assistive technologies interpret how each part of the content functions.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge-org.demo.remotlog.com is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×