Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-5q6g5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-07T10:02:17.605Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The relationship between recreational cannabis use, psychotic-like experiences, and the salience network in adolescent and young adult twins

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2025

Hande Atmaca-Turan
Affiliation:
Institute for Diabetes Research and Metabolic Diseases of the Helmholtz Center Munich, The University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany Neuroscience Department, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey
Didenur Şahin-Çevik
Affiliation:
Neuroscience Department, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey National Magnetic Resonance Research Center (UMRAM), Aysel Sabuncu Brain Research Centre (ASBAM), Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, Heidelberg University, Mannheim, Germany
Serenay Çakar
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey
Fulya Gökalp-Yavuz
Affiliation:
Department of Statistics, Middle East Technical University, Ankara, Turkey The Data Mine, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
Martijn van den Heuvel
Affiliation:
Department of Complex Traits Genetics, Center for Neurogenomics and Cognitive Research, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, The Netherlands Department of Child Psychiatry, Amsterdam UMC, Amsterdam Neuroscience, Amsterdam, The Netherlands
Fruhling Rijsdijk
Affiliation:
Psychology Department, Faculty of Social Sciences, Anton de Kom University of Suriname, Paramaribo, Suriname
Francesca Filbey
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, School of Behavioral and Brain Sciences, University of Texas at Dallas , Richardson, TX, USA
Timothea Toulopoulou*
Affiliation:
Neuroscience Department, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey National Magnetic Resonance Research Center (UMRAM), Aysel Sabuncu Brain Research Centre (ASBAM), Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey Department of Psychology, Bilkent University, Ankara, Turkey First Department of Psychiatry, National and Kapodistrian University of Athens, Athens, Greece Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, NY, USA
*
Corresponding author: Timothea Toulopoulou; Email: ttoulopoulou@bilkent.edu.tr
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

The use of cannabis in adolescence and early adulthood, critical phases for brain development, is linked to psychotic-like experiences (PLEs). The underlying mechanisms, however, remain unclear. This research examined the relationship between recreational cannabis use and PLEs, emphasizing the connectivity of the salience network (SN), which plays a role in salience processing and psychosis. To determine whether this relationship reflects shared genetic or environmental contributions, twin modeling was used.

Methods

We included 232 healthy adolescent Turkish twins who underwent diffusion MRI and psychometric assessment. SN connectivity was quantified using graph theory metrics. Linear mixed models were used to examine the associations among cannabis use, SN factors, and PLEs. Mediation analyses assessed whether SN parameters explained the cannabis–PLEs association. Twin models disentangle genetic and environmental contributions to these traits and their covariation.

Results

Cannabis use was significantly associated with higher overall PLE frequency. A specific SN factor predicted both total and positive PLEs. However, SN connectivity did not mediate the cannabis–PLEs relationship. Twin modeling showed that cannabis use and PLEs were mainly influenced by unique environmental factors. No significant phenotypic covariations were found among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN parameters.

Conclusions

Recreational cannabis use during adolescence and young adulthood is associated with heightened PLEs, although this association is not mediated by SN connectivity. The environment plays an important role during adolescence in shaping these traits independently. The findings underscore the need for longitudinal and genetically informed studies to clarify the mental health effects of adolescent cannabis use.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Background

With changing sociopolitical perspectives and legalization of cannabis in 22 countries and more than 30 U.S. states (Ferland & Hurd, Reference Ferland and Hurd2020), recreational cannabis use has expanded. According to the 2020 European Drug Report, around 90 million people aged 15–64 have used cannabis at least once, with chronic users often beginning during adolescence (NIDA, 2020).

Adolescence, a key phase for brain development, is when cannabis exposure emerges as a psychosis risk factor (Blest-Hopley, Colizzi, Giampietro, & Bhattacharyya, Reference Blest-Hopley, Colizzi, Giampietro and Bhattacharyya2020; Griffith-Lendering et al., Reference Griffith-Lendering, Wigman, Prince van Leeuwen, Huijbregts, Huizink, Ormel, Verhulst, van Os, Swaab and Vollebergh2013; Shrivastava, Johnston, Terpstra, & Bureau, Reference Shrivastava, Johnston, Terpstra and Bureau2014). Recently, it has been linked to psychotic-like experiences (PLEs), which resemble the symptoms of psychosis but are less severe in terms of frequency and disruption (Marconi, Di Forti, Lewis, Murray, & Vassos, Reference Marconi, Di Forti, Lewis, Murray and Vassos2016; Schubart et al., Reference Schubart, van Gastel, Breetvelt, Beetz, Ophoff, Sommer, Kahn and Boks2011; Sideli, Quigley, La Cascia, & Murray, Reference Sideli, Quigley, La Cascia and Murray2020). Although often transient in youth, PLEs may precede a psychotic disorder, particularly when frequent. As recreational cannabis use rises, monitoring its impact on these experiences is increasingly important.

The causal relationship between cannabis and psychosis risk is increasingly recognized. A recent study shows that cannabis consumption increases the probability of psychosis, regardless of the polygenic risk for schizophrenia (Austin-Zimmerman et al., Reference Austin-Zimmerman, Spinazzola, Quattrone, Wu-Choi, Trotta, Li, Johnson, Richards, Freeman, Tripoli, Gayer-Anderson, Rodriguez, Jongsma, Ferraro, La Cascia, Tosato, Tarricone, Berardi, Bonora and Di Forti2024). However, the neurobiological mechanisms underlying this association are still unclear. The endocannabinoid system is crucial during adolescence, and disruptions caused by THC lead to enduring neurobiological changes that affect behavior and function of the brain (Shrivastava et al., Reference Shrivastava, Johnston, Terpstra and Bureau2014). Neuroimaging studies reveal network-level abnormalities throughout the psychosis spectrum (Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Schultze-Lutter, Schimmelmann, Maric, Salokangas, Riecher-Rössler, van der Gaag, Meneghelli, Nordentoft, Marshall, Morrison, Raballo, Klosterkötter and Ruhrmann2015). Other neuroimaging studies, including those using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI), have associated adolescent cannabis use with alterations in white matter (WM) microstructure and brain network architecture (Bava et al., Reference Bava, Frank, McQueeny, Schweinsburg, Schweinsburg and Tapert2009; Courtney et al., Reference Courtney, Sorg, Baca, Doran, Jacobson, Liu and Jacobus2022; Kim et al., Reference Kim, Skosnik, Cheng, Pruce, Brumbaugh, Vollmer, Hetrick, O’Donnell, Sporns, Puce and Newman2011; Orr, Paschall, & Banich, Reference Orr, Paschall and Banich2016; Zalesky et al., Reference Zalesky, Solowij, Yucel, Lubman, Takagi, Harding, Lorenzetti, Wang, Searle, Pantelis and Seal2012). Initial findings indicate that abnormal salience processing may connect cannabis consumption to PLEs (Dawes et al., Reference Dawes, Quinn, Bickerdike, O’Neill, Granger, Pereira, Mah, Haselgrove, Waddington, O’Tuathaigh and Moran2022; Wijayendran, O’Neill, & Bhattacharyya, Reference Wijayendran, O’Neill and Bhattacharyya2018). Cannabis users exhibiting elevated PLEs demonstrate reduced latent inhibition, suggesting challenges in filtering extraneous stimuli and a heightened likelihood of experiencing psychotic-like phenomena. The aberrant salience theory claims that abnormal dopamine transmission in the salience network (SN) leads to the inaccurate salience attribution to neutral stimuli, therefore contributing to psychotic symptoms (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) has been linked to decreased connectivity in the SN, specifically between the insula and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) (Pelgrim, Ramaekers, Wall, Freeman, & Bossong, Reference Pelgrim, Ramaekers, Wall, Freeman and Bossong2023). This might explain how cannabinoids cause psychotic symptoms by changing connectivity in the SN (Bloomfield et al., Reference Bloomfield, Hindocha, Green, Wall, Lees, Petrilli, Costello, Ogunbiyi, Bossong and Freeman2019).

Twin studies, although limited, demonstrate genetic and environmental influences on cannabis use and its impact on brain development. Studies show that cannabis-related decreases in subcortical volumes reflect common familial influences (Pagliaccio et al., Reference Pagliaccio, Barch, Bogdan, Wood, Lynskey, Heath and Agrawal2015). One study found heritability estimates of 44% and 55% for cannabis initiation and problematic use, respectively (Verweij et al., Reference Verweij, Zietsch, Lynskey, Medland, Neale, Martin, Boomsma and Vink2010).

However, no research has yet examined the relationship between cannabis use and PLEs in adolescence or early adulthood, and the genetic and environmental contributions to this relationship. Most prior research focused on chronic cannabis use in Western adult populations, often failing to address brain structural network changes and their association with PLEs in youth. Since multiple circuits are implicated in cannabis-induced brain alterations (McManus, Belnap, Kirsch, Ray, & Grodin, Reference McManus, Belnap, Kirsch, Ray and Grodin2025; Menon, Reference Menon and Toga2015), this study looks at the SN, a neural circuit strongly linked to psychosis and cannabis sensitivity. Most of the previous research has focused on chronic or heavy cannabis users; thus, we limited our sample to low-to-moderate, non-daily cannabis users to identify early-stage subclinical effects on brain structure and PLEs. This approach avoids potential confounds related to prolonged exposure and concomitant substance use.

Our primary aim was to look at the relationship among PLEs, recreational cannabis use, and SN parameters during a critical stage of brain development. Based on this, we performed two separate linear mixed models (LMMs) to examine the associations between these variables. Additionally, we explored whether the factors derived from SN parameters mediated the relationship between cannabis use and PLEs. Lastly, we aimed to distinguish the genetic and environmental contributions that are shared among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN parameters by conducting cross-twin within-trait (CTWT) and cross-twin cross-trait (CTCT) correlations, followed by univariate and bivariate twin models.

Methods

Participants

Participants were 232 healthy twins (116 pairs), recruited for a broader in-house study on brain development and genetic/environmental risk via posters and social media. Inclusion criteria: (1) age 14–24 and (2) native Turkish speaker. Exclusion criteria: current/past psychiatric/neurological disorders; substance use disorder; progressive visual impairment; and IQ < 70. The study was approved by the Bilkent University Ethics Committee and followed the Helsinki Declaration. Informed consent was obtained from all participants; for minors, parental consent was also secured.

Measurement

The Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences (CAPE-42) was used to evaluate PLEs in the general population (Stefanis et al., Reference Stefanis, Hanssen, Smirnis, Avramopoulos, Evdokimidis, Stefanis, Verdoux and Van Os2002). This 42-item self-report scale measures PLEs across negative, depressive, and positive dimensions (Mark & Toulopoulou, Reference Mark and Toulopoulou2015). Participants rated the lifetime frequency of these experiences on a 4-point Likert scale, and a total score was calculated by summing all items. Cannabis use was evaluated with the Cannabis Experience Questionnaire (CEQ), which includes 56 items on subjective effects and usage patterns (Barkus & Lewis, Reference Barkus and Lewis2018). Individuals with fewer than two lifetime uses were categorized as non-users. All others (i.e. those with ≥ 3 lifetime uses) were considered users. To focus on low-to-moderate frequency recreational use, individuals reporting daily cannabis use were excluded. Although daily users may also use cannabis recreationally, we aimed to examine the effects of occasional use on brain connectivity and PLEs. Daily use can affect brain connectivity in different ways, which might make it harder to see the specific impact of occasional use. Therefore, the one participant who reported daily use was excluded from the final analyses.

To assess current intellectual function, the block design and matrix reasoning subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence II (WASI-II) were administered using standard procedures (Wechsler, 1999).

MRI acquisition

Image acquisition was performed with a 3 T Siemens Magnetom Trio scanner using 32-channel radio-frequency coils at the Bilkent University National Magnetic Resonance Research Center (UMRAM), Ankara, Turkey. High-resolution T1-weighted structural images were acquired (TE = 3.02 ms; TR = 2600 ms; TI = 900 ms; flip angle = 8°; slice thickness = 1 mm; field of view (FOV) = 256 mm; matrix = 256 × 256 with 176 slices; and voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). Diffusion-weighted scans were acquired using an echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence (TR = 10740 ms; TE = 102 ms; FOV = 206 ms; matrix 256 × 256; slice thickness = 2 mm; and b value = 1000 s/mm2).

Diffusion MRI imaging

All preprocessing and matrix generation were conducted using the Connectivity Analysis Toolbox (CATO; de Lange, Helwegen, & van den Heuvel, Reference de Lange, Helwegen and van den Heuvel2023). T1-weighted MRI preprocessing was completed in Freesurfer (Fischl, Reference Fischl2012), while diffusion-weighted images (DWI) were processed with FSL v6.0 (Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich, & Smith, Reference Jenkinson, Beckmann, Behrens, Woolrich and Smith2012), using Topup and Eddy tools to correct for distortions and motion artifacts (Andersson & Sotiropoulos, Reference Andersson and Sotiropoulos2016). B vectors were updated, and b0 volumes were used to compute DWI reference images. After aligning DWI and T1 images, Freesurfer segmentations were registered. Diffusion tensors were estimated using the RESTORE algorithm (Chang, Jones, & Pierpaoli, Reference Chang, Jones and Pierpaoli2005) and Levenberg–Marquardt optimization (Press et al., Reference Teukolsky, Vetterling and Flannery1992). Average values of voxels along all streamlines connecting pairs of areas defined by the Desikan–Killany Atlas were used to determine fractional anisotropy (FA) and mean diffusivity (MD). FA and MD values show the average microstructural integrity of WM pathways that connect the SN nodes to other SN regions. Thus, these values are an aggregate measure of all streamlines from each SN node, but not individual tracts (e.g. anterior cingulum).

Anatomical parcellation and network reconstruction

The cortex was parcellated into 114 regions (57 per hemisphere) using the Desikan–Killany Atlas to define network nodes. Whole-brain deterministic tractography was conducted via CATO (de Lange et al., Reference de Lange, Helwegen and van den Heuvel2023) based on the FACT algorithm, using multiple seeds per WM voxel. Tracking was initiated with a step size of 1 mm and continued until either the FA dropped below 0.2, the turning angle between steps exceeded 45°, or the anatomical stopping criteria were met. To exclude biologically implausible tracts, streamlines shorter than 10 mm or longer than 200 mm were discarded. The resulting streamlines between nodes defined a 114×114 weighted connectivity matrix per participant. Network metrics local efficiency (LE) and clustering coefficient (CC) were computed using the Brain Connectivity Toolbox (Rubinov & Sporns, Reference Rubinov and Sporns2010). These were selected because they reflect how effectively local brain regions communicate (LE) and how strongly they are connected to their immediate neighbors (CC). Given the strong mathematical correlation between CC and LE, we report both but acknowledge their conceptual overlap. Such features may be especially relevant for processing and filtering salient information, which may be altered in cannabis-related psychosis risk. Based on prior studies (McManus et al., Reference McManus, Belnap, Kirsch, Ray and Grodin2025; Rössler et al., Reference Rössler, Rössler, Seifritz, Unterrassner, Wyss, Haker and Wotruba2020; Schiwy et al., Reference Schiwy, Forlim, Fischer, Kühn, Becker and Gallinat2022; Zhao et al., Reference Zhao, Bo, Zhang, Chen, Wang, Zhang, Li, Yang, Zhou and Wang2022), the anterior insula and caudal ACC were included as key SN nodes. These were not used as tractography seeds but were specifically analyzed for their local connectivity profiles.

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in R (R Core Team, 2021). Initial analyses involved simple linear regressions to investigate unadjusted associations between cannabis use and PLEs. Two separate models for PLE frequency and positive PLEs were defined to explore potential relationships before accounting for covariates and hierarchical data structure.

Missing data (4.9%) across all 24 structural network features (including FA, MD, CC, and LE measures) were imputed using multivariate imputation by chained equations (mice package) (Laird & Ware, Reference Laird and Ware1982). Correlation heatmaps were created to evaluate the relationships among SN variables across different brain regions. These heatmaps demonstrated strong correlations, supporting the need for dimensionality reduction through factor analysis to reduce complexity and enable more parsimonious modeling in subsequent analysis.

Factor analysis was performed to identify the latent constructs reflecting shared variance among SN parameters. The Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) measure confirmed the suitability of the dataset for factor analysis (KMO = 0.68). Parallel analysis, a widely used method for determining factor retention (Horn, Reference Horn1965), indicated the presence of six factors. Although the test for the sufficiency of six factors showed a significant result (χ2 = 728.05, df = 319, p < 0.001), models with more factors did not provide meaningful improvement. Therefore, the six factors were used as predictors for subsequent analyses. Model performance was assessed using likelihood ratio tests (LRT) and R2 metrics. Recent studies have effectively extracted biologically significant components using similar methodologies (Chamberland et al., Reference Chamberland, Raven, Genc, Duffy, Descoteaux, Parker, Tax and Jones2019; Geeraert, Chamberland, Lebel, & Lebel, Reference Geeraert, Chamberland, Lebel and Lebel2020; Kang, Galdo, & Turner, Reference Kang, Galdo and Turner2022; Vaher et al., Reference Vaher, Galdi, Blesa Cabez, Sullivan, Stoye, Quigley, Thrippleton, Bogaert, Bastin, Cox and Boardman2022).

These six latent factors were simultaneously entered as fixed effects into an LMM predicting CAPE scores, while adjusting for age, sex, IQ, and cannabis use. A random intercept for family ID was included to account for familial clustering among twin pairs. Of the six latent factors, only Factor 6 was statistically significant (p = 0.0339). This factor was mainly influenced by the CC and LE of the left rostral and caudal anterior cingulate cortices, suggesting that Factor 6 represents local connectivity in these regions (Supplementary Table S1). An LRT comparing the full model (with all six factors) to a reduced model (retaining only significant factors) indicated no significant loss of fit (p > 0.05), justifying the use of a more interpretable and statistically efficient model in subsequent analyses. Another LRT confirmed that including a random intercept for family ID significantly improved the model fit (χ2 (1) = 32.6, p < 0.001).

Full model diagnostics, including tests for multicollinearity, interaction effects, assumption checks, and performance metrics, are provided in the Supplementary Materials (Supplementary Figures S2–S3).

Twin model analyses

Classical twin models use monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twins to estimate genetic and environmental influences on a single trait or the overlap between traits (Shakoor et al., Reference Shakoor, McGuire, Cardno, Freeman, Plomin and Ronald2015). While DZ twins share ~50% of additive genetic variance, MZ twins share 100%, along with equal common environmental variance. The CTWT correlations assess the extent to which genetic and environmental factors contribute to the variance in each trait. The univariate twin model further decomposes the variance in a trait into additive genetic (A), common environmental (C), and unique environmental factors (E). A represents the impact of genes that add up to change phenotype and are quantified as narrow-sense heritability (h2), representing the proportion of total phenotypic variance attributable to additive genetic effects. C represents nongenetic factors shared by members of the same family and are quantified as c2, representing the proportion of variance in a trait due to common environmental influences. E represents environmental components that differentiate members of the same family and are quantified as e2, representing the proportion of variance in a trait due to unique environmental influences.

On the other hand, the CTCT reveals whether common genetic and environmental influences contribute to the covariation between two phenotypes (de Vries, van Beijsterveldt, Maes, Colodro-Conde, & Bartels, Reference de Vries, van Beijsterveldt, Maes, Colodro-Conde and Bartels2021). Moreover, using bivariate twin models, we can estimate the shared genetic (r g), common environmental (r c), and unique environmental (r e) correlations between two different traits. Furthermore, we can investigate the overall phenotypic correlation between the two traits (r ph) and decompose this into its additive genetic (r ph-a), shared environmental (r ph-c), and unique environmental (r ph-c) components.

To evaluate the heritability estimates of PLEs, structural connectivity measures, and recreational cannabis use, we first applied CTWT correlations, followed by univariate twin models. Next, we calculated the CTCT correlations, followed by bivariate models to see if PLEs and SN factors shared common genetic and environmental influences with cannabis use.

All twin analyses were conducted using the OpenMx package version 2.20.6 (Boker et al., Reference Boker, Neale, Maes, Wilde, Spiegel, Brick, Spies, Estabrook, Kenny, Bates, Mehta and Fox2011; Neale et al., Reference Neale, Hunter, Pritikin, Zahery, Brick, Kirkpatrick, Estabrook, Bates, Maes and Boker2016) in R version 4.2.2 (R Core Team, 2021). Before the twin analyses, all continuous variables were adjusted for the effects of age, sex, and IQ. All the residuals were standardized, and a saturated model was fitted to the data. For the binary cannabis use variable, age, IQ, and sex were added as covariates to the univariate twin model. To better evaluate the variance estimates of cannabis use, an additional extended model was conducted, including other established environmental risk factors for psychosis, such as childhood trauma, socioeconomic status, and alcohol use as covariates. The χ 2 goodness-of-fit and Akaike information criterion (Akaike, Reference Akaike1974) were used to evaluate the model fit. The h2, c2, and e2 were calculated along with the likelihood-based 95% confidence intervals.

Results

Results of linear regression and linear mixed models

Of 232 individuals, 15 were excluded due to missing data, resulting in a final sample of 217 (59.9% females). Among them, 62 (28.6%) reported recreational cannabis use, while 155 (71.4%) did not. None of the users reported daily consumption; cannabis use was occasional. The average age of onset was 18.26 years (SD = 2.27). Table 1 shows descriptive statistics.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of study participants

* Cannabis use and frequency were assessed using two separate items from the CEQ: one assessing whether participants had ever used cannabis (yes/no), and another assessing frequency of use. While 62 participants reported having used cannabis at least once, only 53 provided frequency information. The remaining 9 users declined to answer the frequency question, resulting in a higher number of missing responses in the frequency item (n = 164), despite having reported cannabis use experience.

Initial linear regression analyses revealed that cannabis use was significantly associated with higher PLE frequency (β̂ = 5.02, p = 0.04), but not with positive PLEs (p = 0.05). Age, sex, and IQ were not significant predictors in either model.

To investigate these associations further, LMMs were conducted for both PLE frequency and positive PLEs. In these models, both recreational cannabis use and SN parameters were included as predictors. The SN parameters were modeled as six latent factors obtained from factor analysis, which was used to minimize dimensionality and capture underlying variation across multiple SN regions (Supplementary Table S1). Correlation heatmaps demonstrate the clustering of SN variables across brain regions, which informed the factor extraction (Supplementary Figure S1).

In the model predicting PLE frequency (Model 1), cannabis use (β̂ = 5.39, p = 0.029) and SN factor 6 (β̂ = 1.96, p = 0.037) emerged as significant predictors. This SN Factor 6 reflects connectivity within the anterior insula and ACC, key SN regions implicated in salience attribution and psychosis risk.

Family-level clustering explained 52% of the variance in PLE frequency (ICC = 0.52), and the inclusion of random intercepts for family ID significantly improved model fit (LRT χ2 = 32.62, df = 1, p < 0.001) (Table 2). For positive PLEs, both cannabis use (β̂ = 2.08, p = 0.032) and SN Factor 6 (β̂ = 0.76, p = 0.043) were significant predictors. Family-level clustering accounted for 45% of the variance in positive PLEs (ICC = 0.45). The inclusion of a random intercept for family ID significantly improved model fit (LRT χ2 = 22.43, df = 1, p < 0.001) (Table 3).

Table 2. Summary of LMM for total CAPE

The bold values indicate statistically significant results at p < 0.05.

Table 3. Summary of LMM for positive dimension

The bold values indicate statistically significant results at p < 0.05.

Mediation analyses

Mediation analysis for PLEs frequency

To investigate the mediating role of the SN in the association between cannabis use and PLE frequency, we conducted an exploratory causal mediation analysis using the mediation package (Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele, & Imai, Reference Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele and Imai2014). Based on prior LMM results, only Factor 6 was included as the mediator due to its significant association with PLEs. Age, sex, and IQ were included as covariates in both the mediator and outcome models to minimize potential confounding.

In the initial mediation model, Factor 6 did not mediate the relationship between cannabis use and PLE frequency (ACME = −0.4054, 95% CI [−1.4606, 0.16], p = 0.20). However, the average direct effect (ADE) was significant (ADE = 5.5660, 95% CI [1.0600, 10.34], p = 0.04), as was the total effect (total effect = 5.1606, 95% CI [0.4023, 10.00], p = 0.04).

A multilevel mediation analysis, accounting for familial clustering, was also conducted using mixed-effects models with cannabis use as a fixed effect and family ID as a random intercept. The outcome model included other covariates as fixed effects. The analysis revealed that ACME was not statistically significant (ACME = −0.470, 95% CI [−1.424, 0.20]), suggesting no evidence of mediation through the SN. However, ADE was significant (ADE = 5.450, 95% CI [1.062, 10.06], p = 0.02), indicating a direct relationship between cannabis use and PLE frequency. Similarly, the total effect was significant (total effect = 4.980, 95% CI [0.101, 9.77], p = 0.04).

Mediation analysis for the positive PLEs

Similarly, we investigated whether SN connectivity mediated the relationship between cannabis use and positive PLEs. The initial mediation analysis using a linear regression model showed no significant mediation through the SN (Factor 6) (ACME = −0.125, 95% CI [−0.400, 0.10], p = 0.22). However, cannabis use demonstrated a significant direct effect on positive PLEs (ADE = 2.045, 95% CI [0.641, 3.75], p < 0.001), as well as a significant total effect (total effect = 1.919, 95% CI [0.573, 3.56], p < 0.001).

These findings were confirmed in the multilevel mediation analysis, which took into consideration family clustering. Cannabis use showed a significant direct effect on positive PLEs (ADE = 5.450, 95% CI [1.062, 10.06], p = 0.02) and total effect (Total Effect = 4.980, 95% CI [0.101, 9.77], p = 0.04). However, SN Factor 6 did not mediate the effect of cannabis use (ACME = −0.470, 95% CI [−1.424, 0.20], p = 0.14).

Bivariate twin analysis

To assess the underlying etiology of the phenotypic associations identified in the LMMs, we next used classical twin modeling to examine whether recreational cannabis use, PLEs, and SN parameters share genetic or environmental influences. Univariate analysis results (Supplementary Table S2) showed that recreational cannabis use was predominantly affected by environmental factors (h2 = 0.00 [−0.61–0.50]; c2 = 0.47 [0.04–0.88]; e2 = 0.53 [0.37–0.79]). To address the potential role of additional environmental risk factors, we ran an extended univariate model including childhood trauma, alcohol use, and socioeconomic status as covariates. After accounting for covariates, the model indicated unique environmental influences on cannabis use (h2 = 0.05 [−1.50–1.35]; c2 = 0.44 [−0.67–1.45]; e2 = 0.56 [0.17–1.14]). Furthermore, PLE frequency (h2 = 0.30 [−0.20–0.81]; c2 = 0.29 [−0.17–0.66]; e2 = 0.41 [0.27–0.61]) and positive PLEs (h2 = 0.45 [−0.16–1.04]; c2 = 0.06 [−0.45–0.51]; e2 = 0.49 [0.33–0.73]) were influenced by unique environmental factors. Then, we calculated CTCT correlations to investigate the covariance among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN factors derived from the DWI analysis. PLE frequency, positive PLEs, and the SN factors did not show any significant correlations with cannabis use (Table 4). Similarly, no significant phenotypic covariations were found among cannabis use, PLEs, and brain structural connectivity based on the bivariate models (Table 5).

Table 4. Cross-twin/sibling cross-trait (CTCT) correlations among cannabis use, CAPE-42 measures, and six factors derived from the salience network

Table 5. Bivariate phenotypic correlations among cannabis use, CAPE-42, and DWI factors obtained from the salience network

Note: rph-a, overall additive genetic correlation between two traits; rph-c, overall shared environmental correlation between two traits; rph-e, overall unique environmental correlation between two traits; rc, common environmental correlations between two traits; re, unique environmental correlations between two traits.

Discussion

This study examined whether recreational cannabis use during adolescence and young adulthood, a key period for brain development, is linked to PLEs. Additionally, we aimed to disentangle the genetic and environmental influences shared among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN parameters using twin models. While our association analyses examine whether cannabis use is related to PLEs and SN alterations, our twin modeling evaluates whether these associations reflect underlying genetic or shared environmental liability. By combining both approaches within the same sample, we aimed to provide a more comprehensive understanding of the etiology of cannabis-related PLEs during adolescence.

We found a significant association between cannabis use and PLE frequency, suggesting that users report more frequent PLEs than non-users. Although the association with the positive PLEs subscale did not achieve significance in the simple linear regression model (p = 0.05), it achieved significance in the LMM after accounting for SN components and familial clustering. Consequently, we approach the result with caution and do not deem it conclusive. Moreover, we found that SN parameters significantly relate to both PLE frequency and positive PLEs, suggesting that SN characteristics may independently contribute to PLE variability. These findings offer preliminary support for the involvement of SN characteristics in PLEs and suggest that cannabis use and SN alterations may represent independent pathways contributing to PLEs. While these findings contribute to current evidence on adolescent cannabis use and susceptibility to psychosis, alternative hypotheses, such as self-medication and pre-existing vulnerabilities, remain plausible.

Another key aim was to test whether SN factors mediate the link between recreational cannabis use and PLEs, given prior evidence of cannabis-related SN disruption, particularly in the ACC and insula. Although some SN factors predicted PLEs, they did not mediate cannabis effects, suggesting the SN may influence PLEs but does not explain the cannabis–PLEs relationship in this sample. To the best of our knowledge, only one other study has investigated whether structural changes in the brain mediate the link between cannabis use and PLEs and found that cannabis use throughout adolescence has no detrimental effect on brain development (DeLisi et al., Reference DeLisi, Bertisch, Szulc, Majcher, Brown, Bappal and Ardekani2006). Another study found that daily cannabis use was not linked to adverse brain changes, while alcohol use negatively affected brain structure in both adolescents and adults (Weiland et al., Reference Weiland, Thayer, Depue, Sabbineni, Bryan and Hutchison2015). Other studies have also suggested a harmful effect of alcohol, but not cannabis, on the brain (Thayer et al., Reference Thayer, YorkWilliams, Karoly, Sabbineni, Ewing, Bryan and Hutchison2017). Our focus on non-daily users may explain discrepancies with studies including chronic users. Evidence from recent studies suggests that cannabis use does not impair WM integrity (Cousijn, Toenders, van Velzen, & Kaag, Reference Cousijn, Toenders, van Velzen and Kaag2022; Francis, Camprodon, & Filbey, Reference Francis, Camprodon and Filbey2023), a finding supported by a meta-analysis of 830 individuals (Lorenzetti et al., Reference Lorenzetti, Kowalczyk, Duehlmeyer, Greenwood, Chye, Yücel, Whittle and Roberts2023). However, as most studies are observational, the observed link between cannabis use and psychosis risk may reflect familial confounding rather than a direct causal effect (Schaefer et al., Reference Schaefer, Jang, Vrieze, Iacono, McGue and Wilson2021), highlighting the importance of twin studies to disentangle genetic and environmental influences.

Building on the findings from the LMMs, we ran twin models to examine whether the observed relationships among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN parameters were driven by shared genetic or environmental influences. Our findings indicate that, during adolescence, recreational cannabis use is predominantly shaped by environmental factors. Moreover, we did not observe significant phenotypic correlations (Rph) between PLEs, SN factors, and recreational cannabis use. While LMMs revealed significant associations among cannabis use, PLEs, and SN characteristics, the bivariate models did not reveal shared additive genetic or environmental influences on these traits. These findings indicate that the LMM-based associations may represent environmentally driven, individual-specific mechanisms that are not shared across our phenotypes; however, we cannot rule out methodological issues.

These findings differ from previous studies that have reported moderate-to-high heritability estimates for chronic cannabis use, as well as a phenotypic correlation between chronic cannabis use and PLEs (Karcher et al., Reference Karcher, Barch, Demers, Baranger, Heath, Lynskey and Agrawal2019; Nesvåg et al., Reference Nesvåg, Reichborn-Kjennerud, Gillespie, Knudsen, Bramness, Kendler and Ystrom2016; Verweij et al., Reference Verweij, Zietsch, Lynskey, Medland, Neale, Martin, Boomsma and Vink2010). Our study focused on recreational cannabis use in adolescents and young adults rather than chronic cannabis use, which may account for these differences. Given that recreational cannabis use increases during adolescence, a developmental period characterized by heightened environmental sensitivity, it is important to distinguish different types of cannabis consumption. Notably, our findings align with prior research investigating the relationship between adolescent cannabis use and psychoticism. Schaefer et al. (Reference Schaefer, Jang, Vrieze, Iacono, McGue and Wilson2021), using a longitudinal twin-control design, found no link between adolescent cannabis use and later psychoticism. Our findings from the twin modelling align with this, as the absence of significant phenotypic correlations suggests that cannabis use and PLEs may reflect distinct environmental influences rather than direct phenotypic overlap.

This study contributes novel insights into recreational cannabis use during adolescence and young adulthood by linking it to PLEs and the SN characteristics and emphasizing the role of environmental factors during this key developmental period (Picchioni et al., Reference Picchioni, Rijsdijk, Toulopoulou, Chaddock, Cole, Ettinger, Oses, Metcalfe, Murray and McGuire2017). Our findings highlight that environmental influences are present in cannabis use among adolescents, an area that has not been extensively explored. We extend previous research by examining covariation among cannabis use, PLEs, and structural brain measures, particularly within a non-Western adolescent and young adult population, a research area and population that is underrepresented in the literature (Batalla et al., Reference Batalla, Bhattacharyya, Yücel, Fusar-Poli, Crippa, Nogué, Torrens, Pujol, Farré and Martin-Santos2013).

Several limitations must be considered when evaluating our results. First, although our sample was larger than some previous studies, it was still relatively modest in size. However, the study assessed brain data on young twins, which is a particularly challenging and valuable sample to recruit. Second, cannabis use was assessed via self-report rather than biological verification (e.g. blood or urine tests). Third, cannabis use was treated as a binary variable (users/non-users), without accounting for age of onset, duration, or frequency. Although this approach was consistent with our focus on occasional recreational use, it limits interpretability regarding cumulative exposure. Prior research has shown that WM alterations associated with cannabis use often localize to similar tracts across adolescents and adults (Baker, Yücel, Fornito, Allen, & Lubman, Reference Baker, Yücel, Fornito, Allen and Lubman2013), and exposure duration has been linked to DTI changes (Gruber, Dahlgren, Sagar, Gönenç, & Lukas, Reference Gruber, Dahlgren, Sagar, Gönenç and Lukas2014; Jacobus & Tapert, Reference Jacobus and Tapert2014; Zalesky et al., Reference Zalesky, Solowij, Yucel, Lubman, Takagi, Harding, Lorenzetti, Wang, Searle, Pantelis and Seal2012). Thus, future studies should use dimensional cannabis measures. However, in the current investigation, these factors were not further investigated due to significant missingness in the relevant questionnaire items, which would have risked statistical validity and decreased model reliability. Furthermore, our sample did not include individuals with daily or heavy cannabis use, nor those with substance use disorders. Rather, it focused on occasional, non-clinical recreational users. This distinction from studies involving chronic or everyday users enabled us to focus on early-stage effects and overcome confounding factors associated with long-term exposure. While this allowed us to investigate early subclinical effects, it limits generalizability to populations with heavier or problematic use, who may exhibit more pronounced alterations in brain structure and psychosis risk (Batalla et al., Reference Batalla, Bhattacharyya, Yücel, Fusar-Poli, Crippa, Nogué, Torrens, Pujol, Farré and Martin-Santos2013; Bloomfield et al., Reference Bloomfield, Hindocha, Green, Wall, Lees, Petrilli, Costello, Ogunbiyi, Bossong and Freeman2019). Moreover, we focused on areas relevant to salience processing because earlier work has linked this to cannabis use and psychosis; other brain networks, such as DMN, might have produced different results. Although mediation analyses offered insights into potential pathways linking cannabis use and PLEs, these results should be interpreted cautiously. Causal mediation methods rely on strong assumptions, particularly the assumption of sequential ignorability, which is unlikely to be fully met in observational studies. Thus, results are exploratory and hypothesis generating. Longitudinal studies are needed to more rigorously evaluate these pathways. Although we did not explicitly test the equal environments assumption, we ran an extended model that included key environmental covariates for cannabis use. After adjusting for these variables, the unique environmental influences remained significant, supporting the role of non-shared environmental factors in cannabis use. Nevertheless, other variables, such as cannabis onset age, could still influence variance estimates. Additionally, the variation due to unique environmental influences in twin models also includes measurement errors. Future studies should examine SN functional connectivity to gain more insight into dynamic brain processes potentially affected by cannabis usage.

In conclusion, our findings indicate that adolescent and early adulthood recreational cannabis use is associated with an increased risk of PLEs, which are primarily influenced by environmental factors rather than genetic predisposition. These symptoms may be transient and have no clear evidence of lasting changes in brain networks like the SN.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725101773.

Data availability statement

All statistical analyses were performed in R. While the full analysis code is not publicly hosted at this stage, it is available from the corresponding author upon request for verification or reproduction of results.

Acknowledgements

We thank Timuçin Baş, Rabia Şen, Kübra Çelikbaş, Tuba Şahin, Seda Arslan, and İlayda Aydoğan for their help with data collection, and Dr. Ebru Öztürk and Dr. Beyza Doğanay-Erdoğan for their statistical support.

Author contribution

TT and FF conceived and designed the study. HA-T and DŞ-Ç collected the data. HA-T, DŞ-Ç, SÇ, FG-Y, MH, FR, and TT contributed to data processing and analysis. HA-T and DŞ-Ç wrote the first draft of the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final version.

Funding statement

This research was partially funded by the Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey, Project Number: 119 K410, to Timothea Toulopoulou. Authors FG-Y and SÇ were supported by the Middle East Technical University Scientific Research Projects Coordination Unit under Grant Number GAP-109-2023-11391.

Competing interests

The authors have nothing to disclose.

Footnotes

HA-T and DŞ-Ç contributed equally to this work.

References

Akaike, H. (1974). A new look at the statistical model identification. IEEE Transactions on Automatic Control, 19(6), 716723. https://doi.org/10.1109/TAC.1974.1100705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersson, J. L. R., & Sotiropoulos, S. N. (2016). An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. NeuroImage, 125, 10631078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin-Zimmerman, I., Spinazzola, E., Quattrone, D., Wu-Choi, B., Trotta, G., Li, Z., Johnson, E., Richards, A. L., Freeman, T. P., Tripoli, G., Gayer-Anderson, C., Rodriguez, V., Jongsma, H. E., Ferraro, L., La Cascia, C., Tosato, S., Tarricone, I., Berardi, D., Bonora, E., … Di Forti, M. (2024). The impact of schizophrenia genetic load and heavy cannabis use on the risk of psychotic disorder in the EU-GEI case-control and UK Biobank studies. Psychological Medicine, 54(15), 41604172. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291724002058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, S. T. E., Yücel, M., Fornito, A., Allen, N. B., & Lubman, D. I. (2013). A systematic review of diffusion weighted MRI studies of white matter microstructure in adolescent substance users. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(8), 17131723. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barkus, E., & Lewis, S. (2018). Cannabis experiences questionnaire. PsycTESTS Dataset. https://doi.org/10.1037/t70219-000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batalla, A., Bhattacharyya, S., Yücel, M., Fusar-Poli, P., Crippa, J. A., Nogué, S., Torrens, M., Pujol, J., Farré, M., & Martin-Santos, R. (2013). Structural and functional imaging studies in chronic cannabis users: A systematic review of adolescent and adult findings. PLoS One, 8(2), e55821. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0055821.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bava, S., Frank, L. R., McQueeny, T., Schweinsburg, B. C., Schweinsburg, A. D., & Tapert, S. F. (2009). Altered white matter microstructure in adolescent substance users. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173(3), 228237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2009.04.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blest-Hopley, G., Colizzi, M., Giampietro, V., & Bhattacharyya, S. (2020). Is the adolescent brain at greater vulnerability to the effects of cannabis? A narrative review of the evidence. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00859.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomfield, M. A. P., Hindocha, C., Green, S. F., Wall, M. B., Lees, R., Petrilli, K., Costello, H., Ogunbiyi, M. O., Bossong, M. G., & Freeman, T. P. (2019). The neuropsychopharmacology of cannabis: A review of human imaging studies. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 195, 132161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2018.10.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boker, S., Neale, M., Maes, H., Wilde, M., Spiegel, M., Brick, T., Spies, J., Estabrook, R., Kenny, S., Bates, T., Mehta, P., & Fox, J. (2011). OpenMx: An open-source extended structural equation Modeling framework. Psychometrika, 76(2), 306317. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9200-6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chamberland, M., Raven, E. P., Genc, S., Duffy, K., Descoteaux, M., Parker, G. D., Tax, C. M. W., & Jones, D. K. (2019). Dimensionality reduction of diffusion MRI measures for improved tractometry of the human brain. NeuroImage, 200, 89100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.06.020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, L., Jones, D. K., & Pierpaoli, C. (2005). RESTORE: Robust estimation of tensors by outlier rejection. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 53(5), 10881095. https://doi.org/10.1002/mrm.20426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Courtney, K. E., Sorg, S., Baca, R., Doran, N., Jacobson, A., Liu, T. T., & Jacobus, J. (2022). The effects of nicotine and cannabis co-use during late adolescence on white matter Fiber tract microstructure. Journal of Studies on Alcohol and Drugs, 83(2), 287295. https://doi.org/10.15288/jsad.2022.83.287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cousijn, J., Toenders, Y. J., van Velzen, L. S., & Kaag, A. M. (2022). The relation between cannabis use, dependence severity and white matter microstructure: A diffusion tensor imaging study. Addiction Biology, 27(1). https://doi.org/10.1111/adb.13081.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dawes, C., Quinn, D., Bickerdike, A., O’Neill, C., Granger, K. T., Pereira, S. C., Mah, S. L., Haselgrove, M., Waddington, J. L., O’Tuathaigh, C., & Moran, P. M. (2022). Latent inhibition, aberrant salience, and schizotypy traits in cannabis users. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 28, 100235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scog.2021.100235.Google ScholarPubMed
de Lange, S. C., Helwegen, K., & van den Heuvel, M. P. (2023). Structural and functional connectivity reconstruction with CATO - a connectivity analysis toolbox. NeuroImage, 273, 120108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120108.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vries, L. P., van Beijsterveldt, T. C. E. M., Maes, H., Colodro-Conde, L., & Bartels, M. (2021). Genetic influences on the covariance and genetic correlations in a bivariate twin model: An application to well-being. Behavior Genetics, 51(3), 191203. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-021-10046-y.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeLisi, L. E., Bertisch, H. C., Szulc, K. U., Majcher, M., Brown, K., Bappal, A., & Ardekani, B. A. (2006). A preliminary DTI study showing no brain structural change associated with adolescent cannabis use. Harm Reduction Journal, 3(1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1186/1477-7517-3-17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferland, J.-M. N., & Hurd, Y. L. (2020). Deconstructing the neurobiology of cannabis use disorder. Nature Neuroscience, 23(5), 600610. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-020-0611-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischl, B. (2012). FreeSurfer. NeuroImage, 62(2), 774781. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francis, A. N., Camprodon, J. A., & Filbey, F. (2023). Functional hyperconnectivity between corticocerebellar networks and altered decision making in young adult cannabis users: Evidence from 7T and multivariate pattern analysis. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 331, 111613. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2023.111613.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geeraert, B. L., Chamberland, M., Lebel, R. M., & Lebel, C. (2020). Multimodal principal component analysis to identify major features of white matter structure and links to reading. PLoS One, 15(8), e0233244. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0233244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Griffith-Lendering, M. F. H., Wigman, J. T. W., Prince van Leeuwen, A., Huijbregts, S. C. J., Huizink, A. C., Ormel, J., Verhulst, F. C., van Os, J., Swaab, H., & Vollebergh, W. A. M. (2013). Cannabis use and vulnerability for psychosis in early adolescence—A TRAILS study. Addiction, 108(4), 733740. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.12050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gruber, S. A., Dahlgren, M. K., Sagar, K. A., Gönenç, A., & Lukas, S. E. (2014). Worth the wait: Effects of age of onset of marijuana use on white matter and impulsivity. Psychopharmacology, 231(8), 14551465. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-013-3326-z.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179185.10.1007/BF02289447CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jacobus, J., & Tapert, S. (2014). Effects of cannabis on the adolescent brain. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 20(13), 21862193. https://doi.org/10.2174/13816128113199990426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., & Smith, S. M. (2012). FSL. NeuroImage, 62(2), 782790. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kang, I., Galdo, M., & Turner, B. M. (2022). Constraining functional coactivation with a cluster-based structural connectivity network. Network Neuroscience, 6(4), 10321065. https://doi.org/10.1162/netn_a_00242.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 1323. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karcher, N. R., Barch, D. M., Demers, C. H., Baranger, D. A. A., Heath, A. C., Lynskey, M. T., & Agrawal, A. (2019). Genetic predisposition vs individual-specific processes in the association between psychotic-like experiences and cannabis use. JAMA Psychiatry, 76(1), 87. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2546.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, D.-J., Skosnik, P. D., Cheng, H., Pruce, B. J., Brumbaugh, M. S., Vollmer, J. M., Hetrick, W. P., O’Donnell, B. F., Sporns, O., Puce, A., & Newman, S. D. (2011). Structural network topology revealed by white matter Tractography in cannabis users: A graph theoretical analysis. Brain Connectivity, 1(6), 473483. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2011.0053.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laird, N. M., & Ware, J. H. (1982). Random-effects models for longitudinal data. Biometrics, 38(4), 963. https://doi.org/10.2307/2529876.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenzetti, V., Kowalczyk, M., Duehlmeyer, L., Greenwood, L.-M., Chye, Y., Yücel, M., Whittle, S., & Roberts, C. A. (2023). Brain anatomical alterations in young cannabis users: Is it all hype? A meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies. Cannabis and Cannabinoid Research, 8(1), 184196. https://doi.org/10.1089/can.2021.0099.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marconi, A., Di Forti, M., Lewis, C. M., Murray, R. M., & Vassos, E. (2016). Meta-analysis of the association between the level of cannabis use and risk of psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(5), 12621269. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mark, W., & Toulopoulou, T. (2015). Psychometric properties of “community assessment of psychic experiences”: Review and meta-analyses. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbvf088. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv088.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McManus, K. R., Belnap, M., Kirsch, D. E., Ray, L. A., & Grodin, E. N. (2025). Effects of alcohol and cannabis co-use on salience network resting state functional connectivity in individuals who drink alcohol heavily. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 268, 112577. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2025.112577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Menon, V. (2015). Salience network. In Toga, A. W. (Ed.), Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference (pp. 597611). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-397025-1.00052-XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institute on Drug Abuse. (2020). Letter from the director. https://Nida.Nih.Gov/Publications/Research-Reports/Marijuana/Letter-Director.Google Scholar
Neale, M. C., Hunter, M. D., Pritikin, J. N., Zahery, M., Brick, T. R., Kirkpatrick, R. M., Estabrook, R., Bates, T. C., Maes, H. H., & Boker, S. M. (2016). OpenMx 2.0: Extended structural equation and statistical Modeling. Psychometrika, 81(2), 535549. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-014-9435-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesvåg, R., Reichborn-Kjennerud, T., Gillespie, N. A., Knudsen, G. P., Bramness, J. G., Kendler, K. S., & Ystrom, E. (2016). Genetic and environmental contributions to the association between cannabis use and psychotic-like experiences in young adult twins. Schizophrenia Bulletin, sbw101. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbw101.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orr, J. M., Paschall, C. J., & Banich, M. T. (2016). Recreational marijuana use impacts white matter integrity and subcortical (but not cortical) morphometry. NeuroImage: Clinical, 12, 4756. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2016.06.006.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pagliaccio, D., Barch, D. M., Bogdan, R., Wood, P. K., Lynskey, M. T., Heath, A. C., & Agrawal, A. (2015). Shared predisposition in the association between cannabis use and subcortical brain structure. JAMA Psychiatry, 72(10), 994. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1054.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pelgrim, T. A. D., Ramaekers, J. G., Wall, M. B., Freeman, T. P., & Bossong, M. G. (2023). Acute effects of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol (THC) on resting state connectivity networks and impact of COMT genotype: A multi-site pharmacological fMRI study. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 251, 110925. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2023.110925.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Picchioni, M. M., Rijsdijk, F., Toulopoulou, T., Chaddock, C., Cole, J. H., Ettinger, U., Oses, A., Metcalfe, H., Murray, R. M., & McGuire, P. (2017). Familial and environmental influences on brain volumes in twins with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 42(2), 122130. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.140277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Press, Teukolsky, Saul A., Vetterling, William T., & Flannery, Brian P.. (1992). Numerical recipes in C: The art of scientific computing (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from https://www.R-project.org/Google Scholar
Rössler, J., Rössler, W., Seifritz, E., Unterrassner, L., Wyss, T., Haker, H., & Wotruba, D. (2020). Dopamine-induced dysconnectivity between salience network and auditory cortex in subjects with psychotic-like experiences: A randomized double-blind placebo-controlled study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(3), 732740. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbz110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rubinov, M., & Sporns, O. (2010). Complex network measures of brain connectivity: Uses and interpretations. NeuroImage, 52(3), 10591069. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schaefer, J. D., Jang, S.-K., Vrieze, S., Iacono, W. G., McGue, M., & Wilson, S. (2021). Adolescent cannabis use and adult psychoticism: A longitudinal co-twin control analysis using data from two cohorts. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 130(7), 691701. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schiwy, L. C., Forlim, C. G., Fischer, D. J., Kühn, S., Becker, M., & Gallinat, J. (2022). Aberrant functional connectivity within the salience network is related to cognitive deficits and disorganization in psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 246, 103111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2022.06.008.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, S. J., Schultze-Lutter, F., Schimmelmann, B. G., Maric, N. P., Salokangas, R. K. R., Riecher-Rössler, A., van der Gaag, M., Meneghelli, A., Nordentoft, M., Marshall, M., Morrison, A., Raballo, A., Klosterkötter, J., & Ruhrmann, S. (2015). EPA guidance on the early intervention in clinical high risk states of psychoses. European Psychiatry, 30(3), 388404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2015.01.013.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schubart, C. D., van Gastel, W. A., Breetvelt, E. J., Beetz, S. L., Ophoff, R. A., Sommer, I. E. C., Kahn, R. S., & Boks, M. P. M. (2011). Cannabis use at a young age is associated with psychotic experiences. Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 13011310. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171000187X.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shakoor, S., McGuire, P., Cardno, A. G., Freeman, D., Plomin, R., & Ronald, A. (2015). A shared genetic propensity underlies experiences of bullying victimization in late childhood and self-rated paranoid thinking in adolescence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 41(3), 754763. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbu142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shrivastava, A., Johnston, M., Terpstra, K., & Bureau, Y. (2014). Cannabis and psychosis: Neurobiology. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 56(1), 8. https://doi.org/10.4103/0019-5545.124708.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sideli, L., Quigley, H., La Cascia, C., & Murray, R. M. (2020). Cannabis use and the risk for psychosis and affective disorders. Journal of Dual Diagnosis, 16(1), 2242. https://doi.org/10.1080/15504263.2019.1674991.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefanis, N. C., Hanssen, M., Smirnis, N. K., Avramopoulos, D. A., Evdokimidis, I. K., Stefanis, C. N., Verdoux, H., & Van Os, J. (2002). Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population. Psychological Medicine, 32(2), 347358. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291701005141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thayer, R. E., YorkWilliams, S., Karoly, H. C., Sabbineni, A., Ewing, S. F., Bryan, A. D., & Hutchison, K. E. (2017). Structural neuroimaging correlates of alcohol and cannabis use in adolescents and adults. Addiction, 112(12), 21442154. https://doi.org/10.1111/add.13923.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: R package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(5). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i05.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vaher, K., Galdi, P., Blesa Cabez, M., Sullivan, G., Stoye, D. Q., Quigley, A. J., Thrippleton, M. J., Bogaert, D., Bastin, M. E., Cox, S. R., & Boardman, J. P. (2022). General factors of white matter microstructure from DTI and NODDI in the developing brain. NeuroImage, 254, 119169. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Verweij, K. J. H., Zietsch, B. P., Lynskey, M. T., Medland, S. E., Neale, M. C., Martin, N. G., Boomsma, D. I., & Vink, J. M. (2010). Genetic and environmental influences on cannabis use initiation and problematic use: A meta-analysis of twin studies. Addiction, 105(3), 417430. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1360-0443.2009.02831.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. The Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Weiland, B. J., Thayer, R. E., Depue, B. E., Sabbineni, A., Bryan, A. D., & Hutchison, K. E. (2015). Daily marijuana use is not associated with brain morphometric measures in adolescents or adults. The Journal of Neuroscience, 35(4), 15051512. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2946-14.2015.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wijayendran, S. B., O’Neill, A., & Bhattacharyya, S. (2018). The effects of cannabis use on salience attribution: A systematic review. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 30(1), 4357. https://doi.org/10.1017/neu.2016.58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zalesky, A., Solowij, N., Yucel, M., Lubman, D. I., Takagi, M., Harding, I. H., Lorenzetti, V., Wang, R., Searle, K., Pantelis, C., & Seal, M. (2012). Effect of long-term cannabis use on axonal fibre connectivity. Brain, 135(7), 22452255. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/aws136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, L., Bo, Q., Zhang, Z., Chen, Z., Wang, Y., Zhang, D., Li, T., Yang, N., Zhou, Y., & Wang, C. (2022). Altered dynamic functional connectivity in early psychosis between the salience network and visual network. Neuroscience, 491, 166175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2022.04.002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of study participants

Figure 1

Table 2. Summary of LMM for total CAPE

Figure 2

Table 3. Summary of LMM for positive dimension

Figure 3

Table 4. Cross-twin/sibling cross-trait (CTCT) correlations among cannabis use, CAPE-42 measures, and six factors derived from the salience network

Figure 4

Table 5. Bivariate phenotypic correlations among cannabis use, CAPE-42, and DWI factors obtained from the salience network

Supplementary material: File

Atmaca-Turan et al. supplementary material

Atmaca-Turan et al. supplementary material
Download Atmaca-Turan et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.5 MB