Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-2ptsp Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-09-28T01:05:14.008Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 10 - Body of Knowledge

The Emerging Science of Emotional Body Expressions

from Section III - Emotion Perception and Elicitation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 September 2025

Jorge Armony
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Patrik Vuilleumier
Affiliation:
University of Geneva
Get access

Summary

When thinking about emotional expressions, most would probably envision facial expressions (e.g., smiling, scowling) or vocalizations (e.g., crying, laughter). Here we focus on the emotional postures and movements of the body – an important, but fairly understudied, signal for emotion perception. During emotional episodes, humans often position and move their bodies in consistent ways that may (or may not) signal their underlying feelings and future actions. We briefly review the historical antecedents of this literature, as well as current knowledge on the neural processing, developmental trajectory, and cultural differences in the emotional perception of body language. We continue by examining the role of the body as a contextualizing agent for disambiguating facial expressions, as well as their inverse relationship – from faces to bodies. Future directions and speculations about how this emerging field may evolve are discussed.

Information

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Book purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Abramson, L., Marom, I., Petranker, R., & Aviezer, H. (2016). Is fear in your head? A comparison of instructed and real-life expressions of emotion in the face and body. Emotion, 17, 557–565.Google Scholar
Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atkinson, A. P., Tunstall, M. L., & Dittrich, W. H. (2007). Evidence for distinct contributions of form and motion information to the recognition of emotions from body gestures. Cognition, 104, 59–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aviezer, H., Ensenberg, N., & Hassin, R. R. (2017). The inherently contextualized nature of facial emotion perception. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 47–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Messinger, D. S., Zangvil, S., Mattson, W. I., Gangi, D. N., & Todorov, A. (2015). Thrill of victory or agony of defeat? Perceivers fail to utilize information in facial movements. Emotion, 15, 791–797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aviezer, H., Trope, Y., & Todorov, A. (2012). Body cues, not facial expressions, discriminate between intense positive and negative emotions. Science, 338, 1225–1229.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Adolphs, R., Marsella, S., Martinez, A. M., & Pollak, S. D. (2019). Emotional expressions reconsidered: Challenges to inferring emotion from human facial movements. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 20, 1–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chronaki, G., Hadwin, J. A., Garner, M., Maurage, P., & Sonuga‐Barke, E. J. (2015). The development of emotion recognition from facial expressions and non‐linguistic vocalizations during childhood. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33, 218–236.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2020). What the face displays: Mapping 28 emotions conveyed by naturalistic expression. American Psychologist, 75, 349.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., Keltner, D., Schroff, F., Jou, B., Adam, H., & Prasad, G. (2021). Sixteen facial expressions occur in similar contexts worldwide. Nature, 589, 251–257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crivelli, C., Russell, J. A., Jarillo, S., & Fernández-Dols, J.-M. (2016). The fear gasping face as a threat display in a Melanesian society. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113, 12403–12407.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions of man and animals. John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dael, N., Bianchi-Berthouze, N., Kleinsmith, A., & Mohr, C. (2016). Measuring body movement: Current and future directions in proxemics and kinesics. In Matsumoto, D., Hwang, H. C., & Frank, M. G. (Eds.), APA handbook of nonverbal communication (pp. 551–587). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
de Gelder, B. (2009). Why bodies? Twelve reasons for including bodily expressions in affective neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 364, 3475–3484.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Gelder, B., Snyder, J., Greve, D., Gerard, G., & Hadjikhani, N. (2004). Fear fosters flight: A mechanism for fear contagion when perceiving emotion expressed by a whole body. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 16701–16706.Google ScholarPubMed
de Gelder, B., & Van den Stock, J. (2011). The Bodily Expressive Action Stimulus Test (BEAST). Construction and validation of a stimulus basis for measuring perception of whole body expression of emotions. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Gelder, B., Van den Stock, J., Meeren, H. K., Sinke, C. B., Kret, M. E., & Tamietto, M. (2010). Standing up for the body. Recent progress in uncovering the networks involved in the perception of bodies and bodily expressions. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 513–527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Meijer, M. (1989). The contribution of general features of body movement to the attribution of emotions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 13, 247–268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Downing, P. E., Jiang, Y., Shuman, M., & Kanwisher, N. (2001). A cortical area selective for visual processing of the human body. Science, 293, 2470–2473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. (1965). Differential communication of affect by head and body cues. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 2, 726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ekman, P. & Friesen, W. V. (1971). Constants across cultures in the face and emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 17, 124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elfenbein, H. A., Beaupré, M., Lévesque, M., & Hess, U. (2007). Toward a dialect theory: cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expressions. Emotion, 7, 131.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Crivelli, C. (2013). Emotion and expression: Naturalistic studies. Emotion Review, 5, 24–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernández-Dols, J.-M., & Ruiz-Belda, M.-A. (1995). Are smiles a sign of happiness? Gold medal winners at the Olympic Games. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1113–1119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrari, C., Ciricugno, A., Urgesi, C., & Cattaneo, Z. (2022). Cerebellar contribution to emotional body language perception: A TMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 17, 81–90.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grèzes, J., Adenis, M. S., Pouga, L., & Armony, J. L. (2013). Self-relevance modulates brain responses to angry body expressions. Cortex, 48, 2210–2220.Google Scholar
Grosbras, M. H., & Paus, T. (2006). Brain networks involved in viewing angry hands or faces. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1087–1096.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadjikhani, N., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Seeing fearful body expressions activates the fusiform cortex and amygdala. Current Biology, 13, 2201–2205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heck, A., Chroust, A., White, H., Jubran, R., & Bhatt, R. S. (2018). Development of body emotion perception in infancy: From discrimination to recognition. Infant Behavior and Development, 50, 42–51.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herba, C. M., Landau, S., Russell, T., Ecker, C., & Phillips, M. L. (2006). The development of emotion‐processing in children: Effects of age, emotion, and intensity. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47, 1098–1106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinzman, L., & Kelly, S. D. (2013). Effects of emotional body language on rapid out-group judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49, 152–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hu, Y. & O’Toole, A. (2022). Integrating faces and bodies in social trait perception. Journal of Vision, 22, 3942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. T. (1932). A study of the expression of bodily posture. The Journal of General Psychology, 7, 405–437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleinsmith, A., De Silva, P. R., & Bianchi-Berthouze, N. (2006). Cross-cultural differences in recognizing affect from body posture. Interacting with Computers, 18, 1371–1389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koppensteiner, M., Stephan, P., & Jäschke, J. P. M. (2016). Moving speeches: Dominance, trustworthiness and competence in body motion. Personality and Individual Differences, 94, 101–106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kret, M., Pichon, S., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2011). Similarities and differences in perceiving threat from dynamic faces and bodies. An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 54, 1755–1762.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kret, M., Stekelenburg, J. J., Roelofs, K., & de Gelder, B. (2013). Perception of face and body expressions using electromyography, pupillometry and gaze measures. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 28.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kudoh, T., & Matsumoto, D. (1985). Cross-cultural examination of the semantic dimensions of body postures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecker, M., & Aviezer, H. (2021). More than words? Semantic emotion labels boost context effects on faces. Affective Science, 2, 163–170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lecker, M., Dotsch, R., Bijlstra, G., & Aviezer, H. (2020). Bidirectional contextual influence between faces and bodies in emotion perception. Emotion, 20, 1154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D., & Kudoh, T. (1987). Cultural similarities and differences in the semantic dimensions of body postures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 11, 166–179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meeren, H. K., Hadjikhani, N., Ahlfors, S. P., Hämäläinen, M. S., & de Gelder, B. (2016). Early preferential responses to fear stimuli in human right dorsal visual stream—a MEG study. Scientific Reports, 6, 24831.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meeren, H. K. M., van Heijnsbergen, C. C. R. J., & de Gelder, B. (2005). Rapid perceptual integration of facial expression and emotional body language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 16518–16523.Google ScholarPubMed
Missana, M., Atkinson, A. P., & Grossmann, T. (2015). Tuning the developing brain to emotional body expressions. Developmental Science, 18, 243–253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Montepare, J., Koff, E., Zaitchik, D., & Albert, M. (1999). The use of body movements and gestures as cues to emotions in younger and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23, 133–152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Motley, M. T., & Camden, C. T. (1988). Facial expression of emotion: A comparison of posed expressions versus spontaneous expressions in an interpersonal communication setting. Western Journal of Communication, 52, 1–22.Google Scholar
Naab, P. J., & Russell, J. A. (2007). Judgments of emotion from spontaneous facial expressions of New Guineans. Emotion, 7, 736–744.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, N. L., & Russell, J. A. (2011). Preschoolers’ use of dynamic facial, bodily, and vocal cues to emotion. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 52–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peelen, M. V., Atkinson, A. P., Andersson, F., & Vuilleumier, P. (2007). Emotional modulation of body-selective visual areas. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 274–283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peelen, M. V., & Downing, P. E. (2007). The neural basis of visual body perception. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8, 636–648.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., & Grèzes, J. (2008). Two different faces of threat. Comparing the neural systems for recognizing fear and anger in dynamic body expressions. NeuroImage, 47, 1873–1883.Google Scholar
Pichon, S., de Gelder, B., & Grèzes, J. (2009). Emotional modulation of visual and motor areas by dynamic body expressions of anger. Social Neuroscience, 3, 199–212.Google Scholar
Roether, C. L., Omlor, L., Christensen, A., & Giese, M. A. (2009). Critical features for the perception of emotion from gait. Journal of Vision, 9, 15.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, P. D., Polson, L., & Grosbras, M.-H. (2012). Developmental changes in emotion recognition from full-light and point-light displays of body movement. PLoS ONE, 7, e44815.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samson, D., Apperly, I. A., Chiavarino, C., & Humphreys, G. W. (2004). Left temporoparietal junction is necessary for representing someone else’s belief. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 499–500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scherer, K. R., Wallbott, H. G., & Summerfield, A. B. (Eds.) (1986). Experiencing emotion: A cross-cultural study. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schützwohl, A., & Reisenzein, R. (2012). Facial expressions in response to a highly surprising event exceeding the field of vision: A test of Darwin’s theory of surprise. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 657–664.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwarzlose, R. F., Baker, C. I., & Kanwisher, N. (2005). Separate face and body selectivity on the fusiform gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 11055–11059.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stark, K. (2019). The ambiguous face of ecstatic delight: Understanding the role of dynamic faces and bodies in emotion recognition. Unpublished Master’s thesis, Humboldt University of Berlin, Berlin, Germany.Google Scholar
Stekelenburg, J. J., & de Gelder, B. (2004). The neural correlates of perceiving human bodies: an ERP study on the body-inversion effect. Neuroreport, 15, 777–780.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tamietto, M., Castelli, L., Vighetti, S., Perozzo, P., Geminiani, G., Weiskrantz, L., & de Gelder, B. (2009). Unseen facial and bodily expressions trigger fast emotional reactions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 17661–17666.Google ScholarPubMed
Tamietto, M., Geminiani, G., Genero, R., & de Gelder, B. (2007). Seeing fearful body language overcomes attentional deficits in patients with neglect. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 445–454.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thoma, P., Soria Bauser, D., & Suchan, B. (2013). BESST (Bochum Emotional Stimulus Set) – A pilot validation study of a stimulus set containing emotional bodies and faces from frontal and averted views. Psychiatry Research, 209, 98–109.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Matsumoto, D. (2008). The spontaneous expression of pride and shame: Evidence for biologically innate nonverbal displays. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 11655–11660.Google ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2008). The nonverbal expression of pride: Evidence for cross-cultural recognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 516.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuminello, E. R., & Davidson, D. (2011). What the face and body reveal: In-group emotion effects and stereotyping of emotion in African American and European American children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 110, 258–274.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van den Stock, J., Tamietto, M., Sorger, B., Pichon, S., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2011). Cortico-subcortical visual, somatosensory, and motor activations for perceiving dynamic whole-body emotional expressions with and without striate cortex (V1). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 16188–16193.Google ScholarPubMed
van de Riet, W. A. C., Grèzes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2009). Specific and common brain regions involved in the perception of faces and bodies and the representation of their emotional expressions. Social Neuroscience, 4, 101–120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vuilleumier, P., & Schwartz, S. (2001). Emotional facial expressions capture attention. Neurology, 56, 153–158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expression of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 879–896.3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Watson, R., & de Gelder, B. (2020). The representation and plasticity of body emotion expression. Psychological Research, 84, 1400–1406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiskrantz, L. (1996). Blindsight revisited. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 6, 215–220.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wenzler, S., Levine, S., van Dick, R., Oertel-Knöchel, V., & Aviezer, H. (2016). Beyond pleasure and pain: Facial expression ambiguity in adults and children during intense situations. Emotion, 16, 807–814.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wieser, M. J., & Brosch, T. (2012). Faces in context: A review and systematization of contextual influences on affective face processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 471.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592–598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Witkower, Z., Tracy, J. L., Pun, A., & Baron, A. S. (2021). Can children recognize bodily expressions of emotion? Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 45, 505–518.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zieber, N., Kangas, A., Hock, A., & Bhatt, R. S. (2014a). The development of intermodal emotion perception from bodies and voices. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 68–79.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zieber, N., Kangas, A., Hock, A., & Bhatt, R. S. (2014b). Infants’ perception of emotion from body movements. Child Development, 85, 675–684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Accessibility standard: WCAG 2.0 A

The PDF of this book conforms to version 2.0 of the Web Content Accessibility Guidelines (WCAG), ensuring core accessibility principles are addressed and meets the basic (A) level of WCAG compliance, addressing essential accessibility barriers.

Content Navigation

Table of contents navigation
Allows you to navigate directly to chapters, sections, or non‐text items through a linked table of contents, reducing the need for extensive scrolling.
Index navigation
Provides an interactive index, letting you go straight to where a term or subject appears in the text without manual searching.

Reading Order & Textual Equivalents

Single logical reading order
You will encounter all content (including footnotes, captions, etc.) in a clear, sequential flow, making it easier to follow with assistive tools like screen readers.
Short alternative textual descriptions
You get concise descriptions (for images, charts, or media clips), ensuring you do not miss crucial information when visual or audio elements are not accessible.
Full alternative textual descriptions
You get more than just short alt text: you have comprehensive text equivalents, transcripts, captions, or audio descriptions for substantial non‐text content, which is especially helpful for complex visuals or multimedia.

Visual Accessibility

Use of high contrast between text and background colour
You benefit from high‐contrast text, which improves legibility if you have low vision or if you are reading in less‐than‐ideal lighting conditions.

Structural and Technical Features

ARIA roles provided
You gain clarity from ARIA (Accessible Rich Internet Applications) roles and attributes, as they help assistive technologies interpret how each part of the content functions.

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure no-reply@cambridge-org.demo.remotlog.com is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×