Hostname: page-component-6bb9c88b65-x9fsb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-26T00:38:17.396Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Investigating risk factor and consequence accounts of executive functioning impairments in psychopathology: an 8-year study of at-risk individuals in Brazil

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2025

René Freichel*
Affiliation:
Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT)-lab, Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/04dkp9463 University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Sacha Epskamp
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/01tgyzw49 National University of Singapore , Singapore
Peter J. de Jong
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology, https://ror.org/012p63287 University of Groningen , Groningen, The Netherlands
Janna Cousijn
Affiliation:
Center for Substance use and Addiction Research (CESAR), Department of Psychology, Education & Child Studies, https://ror.org/057w15z03 Erasmus University Rotterdam , Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Ingmar Franken
Affiliation:
Center for Substance use and Addiction Research (CESAR), Department of Psychology, Education & Child Studies, https://ror.org/057w15z03 Erasmus University Rotterdam , Rotterdam, The Netherlands
Giovanni A. Salum
Affiliation:
https://ror.org/01bfgxw09 Child Mind Institute , New York, United States https://ror.org/046dyet60 National Institute of Developmental Psychiatry for Children and Adolescents (INCT-CNPq) , São Paulo, Brazil https://ror.org/041yk2d64 Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul , Hospital de Clinicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil
Pedro Mario Pan
Affiliation:
https://ror.org/046dyet60 National Institute of Developmental Psychiatry for Children and Adolescents (INCT-CNPq) , São Paulo, Brazil https://ror.org/02k5swt12 Universidade Federal de São Paulo , Department of Psychiatry, Laboratory of Integrative Neuroscience (LiNC), São Paulo, Brazil
Ilya M. Veer
Affiliation:
Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT)-lab, Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/04dkp9463 University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Reinout W. Wiers
Affiliation:
Addiction Development and Psychopathology (ADAPT)-lab, Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/04dkp9463 University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam, The Netherlands Center for Urban Mental Health, https://ror.org/04dkp9463 University of Amsterdam , Amsterdam, The Netherlands
*
Corresponding author: René Freichel; Email: r.freichel@uva.nl
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Executive functioning (EF) impairments are widely known to represent transdiagnostic risk factors of psychopathology. However, a recent alternative account has been proposed, according to which EF impairments emerge as consequences of psychopathology.

Methods

Using a longitudinal cross-lagged panel network analysis approach, we tested these competing theoretical accounts at different stages during adolescence. We used data from the Brazilian High-Risk Cohort Study for the Development of Childhood Psychiatric Disorders, in which 61% of individuals at wave 1 were selected due to their high risk for psychopathology. Participants were assessed across three assessment waves during early (wave 1: n = 1,992, mean age = 10.20 years) and middle adolescence (wave 2: n = 1,633, mean age = 13.48 years; wave 3: n = 1,439, mean age = 18.20 years). We examined associations between working memory, inhibitory control, and broad-band measures of psychopathology.

Results

During early adolescence, lower inhibitory control was a risk factor for externalizing problems that, in turn, predicted lower working memory capacity. During middle adolescence, bidirectional associations became more prominent: inhibitory control and working memory functioned as both risk factors and consequences. Externalizing problems both predicted and were predicted by poor inhibitory control. Internalizing and externalizing symptoms showed bidirectional associations over time. Externalizing problems predicted more internalizing symptoms, whereas internalizing symptoms predicted fewer externalizing problems during middle adolescence.

Conclusions

Our results corroborate dynamic theories that describe executive dysfunctions as precursors and consequences of psychopathology in middle adolescence.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Adolescence represents a critical period marked by neurobehavioral changes, brain maturation, and cognitive-emotional development (Dahl, Allen, Wilbrecht, & Suleiman, Reference Dahl, Allen, Wilbrecht and Suleiman2018). As widely noted, this period of sensitivity may set the stage for developing adolescent mental health problems that often continue into adulthood (Lee et al., Reference Lee, Heimer, Giedd, Lein, Šestan, Weinberger and Casey2014; Paus, Keshavan, & Giedd, Reference Paus, Keshavan and Giedd2008). Three-fourths of all lifetime cases of mental disorders have an onset before the age of 24 years (Kessler et al. (Reference Kessler, Berglund, Demler, Jin, Merikangas and Walters2005), hence, onset in childhood, adolescence, or emerging adulthood. Different cognitive functions, in particular inhibitory control and working memory, have been proposed to play dual roles as being both risk factors and consequences of psychopathology in adolescence and emerging adulthood (Goschke, Reference Goschke2014; Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long, & Weigard, Reference Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long and Weigard2017; Liu & Pérez-Edgar, Reference Liu and Pérez-Edgar2019; Verdejo-García, Lawrence, & Clark, Reference Verdejo-García, Lawrence and Clark2008; White et al., Reference White, Marmorstein, Crews, Bates, Mun and Loeber2011). However, little is known about the specificity of these roles with respect to (a) different stages during adolescence and (b) different broad-band symptom domains of psychopathology (internalizing/externalizing). Understanding age-specific cognitive antecedents of developmental psychopathology is crucial for developing time-sensitive interventions and prevention efforts.

Executive functions are defined as a broad class of cognitive control components (Miyake et al., Reference Miyake, Friedman, Emerson, Witzki, Howerter and Wager2000) that include inhibitory control, shifting attention, and working memory. Different developmental profiles with a general improvement of these functions across adolescence and young adulthood have been described (e.g., Ferguson, Brunsdon, & Bradford, Reference Ferguson, Brunsdon and Bradford2021). Executive functions rapidly develop in mid-adolescence (ages 10–15) before stabilizing in early adulthood (Tervo-Clemmens et al., Reference Tervo-Clemmens, Calabro, Parr, Fedor, Foran and Luna2023). These developments are accompanied by neural changes in various brain regions, including the prefrontal cortex, as well as hormonal shifts for heightened social and affective processing (Crone & Dahl, Reference Crone and Dahl2012). Some evidence points to variations in trajectories of different executive functions (Steinberg et al., Reference Steinberg, Albert, Cauffman, Banich, Graham and Woolard2008) – with linear decreases in impulsivity from age 10 but a non-linear change in sensation-seeking (increasing until age 10 before stabilizing).

Cognitive processes and their development have been more broadly associated with mental health (see RDoC framework, Insel et al., Reference Insel, Cuthbert, Garvey, Heinssen, Pine, Quinn, Sanislow and Wang2010). Recent studies proposed that global deficits in EF increase the transdiagnostic vulnerability to psychopathology during adolescence for at-risk individuals, such as children who experienced neglect (Schäfer et al., Reference Schäfer, McLaughlin, Manfro, Pan, Rohde, Miguel, Simioni, Hoffmann and Salum2023; Wade, Zeanah, Fox, & Nelson, Reference Wade, Zeanah, Fox and Nelson2020). Similarly, global executive functions prospectively predicted the general psychopathology factor (‘p factor’; Caspi et al., Reference Caspi, Houts, Belsky, Goldman-Mellor, Harrington, Israel, Meier, Ramrakha, Shalev, Poulton and Moffitt2014) during early (i.e., 10–14 years) adolescence (Martel et al., Reference Martel, Pan, Hoffmann, Gadelha, do Rosário, Mari, Manfro, Miguel, Paus, Bressan, Rohde and Salum2017; Romer & Pizzagalli, Reference Romer and Pizzagalli2021). In addition to these associations with overall psychopathology, some evidence points to a potential specificity between different executive functions and broad problem domains of psychopathology (internalizing, externalizing). For instance, working memory constitutes one of the most central executive functions (Kane & Engle, Reference Kane and Engle2002) and has been specifically associated with externalizing disorders in children (Huang-Pollock et al., Reference Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long and Weigard2017). Inhibitory control, described as the ability to control impulses or automatic behaviors, has been established as a core aspect of emotion regulation and was implicated in both externalizing (Berger & Buttelmann, Reference Berger and Buttelmann2022) and internalizing problems (Sætren, Augusti, & Hafstad, Reference Sætren, Augusti and Hafstad2021) during childhood and adolescence.

The past two decades of research in developmental psychopathology have led to different theoretical approaches to integrate these findings. The risk factor theory proposes that EF impairments precede and increase individuals’ vulnerability to developing social, emotional, and behavioral problems later in life. According to the model by Carver, Johnson, and Timpano (Reference Carver, Johnson and Timpano2017), low inhibitory control may lead to internalizing or externalizing problems depending on the level of incentive sensitivity (low: internalizing, high: externalizing). Conceptually related, Wiers et al. (Reference Wiers, Anderson, Van Bockstaele, Salemink, Hommel, Williams, Rhodes and Conner2018) suggested that relatively weak executive functions constitute a general risk factor for psychopathology and that temperament determines the primary area of problems (internalizing or externalizing). In the field of addiction, there is strong evidence in favor of a pre-existing cognitive vulnerability, particularly with respect to impulsive behaviors (e.g., Verdejo-García et al., Reference Verdejo-García, Lawrence and Clark2008; White et al., Reference White, Marmorstein, Crews, Bates, Mun and Loeber2011), going back to the classical work of Gorenstein and Newman (Reference Gorenstein and Newman1980). Another study on executive functioning deficits in daily life (Letkiewicz et al., Reference Letkiewicz, Miller, Crocker, Warren, Infantolino, Mimnaugh and Heller2014) found that poor EF prospectively predicted depressive symptoms, while the reverse pathway was not present.

A contrasting theory, namely the ‘complication’ (Maasalo et al., Reference Maasalo, Lindblom, Kiviruusu, Santalahti and Aronen2021), ‘consequence’, or ‘scar’ account, suggests that EF impairments may represent consequences of psychopathological processes present in internalizing and externalizing disorders. Thus, weak EF may constitute a consequence of psychopathology. A recent study among 7–9-year-old children found evidence in favor of this consequence account for externalizing symptoms that constrained the development of inhibitory control (Maasalo et al., Reference Maasalo, Lindblom, Kiviruusu, Santalahti and Aronen2021). In addition, there is also some evidence in favor of a consequence account, with early substance use negatively impacting the development of executive functions in adolescence, with the strongest evidence from animal studies (e.g., Spear, Reference Spear2018), although the evidence is weaker in human development (e.g., see the systematic review by de Goede et al., Reference de Goede, van der Mark-Reeuwijk, Braun, le Cessie, Durston, Engels, Goudriaan, Moons, Vollebergh, de Vries, Wiers and Oosterlaan2021). At the level of broad-band symptom domains, more internalizing and externalizing symptoms among 13–14 year old adolescents predicted lower EF 3–4 years later on (Brieant, King-Casas, & Kim-Spoon, Reference Brieant, King-Casas and Kim-Spoon2022). A recent study using the Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study showed that p-factor scores, which represent a general dimension of psychopathology across internalizing and externalizing symptoms, prospectively predicted change in EF, but the reverse direction was also present (Romer & Pizzagalli, Reference Romer and Pizzagalli2021). This suggests that EF may serve a dual role, acting both as a risk factor for the development of psychopathology and as a consequence of it.

Nevertheless, the majority of studies in humans explicitly focused on the ‘risk factor’ account by including different executive functions as predictors and symptom measures as outcomes (Freichel, Pfirrmann, de Jong et al., Reference Freichel, Pfirrmann, de Jong, Cousijn, Franken, Oldehinkel, Veer and Wiers2023). Thus, less is known about how various executive functions are longitudinally associated with internalizing and externalizing symptoms at different stages of development. Existing studies on the prospective associations between different EFs, on the one hand, and both internalizing and externalizing symptoms, on the other hand, show several characteristics that constrain the possibility to differentiate between the risk factor versus consequence account of EFs in psychopathology: (1) Most studies focused on late childhood and early adolescence, and little is known about associations during middle adolescence; (2) through the use of traditional statistical approaches, executive functions have been commonly treated as sole predictors; (3) studies spanning longer time periods during adolescence typically examined average changes across time, and they thus failed to capture potential changes in the structure of associations (‘covariances’) at different change points during adolescence (Freichel, Pfirrmann, Cousjin et al., Reference Freichel, Pfirrmann, Cousjin, de Jong, Franken, Banaschewski, Bokde, Desrivières, Flor, Grigis, Garavan, Heinz, Martinot, Martinot, Artiges, Nees, Orfanos, Poustka, Hohmann and Consortium2023).

With data from the Brazilian High-Risk Cohort Study for Mental Conditions, a large longitudinal dataset of adolescents (Salum et al., Reference Salum, Gadelha, Pan, Moriyama, Graeff-Martins, Tamanaha, Alvarenga, Krieger, Fleitlich-Bilyk, Jackowski, Sato, Brietzke, Polanczyk, Brentani, de Jesus Mari, Do Rosário, Manfro, Bressan, Mercadante and Rohde2015) assessed during three waves, the present study aimed to fill these critical caveats by addressing two key research questions: (1) Are particular domains of EF, specifically working memory and inhibitory control, risk factors for or consequences of both internalizing and externalizing symptoms? Based on prior studies (e.g., Yang et al., Reference Yang, Shields, Zhang, Wu, Chen and Romer2022), we predicted that inhibitory control would be a general risk factor for both. Moreover, we predicted that working memory would be a risk factor specific to externalizing symptoms. (2) Do these associations differ between different stages of adolescence? We investigated the associations between cognitive measures and internalizing/externalizing symptoms through the use of a novel methodological approach, namely cross-lagged network analysis (Wysocki, Rhemtulla, Bork, & Cramer, Reference Wysocki, Rhemtulla, Bork and Cramer2022). This exploratory, methodological approach allowed us to study the dynamic interplay (of risk factors and consequences) between specific waves without presupposing the role of EFs as either predictors or outcomes in symptom development.

Materials and methods

Data source and procedure

We used data from the Brazilian High-Risk Cohort Study for the Development of Childhood Psychiatric Disorders (BHRCS). This longitudinal panel study recruited a school-based community sample across two cities in Brazil (São Paulo, Porto Alegre). The recruitment targeted both high-risk children (i.e., based on extensive family history of mental disorders screening) and a randomly selected community sample. Our analyses included the entire sample, which combines both high-risk and randomly selected participants, to ensure sufficient variability in all symptom and cognitive measures. This approach was essential as network analyses require variability to capture patterns of covariance between different levels of cognitive functioning and symptom development. Participants were invited to three study visits (wave 1: years 2010–2011, wave 2: years 2013–2014, wave 3: years 2017–2019) that included the administration of clinical interviews and neuropsychological assessments. The study design and sample selection are described in more detail elsewhere (Salum et al., Reference Salum, Gadelha, Pan, Moriyama, Graeff-Martins, Tamanaha, Alvarenga, Krieger, Fleitlich-Bilyk, Jackowski, Sato, Brietzke, Polanczyk, Brentani, de Jesus Mari, Do Rosário, Manfro, Bressan, Mercadante and Rohde2015). The study was approved by the ethical committees of the universities at each site, and all parents of participants provided informed consent.

Measures

Psychopathology

CBCL. The Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL, Brazilian version) (Bordin, Mari, & Caeiro, Reference Bordin, Mari and Caeiro1995) was used as a parent-report questionnaire to assess psychopathology symptoms. A parent or caregiver completed the checklist with 120 items that assessed emotional-behavioral problems (Bordin et al., Reference Bordin, Rocha, Paula, Teixeira, Achenbach, Rescorla and Silvares2013), specifically the syndrome scales: anxious/depressed, withdrawn, somatic complaints, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, rule-breaking problems, aggressive behavior, and other problems. These dimensions can be grouped into two broad-band scales (Cohen, Gotlieb, Kershner, & Wehrspann, Reference Cohen, Gotlieb, Kershner and Wehrspann1985): internalizing symptoms (withdrawal, somatic complaints, anxiety/depression) and externalizing symptoms (rule-breaking problems, aggressive behavior). The CBCL was shown to be a valid assessment tool across cultures (Ivanova et al., Reference Ivanova, Dobrean, Dopfner, Erol, Fombonne, Fonseca, Frigerio, Grietens, Hannesdottir, Kanbayashi, Lambert, Achenbach, Larsson, Leung, Liu, Minaei, Mulatu, Novik, Oh and Chen2007).

ABCL. At the third measurement wave, participants aged 18 and above were administered the Adult Behavioral Checklist (ABCL, Achenbach & Rescorla, Reference Achenbach and Rescorla2003). The ABCL is part of the Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment and consists of 118 items that are completed by a close informant (e.g., partner, parents, friends). Similar to the CBCL (for participants below 18 years of age), we derived two broad-band scales (internalizing: withdrawn, somatic complaints, anxious/depressed; externalizing: rule-breaking problems, aggressive behavior) as well as separate empirically derived scales.

Executive functions

Digit span task. A digit span task (forward/backward) from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2002) was used to assess short-term working memory capacity in the context of verbal information. Participants were instructed to listen to a sequence of numbers and repeat it either forward or backward. As the sequence got increasingly longer, the task became more difficult.

Corsi blocks. A corsi blocks (forward/backward) task (Vandierendonck, Kemps, Fastame, & Szmalec, Reference Vandierendonck, Kemps, Fastame and Szmalec2004) assessed the short-term working memory capacity of visual–spatial information. Participants were instructed to repeat a spatial sequence a researcher indicated by tapping up to nine identical blocks. We first standardized the backward digit span scores from both tasks (digit span and corsi block span) and then averaged these scores for every participant to create one aggregate measure of working memory. This was considered appropriate as both tasks’ scores correlated moderately (r = 0.46, p < 0.01). This process also increased the reliability of the measure and allowed us to prevent issues of multicollinearity (i.e., two strongly interconnected nodes) that are common problems in network analysis (Borsboom et al., Reference Borsboom, Deserno, Rhemtulla, Epskamp, Fried, McNally, Robinaugh, Perugini, Dalege, Costantini, Isvoranu, Wysocki, van Borkulo, van Bork and Waldorp2021). Higher averaged digit span scores indicate better working memory capacity.

Inhibitory control

A Go/no-go task (Bitsakou, Psychogiou, Thompson, & Sonuga-Barke, Reference Bitsakou, Psychogiou, Thompson and Sonuga-Barke2008) was used to assess inhibitory control. Participants were instructed to press buttons indicating the direction of arrows as soon and as accurately as possible. When a double-headed arrow appeared, participants were instructed to stop pressing the button (no-go). There were 75 go-trials and 25 no-go trials. All stimuli were displayed for 100 ms, and the intertrial period was 1500 ms. As relevant measures of inhibitory control, we included participants’ average RT of correct Go trials and the number of commission errors. The use of these two distinct measures was considered appropriate as they capture different dimensions of inhibitory control and may account for a possible speed-accuracy tradeoff. We used observed scores for the cognitive task measures, rather than latent variables, to avoid issues related to disattenuation that can arise when measurement error is removed from only some variables in the model. We removed outlier reaction times and accuracy scores with an absolute z-score equal to or larger than 4 (Brunnekreef et al., Reference Brunnekreef, De Sonneville, Althaus, Minderaa, Oldehinkel, Verhulst and Ormel2007). To facilitate interpretation, we inverted the two indices of inhibitory control (reaction times and error) such that higher scores indicate better inhibitory control (lower reaction times and higher accuracy).

Data preprocessing

For all analyses, we only included individuals with no missing data at the first wave for the CBCL measures and cognitive tasks (digit span, corsi-blocks, Go/NoGo task). In the Supplementary Table S1, we provide the proportion of missingness for all individual subscales/measures. The second analysis (cross-lagged network analysis from wave 2 to wave 3) included individuals with available CBCL (age below 18 years) and ABCL (age above 18 years) at the third wave. The distributions for all subscales of the available CBCL and ABCL measures appeared similar, and thus, we first standardized the CBCL/ABCL subscales separately before integrating them into the model.

Cross-lagged network analysis

To examine the temporal associations between cognitive markers and symptoms, we used cross-lagged network analysis (Wysocki et al., Reference Wysocki, Rhemtulla, Bork and Cramer2022). This approach, as used in extant literature (Freichel, Pfirrmann, de Jong et al., Reference Freichel, Pfirrmann, de Jong, Cousijn, Franken, Oldehinkel, Veer and Wiers2023; Zainal & Newman, Reference Zainal and Newman2022b), implements a series of regularized regressions to estimate cross-lagged (different nodes predicting each other over time) and autoregressive (node predicting itself over time) effects at every change point. The Least Absolute Shrinkage and Selection Operator (LASSO) with 10-fold cross-validation (to find the optimal γ-value) was used to shrink weak estimates to zero. The cross-lagged estimates describe temporal associations between nodes while controlling for all other variables in the network. This cross-lagged network analysis method has two key benefits that are important in the present application: (1) The model identifies temporal associations without requiring an a-priori specification of predictors and outcomes that are based on theoretical assumptions about the nature of EF in symptom development; (2) By controlling for all other variables in the network, the model can inform conclusions about the relative importance of different measures, such as executive functions. Considering the variance in the age range of our sample (within every wave), we included age as a covariate in the model following the approach of Funkhouser et al. (Reference Funkhouser, Chacko, Correa, Kaiser and Shankman2021) and Zainal and Newman (Reference Zainal and Newman2022a). This meant that age was an additional predictor (covariate); however, it was not predicted by any other variable in the model. Similarly, we conducted a sensitivity analysis including gender as a covariate. The models were estimated using the glmnet (Friedman et al., Reference Friedman, Hastie, Simon, Tibshirani, Hastie and Matrix2017) and visualized using the qgraph (Epskamp et al., Reference Epskamp, Cramer, Waldorp, Schmittmann and Borsboom2012) R packages. We used non-parametric bootstrapping procedures (Epskamp, Borsboom, & Fried, Reference Epskamp, Borsboom and Fried2018) with 1,000 bootstraps to examine the accuracy of the estimated edge weights.

Results

Sample characteristics

Table 1 summarizes demographic and clinical characteristics at all three waves separately for the high-risk and randomly selected population samples. The high-risk group comprised 60.89% of the sample. Boys were slightly overrepresented overall, but there were no significant differences in gender distribution between samples. At the aggregate level, we found notable trends with increases in internalizing symptoms (in particular withdrawn-depressed problems) across waves, while externalizing symptoms peaked during the second wave. Participants in the high-risk sample were slightly older (<0.3 years). The high-risk group exhibited significantly higher levels of psychopathology across all CBCL subscales, with internalizing and externalizing symptoms showing consistent differences between the groups at all three waves.

Table 1. Sample characteristics at all waves

Note: High-risk refers to the high-risk sample. Randomly selected refers to the randomly selected community sample. The range of the CBCL scales differs between syndromes. N refers to the number of participants with no missing data on the CBCL/ABCL internalizing and externalizing measures. w1 = wave 1, w2 = wave 2, w3 = wave 3. To facilitate data interpretation, we display only the CBLC scores and the raw scores (RT and errors) of the inhibitory control tasks. Chi-squared tests were used to compare gender distributions, while two-sided t-tests were used to assess differences in all symptom and cognitive measures. The significance columns refer to p < 0.05. The Supplementary Table S3 includes all relevant test statistics.

Abbreviations: CBCL = Child Behavior Checklist, RT = Reaction Time, M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation, ms = milliseconds, w = wave.

Participants’ cognitive performance also steadily improved throughout the three waves, with higher speed and accuracy in both the inhibition and working memory tasks during the later waves. The high-risk and randomly selected samples showed significant differences in inhibitory control, where the randomly selected group showed fewer errors at wave 2 and faster reaction-times at wave 3. Moreover, individuals in the high-risk group exhibited lower working memory capacity at wave 3.

There was substantial attrition throughout the three waves, with 35.14% of individuals from the first wave dropping out during the study. A supplemental analysis (see Supplementary Table S2) revealed that age, gender, externalizing symptoms, and cognitive performance at wave 1 were not associated with dropout. However, higher internalizing symptoms at wave 1 predicted a lower probability of dropout.

Associations during early adolescence

Figure 1 visualizes the temporal associations occurring during early adolescence (from wave 1 to wave 2, age: M wave 1 = 10.2, M wave 2 = 13.5). Directed edges (i.e., arrows) indicate temporal associations between nodes from wave 1 predicting nodes at wave 2. At the aggregate level, both indices of inhibitory control (lower commission errors and faster reaction time) predicted fewer externalizing problems. Commission errors and the index of working memory capacity predicted each other over time. A higher working memory capacity predicted fewer commission errors, and, to a lesser extent, the opposite pathway was present. Both externalizing and internalizing problems predicted a lower working memory span over time. There was also a strong positive association between working memory capacity and speed of inhibitory control. Higher working memory capacity predicted faster reaction times in the inhibitory control task, and the reverse direction was present as well. The estimated networks were sufficiently stable (see Supplementary Figures S1and S2 for the results from our bootstrapping stability analysis).

Figure 1. Wave 1 to wave 2 temporal associations (broad-band scales).

Note. The network depicts temporal associations from wave 1 to wave 2. Each node represents a construct measured at both waves. Outgoing edges (arrows) reflect how a construct at wave 1 predicts another construct at wave 2, while incoming edges reflect how a construct is predicted by other constructs from wave 1. The outcome measures corresponding to the inhibitory control tasks have been inverted to facilitate interpretation. Higher scores on all cognitive control measures (RTCor, ComError, WM_Span) indicate better performance. The colors (blue = positive; red = negative) and thickness of the edges represent the direction and strength of associations, respectively. The edge weights are scaled based on the highest absolute edge weight in the network. The circular arrows on top of each node indicate autoregressive effects (i.e., the extent to which a construct predicts itself over time from wave 1 to wave 2).

Predictive associations from early to middle adolescence

Next, we examined the associations between cognitive functioning measures and symptoms during the change from early to middle adolescence (age: M wave 2 = 13.5, M wave 3 = 18.20). The aggregate temporal network (see Figure 2) is more dense (proportion of non-zero edge: 92% for waves 2–3, 72% for waves 1–2) and contains more reciprocal associations than the previous network during early adolescence. Consistent with the associations observed during early adolescence, better inhibitory control (commission errors and reaction time) predicted fewer externalizing problems. However, we also found effects in the opposite direction, with more externalizing problems predicting worse inhibitory control. There was a weak negative association between internalizing symptoms and working memory span. Moreover, better inhibitory control (fewer commission errors) predicted fewer internalizing problems. Finally, we observed reciprocal associations between externalizing and internalizing symptoms with different signs: A higher level of externalizing symptoms predicted more internalizing symptoms, whereas higher levels of internalizing symptoms at wave 2 predicted fewer externalizing problems later on (wave 3).

Figure 2. Wave 2 to wave 3 temporal associations (broad-band scales).

Note. The network depicts temporal associations from wave 2 to wave 3. Each node represents a construct measured at both waves. Outgoing edges (arrows) reflect how a construct at wave 2 predicts another construct at wave 3, while incoming edges reflect how a construct is predicted by other constructs from wave 2. The outcome measures corresponding to the inhibitory control tasks have been inverted to facilitate interpretation. Higher scores on all cognitive control measures (RTCor, ComError, WM_Span) indicate better performance. The colors (blue = positive, red = negative) and thickness of the edges represent the direction and strength of associations, respectively. The edge weights are scaled based on the highest absolute edge weight in the network. The circular arrows on top of each node indicate autoregressive effects (i.e., the extent to which a construct predicts itself over time from wave 2 to wave 3).

Sensitivity analyses

In addition to the cross-lagged network analysis, we estimated the panel graphical vector-autoregression panel model (Epskamp, Reference Epskamp2020) on all three waves of data to examine whether we may be able to obtain an average within-person temporal network (across the three waves). The panel GVAR model is similar to a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model, requires three waves of data, and yields partial (directed) within-person correlations. The model assumes stationarity, and given the substantial trends, we standardized the data at each time point, which inflated the model fit statistics. The panel GVAR model estimated on the detrended data showed poor fit (TLI = 0.56, CFI = 0.73, RMSEA = 0.084), further suggesting that the network structure changes over the course of the three waves. Thus, we considered a cross-lagged network analysis approach examining wave-by-wave associations defensible. Moreover, we re-estimated the cross-lagged network analyses with gender as an additional covariate. This sensitivity analysis (see Supplementary Figures S3 and S4) confirmed that key findings remain consistent when controlling for gender.

Discussion

The present study identified dynamic associations between EF measures, namely working memory and inhibitory control, and broadband scales of internalizing and externalizing symptoms during different stages of adolescence. Our findings revealed two key pathways through which executive functions play into transdiagnostic symptom development.

Relatively weak inhibitory control is a risk factor for externalizing symptoms throughout adolescence

During both early and middle adolescence (average ages 10–14, 14–18), relatively weak inhibitory control, indicated by high scores on commission errors and higher reaction times, predicted more externalizing symptoms. The specificity of this association has been found in prior study (Bohlin, Eninger, Brocki, & Thorell, Reference Bohlin, Eninger, Brocki and Thorell2012; Quach et al., Reference Quach, Tervo-Clemmens, Foran, Calabro, Chung, Clark and Luna2020). Our study replicated and extended these findings because we examined a longer time frame ranging from ages 10 to 18 and controlled for a range of other variables, including working memory capacity and internalizing symptoms. Low inhibitory control may lead to emotion regulation difficulties and more impulsive behaviors. In line with this risk-account, a recent study by Hentges et al. (Reference Hentges, Krug, Shaw, Wilson, Dishion and Lemery-Chalfant2020) showed that an intervention on inhibitory control during early childhood was associated with a reduction in externalizing symptoms at age 14. Further research is needed to better understand the time frame at which the negative repercussions of low inhibitory control in early adolescence can still be mitigated. Our results further indicated that externalizing symptoms predicted future internalizing symptoms during the change from early to middle adolescence. Likely, externalizing behaviors, such as delinquency or aggressive behaviors, may lead to adverse reactions from peers, with a potential loss of social status. This may include peer rejection and academic challenges – all constituting stressors that could explain the increase in internalizing symptoms later on (Weeks et al., Reference Weeks, Ploubidis, Cairney, Wild, Naicker and Colman2016).

Adolescent externalizing and internalizing problems predict lower working memory capacity

A novel finding from our analyses (at both change points) is that internalizing and externalizing problems predicted a lower working memory capacity over time. There may be multiple underlying mechanisms that could explain these associations. Emotional and behavioral problems may lead to extensive worrying or rumination that could tax cognitive resources, such as working memory (Levens, Muhtadie, & Gotlib, Reference Levens, Muhtadie and Gotlib2009). Our analyses also revealed a weak positive association between working memory capacity and externalizing problems during early adolescence (see Figure 1). Relatively high working memory capacity was associated with higher scores on externalizing symptoms during early adolescence, specifically with more rule-breaking behavior. We provide possible explanations for this time-sensitive role of working memory: Likely, higher working memory at age 10 allows adolescents to engage in more sophisticated, complex behaviors and social settings that may, in turn, lead to situations during which rule-breaking or externalizing behaviors can be shown. This aligns with the proposed view of social working memory as a key cognitive competency associated with individuals’ social network size (Krol, Meyer, Lieberman, & Bartz, Reference Krol, Meyer, Lieberman and Bartz2018). In particular, high working memory at age 10 may put adolescents into contact with social environments that increase the likelihood of externalizing symptoms (aggressive and rule-breaking behaviors) later on. These speculations warrant further research into potential mediating factors, such as the role of peer relationships and family dynamics.

In addition to the predictive associations between EF and symptoms outlined above, we found a complex pattern of associations within the EF and internalizing/externalizing symptom domains. In both early and middle adolescence, we found that a higher working memory capacity predicted lower speed during the inhibition task and more attention problems. This is in line with previous research showing that these different cognitive functions are interrelated during development (Beattie, Schutte, & Cortesa, Reference Beattie, Schutte and Cortesa2018). In addition, our findings regarding the evolving dynamic relationships among executive functions during adolescence, the distinct roles of inhibition and working memory, and the overall growth of cognitive abilities throughout align with the cognitive mutualism theory (Kievit, Reference Kievit2020). This theory describes that positive associations between EFs contribute to general cognitive ability during sensitive developmental windows. Future studies should further integrate these dynamic associations between different EFs in developmental psychopathology theories.

Risk factor and consequence accounts depend on the developmental stage

Altogether, our findings underscore the importance of understanding the role of executive functions in the context of different developmental stages. The conceptual Figure 3 illustrates the evidence in favor of the aforementioned theoretical accounts (risk factor versus consequence theory of cognitive dysfunction) based on our cross-lagged network models. We found evidence for the risk factor theory of inhibitory control, according to which cognitive dysfunction (see ‘C factor’, Abramovitch, Short, & Schweiger, Reference Abramovitch, Short and Schweiger2021) precedes the development of externalizing symptoms in early adolescence. However, working memory in early adolescence appears to be a cognitive function that is a ‘complication’ (Maasalo et al., Reference Maasalo, Lindblom, Kiviruusu, Santalahti and Aronen2021) of this transdiagnostic symptom development. In contrast, during middle adolescence (waves 2–3), we observed more reciprocal relationships, with working memory and inhibitory control being both risk factors and consequences. Likely, psychopathology in early life may impact the normative development of executive functions (Rudd et al., Reference Rudd, Roubinov, Jones-Mason, Alkon and Bush2021), which, in turn, exert their influence as catalysts of more or less adaptive developmental processes. Our study used a novel panel network analytical approach that allowed us to test different theoretical approaches by studying working memory and inhibitory control as both predictors and outcomes in parallel.

Figure 3. Evidence for risk factors and consequence accounts for different cognitive functions, transdiagnostic dimensions, and stages during adolescence.

Note. This figure was based on the presence of directed edges between cognitive control functions (working memory, inhibitory control) and transdiagnostic dimensions (internalizing, externalizing symptoms) at different stages during adolescence (Figure 1: early adolescence, Figure 2: early to middle adolescence).

There are, however, several limitations that should be noted. First, cross-lagged panel network analysis conflates within- and between-person effects (Hamaker, Kuiper, & Grasman, Reference Hamaker, Kuiper and Grasman2015) and may result in a low specificity in identifying true within-person temporal effects (Freichel et al., Reference Freichel, Veer, Wiers, McNally and Epskamp2024). The relevant network model for identifying within-person associations (i.e., the panel GVAR model) showed a poor model fit, even when the temporal trends were removed. This provided further evidence that the network structure indeed changes during adolescence. However, as we did not separate within- and between-person effects, these temporal estimates should not be interpreted as causal, mechanistic processes. Second, there was a significant attrition rate, in particular, during the second assessment wave, and thus, our findings may be biased. Third, we used two well-validated measures of EF, namely working memory and inhibitory control. Future studies could test temporal associations between transdiagnostic symptom measures and a wider range of executive functions, including measures of cognitive flexibility, shifting, updating, and verbal and motor speed. Deriving a common EF factor and integrating it in models of symptom dynamics may provide additional insights. Lastly, our analysis of the last assessment wave combined information from the CBCL/ABCL assessment. While this is common in studies with a wide age range (Savage et al., Reference Savage, Verhulst, Copeland, Althoff, Lichtenstein and Roberson-Nay2015), it is important to acknowledge that these two measures, though designed to be analogous, may assess somewhat different aspects in children versus adults. This could introduce a degree of measurement invariance that may affect the accuracy and interpretation of the observed cross-lagged effects.

A greater understanding of the dynamic associations between EF and psychopathology may provide insights into the sensitive time windows in adolescence, during which interventions may be useful. Our findings showed that during early adolescence (1) lower inhibitory control is a risk factor for externalizing symptoms and (2) working memory capacity is a consequence of externalizing symptoms. During middle adolescence, both working memory and inhibitory control serve as both risk factors and consequences of symptom development. Further research is needed to tailor cognitive developmental cascade theories to specific phases of adolescent development. The ultimate objective is to develop early intervention strategies that target relevant EFs in time to prevent an ‘escalation’ of negative symptom dynamics from arising during adolescence.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725100639.

Data availability statement

The authors have obtained the data under a license, so it cannot be shared directly. .

Author contribution

R. F.: Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. S. E.: Methodology, Visualization, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization. P. J. J.: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization. J. C.: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization. I. F.: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization. G. A. S.: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, resources, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization, Data curation. P. M. P.: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Project administration, resources, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Conceptualization, Data curation. I. M. V.: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Methodology, Conceptualization. R. W. W.: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Methodology, Conceptualization.

Funding statement

This study is part of the project ‘New Science of Mental Disorders’ (www.nsmd.eu), supported by the Dutch Research Council and the Dutch Ministry of Education, Culture and Science (NWO gravitation grant number 024.004.016). The Brazilian High Risk Cohort Study also received funding from the National Institute of Developmental Psychiatric for Children and Adolescent (INPD) (Grant/Award Numbers: CNPq465550/2014‐2, FAPESP2014/50917‐0 & 2021/05332-8).

Competing interest

All authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethical standard

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this study comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References

Abramovitch, A., Short, T., & Schweiger, A. (2021). The C factor: Cognitive dysfunction as a transdiagnostic dimension in psychopathology. Clinical Psychology Review, 86, 102007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2021.102007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Achenbach, T. M., & Rescorla, L. (2003). Manual for the ASEBA adult forms & profiles: For ages 18–59 : Adult self-report and adult behavior checklist. ASEBA.Google Scholar
Beattie, H. L., Schutte, A. R., & Cortesa, C. S. (2018). The relationship between spatial working memory precision and attention and inhibitory control in young children. Cognitive Development, 47, 3245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.02.002.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berger, P., & Buttelmann, D. (2022). A meta-analytic approach to the association between inhibitory control and parent-reported behavioral adjustment in typically-developing children: Differentiating externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. Developmental Science, 25(1), e13141. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bitsakou, P., Psychogiou, L., Thompson, M., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2008). Inhibitory deficits in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder are independent of basic processing efficiency and IQ. Journal of Neural Transmission (Vienna, Austria: 1996), 115(2), 261268. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00702-007-0828-z.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bohlin, G., Eninger, L., Brocki, K. C., & Thorell, L. B. (2012). Disorganized attachment and inhibitory capacity: Predicting externalizing problem behaviors. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(3), 449458. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9574-7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bordin, I., Mari, J. J., & Caeiro, M. F. (1995). Validação da versão Brasileira do [validation of the Brazilian version of the child behavior checklist (CBCL)]. Revista ABP-APAL, 17(2), 5566.Google Scholar
Bordin, I., Rocha, M. M., Paula, C. S., Teixeira, M. C. T. V., Achenbach, T. M., Rescorla, L. A., & Silvares, E. F. M. (2013). Child behavior checklist (CBCL), youth self-report (YSR) and teacher’s report form (TRF): An overview of the development of the original and Brazilian versions. Cadernos De Saude Publica, 29(1), 1328. https://doi.org/10.1590/s0102-311x2013000100004.Google ScholarPubMed
Borsboom, D., Deserno, M. K., Rhemtulla, M., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., McNally, R. J., Robinaugh, D. J., Perugini, M., Dalege, J., Costantini, G., Isvoranu, A.-M., Wysocki, A. C., van Borkulo, C. D., van Bork, R., & Waldorp, L. J. (2021). Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science. Nature Reviews Methods Primers, 1(1), 58. https://doi.org/10.1038/s43586-021-00055-w.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brieant, A., King-Casas, B., & Kim-Spoon, J. (2022). Transactional relations between developmental trajectories of executive functioning and internalizing and externalizing symptomatology in adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 34(1), 213224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420001054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brunnekreef, J., De Sonneville, L. M. J., Althaus, M., Minderaa, R. B., Oldehinkel, A. J., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). Information processing profiles of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems: Evidence from a population-based sample of preadolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 48(2), 185193. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01695.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carver, C. S., Johnson, S. L., & Timpano, K. R. (2017). Toward a functional view of the P factor in psychopathology. Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 5(5), 880889. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617710037.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., Houts, R. M., Belsky, D. W., Goldman-Mellor, S. J., Harrington, H., Israel, S., Meier, M. H., Ramrakha, S., Shalev, I., Poulton, R., & Moffitt, T. E. (2014). The p factor: One general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clinical Psychological Science, 2(2), 119137. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702613497473.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, N. J., Gotlieb, H., Kershner, J., & Wehrspann, W. (1985). Concurrent validity of the internalizing and externalizing profile patterns of the Achenbach child behavior checklist. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 53(5), 724728. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.53.5.724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social-affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 13(9), 636650. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L., & Suleiman, A. B. (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature, 554(7693), Article 7693. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25770CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Goede, J., van der Mark-Reeuwijk, K. G., Braun, K. P., le Cessie, S., Durston, S., Engels, R. C. M. E., Goudriaan, A. E., Moons, K. G. M., Vollebergh, W. A. M., de Vries, T. J., Wiers, R. W., & Oosterlaan, J. (2021). Alcohol and brain development in adolescents and young adults: A systematic review of the literature and advisory report of the health Council of the Netherlands. Advances in Nutrition, 12(4), 13791410. https://doi.org/10.1093/advances/nmaa170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Epskamp, S. (2020). Psychometric network models from time-series and panel data. Psychometrika, 85(1), 206231.10.1007/s11336-020-09697-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, D., & Fried, E. I. (2018). Estimating psychological networks and their accuracy: A tutorial paper. Behavior Research Methods, 50(1), 195212. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0862-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epskamp, S., Cramer, A. O. J., Waldorp, L. J., Schmittmann, V. D., & Borsboom, D. (2012). Qgraph: Network visualizations of relationships in psychometric data. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(1), Article 1. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i04CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E. A., & Bradford, E. E. F. (2021). The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1382. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freichel, R., Pfirrmann, J., Cousjin, J., de Jong, P., Franken, I., Banaschewski, T., Bokde, A. L. W., Desrivières, S., Flor, H., Grigis, A., Garavan, H., Heinz, A., Martinot, J.-L., Martinot, M.-L. P., Artiges, E., Nees, F., Orfanos, D. P., Poustka, L., Hohmann, S., & Consortium, I. (2023). Drinking motives, personality traits and life stressors – Identifying pathways to harmful alcohol use in adolescence using a panel network approach. Addiction, 118(10), 19081919. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.16231.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freichel, R., Pfirrmann, J., de Jong, P. J., Cousijn, J., Franken, I. H. A., Oldehinkel, A. J., Veer, I. M., & Wiers, R. W. (2023). Executive functioning, internalizing and externalizing symptoms – Understanding developmental dynamics through panel network approaches. JAACAP Open, 2(1), 6677. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaacop.2023.11.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freichel, R., Veer, I., Wiers, R., McNally, R. J., & Epskamp, S. (2024). Cross-lagged panel models for studying psychopathology: A comparative overview of structural equation and panel network approaches. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/b94qtCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, J., Hastie, T., Simon, N., Tibshirani, R., Hastie, M. T., & Matrix, D. (2017). Package ‘glmnet.’. Journal of Statistical Software, 33(1), 122.Google Scholar
Funkhouser, C. J., Chacko, A. A., Correa, K. A., Kaiser, A. J. E., & Shankman, S. A. (2021). Unique longitudinal relationships between symptoms of psychopathology in youth: A cross-lagged panel network analysis in the ABCD study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(2), 184194. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gorenstein, E. E., & Newman, J. P. (1980). Disinhibitory psychopathology: A new perspective and a model for research. Psychological Review, 87(3), 301315.10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goschke, T. (2014). Dysfunctions of decision-making and cognitive control as transdiagnostic mechanisms of mental disorders: Advances, gaps, and needs in current research. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 23(S1), 4157. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1410.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamaker, E. L., Kuiper, R. M., & Grasman, R. P. (2015). A critique of the cross-lagged panel model. Psychological Methods, 20(1), 102. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038889.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hentges, R. F., Krug, C. M. W., Shaw, D. S., Wilson, M. N., Dishion, T. J., & Lemery-Chalfant, K. (2020). The long-term indirect effect of the early family check-up intervention on adolescent internalizing and externalizing symptoms via inhibitory control. Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 15441554. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang-Pollock, C., Shapiro, Z., Galloway-Long, H., & Weigard, A. (2017). Is poor working memory a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 14771490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0219-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Insel, T., Cuthbert, B., Garvey, M., Heinssen, R., Pine, D. S., Quinn, K., Sanislow, C., & Wang, P. (2010). Research domain criteria (RDoC): Toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 748751. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, M. Y., Dobrean, A., Dopfner, M., Erol, N., Fombonne, E., Fonseca, A. C., Frigerio, A., Grietens, H., Hannesdottir, H., Kanbayashi, Y., Lambert, M., Achenbach, T. M., Larsson, B., Leung, P., Liu, X., Minaei, A., Mulatu, M. S., Novik, T. S., Oh, K. J., & Chen, W. J. (2007). Testing the 8-syndrome structure of the child behavior checklist in 30 societies. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 36(3), 405417. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374410701444363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of prefrontal cortex in working-memory capacity, executive attention, and general fluid intelligence: An individual-differences perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(4), 637671. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196323.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Berglund, P., Demler, O., Jin, R., Merikangas, K. R., & Walters, E. E. (2005). Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Archives of General Psychiatry, 62(6), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kievit, R. A. (2020). Sensitive periods in cognitive development: A mutualistic perspective. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 36, 144149. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.10.007.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krol, S. A., Meyer, M. L., Lieberman, M. D., & Bartz, J. A. (2018). Social working memory predicts social network size in humans. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology, 4(4), 387399. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-018-0100-9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, F. S., Heimer, H., Giedd, J. N., Lein, E. S., Šestan, N., Weinberger, D. R., & Casey, B. J. (2014). Adolescent mental health – Opportunity and obligation. Science, 346(6209), 547549. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1260497.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Letkiewicz, A. M., Miller, G. A., Crocker, L. D., Warren, S. L., Infantolino, Z. P., Mimnaugh, K. J., & Heller, W. (2014). Executive function deficits in daily life prospectively predict increases in depressive symptoms. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 38(6), 612620. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-014-9629-5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levens, S. M., Muhtadie, L., & Gotlib, I. H. (2009). Rumination and impaired resource allocation in depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(4), 757766. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017206.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, P., & Pérez-Edgar, K. E. (2019). Developmental pathways from early Behavioral inhibition to later anxiety: An integrative review of developmental psychopathology research and translational implications. Adolescent Research Review, 4(1), 4558. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40894-018-0092-5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maasalo, K., Lindblom, J., Kiviruusu, O., Santalahti, P., & Aronen, E. T. (2021). Longitudinal associations between inhibitory control and externalizing and internalizing symptoms in school-aged children. Development and Psychopathology, 33(3), 843855. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579420000176.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martel, M. M., Pan, P. M., Hoffmann, M. S., Gadelha, A., do Rosário, M. C., Mari, J. J., Manfro, G. G., Miguel, E. C., Paus, T., Bressan, R. A., Rohde, L. A., & Salum, G. A. (2017). A general psychopathology factor (P factor) in children: Structural model analysis and external validation through familial risk and child global executive function. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 126(1), 137148. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000205.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The Unity and Diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paus, T., Keshavan, M., & Giedd, J. N. (2008). Why do many psychiatric disorders emerge during adolescence? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(12), 947957. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2513.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quach, A., Tervo-Clemmens, B., Foran, W., Calabro, F. J., Chung, T., Clark, D. B., & Luna, B. (2020). Adolescent development of inhibitory control and substance use vulnerability: A longitudinal neuroimaging study. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 42, 100771. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2020.100771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Romer, A. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Is executive dysfunction a risk marker or consequence of psychopathology? A test of executive function as a prospective predictor and outcome of general psychopathology in the adolescent brain cognitive development study®. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 51, 100994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100994.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rudd, K. L., Roubinov, D. S., Jones-Mason, K., Alkon, A., & Bush, N. R. (2021). Developmental consequences of early life stress on risk for psychopathology: Longitudinal associations with children’s multisystem physiological regulation and executive functioning. Development and Psychopathology, 33(5), 17591773. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579421000730.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sætren, S. S., Augusti, E.-M., & Hafstad, G. S. (2021). Affective inhibitory control and risk for internalizing problems in adolescents exposed to child maltreatment: A population-based study. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 130, 113125. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salum, G. A., Gadelha, A., Pan, P. M., Moriyama, T. S., Graeff-Martins, A. S., Tamanaha, A. C., Alvarenga, P., Krieger, F. V., Fleitlich-Bilyk, B., Jackowski, A., Sato, J. R., Brietzke, E., Polanczyk, G. V., Brentani, H., de Jesus Mari, J., Do Rosário, M. C., Manfro, G. G., Bressan, R. A., Mercadante, M. T., & Rohde, L. A. (2015). High risk cohort study for psychiatric disorders in childhood: Rationale, design, methods and preliminary results. International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research, 24(1), 5873. https://doi.org/10.1002/mpr.1459.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, J., Verhulst, B., Copeland, W., Althoff, R. R., Lichtenstein, P., & Roberson-Nay, R. (2015). A genetically informed study of the longitudinal relation between irritability and anxious/depressed symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 54(5), 377384. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2015.02.010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schäfer, J. L., McLaughlin, K. A., Manfro, G. G., Pan, P., Rohde, L. A., Miguel, E. C., Simioni, A., Hoffmann, M. S., & Salum, G. A. (2023). Threat and deprivation are associated with distinct aspects of cognition, emotional processing, and psychopathology in children and adolescents. Developmental Science, 26(1), e13267. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.13267.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., Albert, D., Cauffman, E., Banich, M., Graham, S., & Woolard, J. (2008). Age differences in sensation seeking and impulsivity as indexed by behavior and self-report: evidence for a dual systems model. Developmental Psychology, 44(6), 1764.10.1037/a0012955CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spear, L. P. (2018). Effects of adolescent alcohol consumption on the brain and behaviour. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 19(4), 4. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2018.10.Google ScholarPubMed
Tervo-Clemmens, B., Calabro, F. J., Parr, A. C., Fedor, J., Foran, W., & Luna, B. (2023). A canonical trajectory of executive function maturation from adolescence to adulthood. Nature Communications, 14(1), 6922. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-023-42540-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vandierendonck, A., Kemps, E., Fastame, M. C., & Szmalec, A. (2004). Working memory components of the Corsi blocks task. British Journal of Psychology (London, England: 1953), 95(Pt 1), 5779. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712604322779460CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verdejo-García, A., Lawrence, A. J., & Clark, L. (2008). Impulsivity as a vulnerability marker for substance-use disorders: Review of findings from high-risk research, problem gamblers and genetic association studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(4), 777810. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.11.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wade, M., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., & Nelson, C. A. (2020). Global deficits in executive functioning are transdiagnostic mediators between severe childhood neglect and psychopathology in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 50(10), 16871694. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719001764.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2002). WISC III: Escala de inteligência Wechsler Para crianças: 3a edição. São Paulo: Casa Do Psicólogo.Google Scholar
Weeks, M., Ploubidis, G. B., Cairney, J., Wild, T. C., Naicker, K., & Colman, I. (2016). Developmental pathways linking childhood and adolescent internalizing, externalizing, academic competence, and adolescent depression. Journal of Adolescence, 51, 3040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2016.05.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, H. R., Marmorstein, N. R., Crews, F. T., Bates, M. E., Mun, E.-Y., & Loeber, R. (2011). Associations between heavy drinking and changes in impulsive behavior among adolescent boys. Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental Research, 35(2), 295303. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1530-0277.2010.01345.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiers, R. W., Anderson, K. G., Van Bockstaele, B., Salemink, E., & Hommel, B. (2018). Affect, dual-processing, developmental psychopathology, and health behaviors. In Williams, D. M., Rhodes, R. E., & Conner, M. T. (Eds.), Affective determinants of health behavior (p. 158184). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780190499037.003.0008Google Scholar
Wysocki, A., Rhemtulla, M., Bork, R. van, & Cramer, A. (2022). Cross-lagged network models. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vjr8zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Zhang, Y., Wu, H., Chen, H., & Romer, A. L. (2022). Child executive function and future externalizing and internalizing problems: A meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 97, 102194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zainal, N. H., & Newman, M. G. (2022a). Elevated anxious and depressed mood relates to future executive dysfunction in older adults: A longitudinal network analysis of psychopathology and cognitive functioning. Clinical Psychological Science, 11(2), 218238. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026221114076.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zainal, N. H., & Newman, M. G. (2022b). Prospective network analysis of proinflammatory proteins, lipid markers, and depression components in midlife community women. Psychological Medicine, 53(11), 52675278. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329172200232XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Sample characteristics at all waves

Figure 1

Figure 1. Wave 1 to wave 2 temporal associations (broad-band scales).Note. The network depicts temporal associations from wave 1 to wave 2. Each node represents a construct measured at both waves. Outgoing edges (arrows) reflect how a construct at wave 1 predicts another construct at wave 2, while incoming edges reflect how a construct is predicted by other constructs from wave 1. The outcome measures corresponding to the inhibitory control tasks have been inverted to facilitate interpretation. Higher scores on all cognitive control measures (RTCor, ComError, WM_Span) indicate better performance. The colors (blue = positive; red = negative) and thickness of the edges represent the direction and strength of associations, respectively. The edge weights are scaled based on the highest absolute edge weight in the network. The circular arrows on top of each node indicate autoregressive effects (i.e., the extent to which a construct predicts itself over time from wave 1 to wave 2).

Figure 2

Figure 2. Wave 2 to wave 3 temporal associations (broad-band scales).Note. The network depicts temporal associations from wave 2 to wave 3. Each node represents a construct measured at both waves. Outgoing edges (arrows) reflect how a construct at wave 2 predicts another construct at wave 3, while incoming edges reflect how a construct is predicted by other constructs from wave 2. The outcome measures corresponding to the inhibitory control tasks have been inverted to facilitate interpretation. Higher scores on all cognitive control measures (RTCor, ComError, WM_Span) indicate better performance. The colors (blue = positive, red = negative) and thickness of the edges represent the direction and strength of associations, respectively. The edge weights are scaled based on the highest absolute edge weight in the network. The circular arrows on top of each node indicate autoregressive effects (i.e., the extent to which a construct predicts itself over time from wave 2 to wave 3).

Figure 3

Figure 3. Evidence for risk factors and consequence accounts for different cognitive functions, transdiagnostic dimensions, and stages during adolescence.Note. This figure was based on the presence of directed edges between cognitive control functions (working memory, inhibitory control) and transdiagnostic dimensions (internalizing, externalizing symptoms) at different stages during adolescence (Figure 1: early adolescence, Figure 2: early to middle adolescence).

Supplementary material: File

Freichel et al. supplementary material

Freichel et al. supplementary material
Download Freichel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 20.7 MB