Hostname: page-component-5447f9dfdb-89v5q Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-29T19:57:29.628Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The shadow of trauma: impaired mentalization in clinical populations – a systematic review

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 July 2025

Marianna Gorgellino*
Affiliation:
Treatment and Early Intervention in Psychosis Program (TIPP), Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/05a353079Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
Geetanjali Kumar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London, UK
Yusra Parkar
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London, UK
Ana Catalan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Basurto University Hospital, Bilbao, Spain Biocruces Bizkaia Health Research Institute, Barakaldo, Spain Department of Neurosciences, University of the Basque Country (UPV/EHU), Leioa, Spain Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
Natalia Fares-Otero
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Institute of Neurosciences (UBNeuro), Hospital Clinic of Barcelona, IDIBAPS, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain
Martin Debbané
Affiliation:
Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Geneva, Geneva, Switzerland Research Department of Clinical, Educational, and Health Psychology, University College London, London, UK
Marco Armando
Affiliation:
Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/05a353079Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland
Luis Alameda
Affiliation:
Treatment and Early Intervention in Psychosis Program (TIPP), Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/05a353079Lausanne University Hospital (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London, UK National Psychosis Unit, South London and Maudsley Foundation TRUST, National Health Services, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Marianna Gorgellino; Email: marianna.gorgellino@chuv.ch
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Mentalizing defines the set of social cognitive imaginative activities that enable interpretation of behaviors as arising from intentional mental states. Mentalization impairments have been related to childhood trauma (CT) and are widely present in people suffering from mental disorders. Nevertheless, the link between CT exposure, mentalization abilities, and related psychopathology remains unclear. This study aims to systematically review the evidence in this domain.

Methods

A Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA)-compliant systematic review of literature published until December 2022 was conducted through an Ovid search (Medline, Embase, and PsycINFO). The review was registered in the Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO) (CRD42023455602).

Results

Twenty-nine studies were included in the qualitative synthesis. Twenty studies (69%) showed a significant negative correlation between CT and mentalization. There was solid evidence for this association in patients with psychotic disorders, as almost half the studies focused on this population. The few studies focusing on unipolar depression, personality disorders, and opioid addiction also reported a negative impact of CT on mentalization. In contrast, evidence for post-traumatic stress disorder was inconsistent, and no evidence was found for bipolar disorder. When stratifying for subtypes of CT, there was solid evidence that neglect (physical and emotional) decreased mentalization capacity, while abuse (physical, emotional, or sexual) was not associated with mentalization impairments.

Conclusions

Although causality cannot be established, there was substantial evidence that CT negatively affects mentalization across various psychiatric disorders, particularly psychotic disorders. These findings highlight the potential of targeting mentalization impairments in prevention and treatment strategies aiming to reduce the incidence and the social functioning burden of mental illness.

Information

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Mentalization defines the set of social cognitive imaginative activities that enable the interpretation of behaviors as arising from intentional mental states. This capacity enables individuals to imagine and understand their own mental states, as well as those of others, including thoughts, feelings, intentions, and desires. As such, mentalization is crucial for effective social interactions and the ability to adapt in complex social contexts (Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist, & Target, Reference Fonagy, Gergely, Jurist and Target2002). The concept of mentalization is closely linked to metacognition, Theory of Mind (ToM), reflective functioning, cognitive empathy, and social cognition (Fonagy, Luyten, & Strathearn, Reference Fonagy, Luyten and Strathearn2011; Lysaker, Gagen, Moritz, & Schweitzer, Reference Lysaker, Gagen, Moritz and Schweitzer2018). While metacognition emphasizes more on self-reflection and regulating one’s own cognitive processes, mentalization, cognitive empathy, and Theory of Mind – a key component of social cognition – primarily focus on understanding others. Reflective functioning, a core aspect of mentalization, centers on the comprehension of attachment-related experiences and emotions within relational contexts. Despite their distinct emphases, these interconnected concepts collectively underscore the complexity of human social-cognitive abilities, essential in enabling self-awareness, complex social interactions, and navigation of both personal and others’ mental worlds.

Historically, research on mentalization deficits has focused primarily on borderline personality disorder and psychotic disorders, contributing significantly to understanding these conditions. In individuals with borderline personality disorder, mentalization theory has played a central role in explaining emotional dysregulation and interpersonal dysfunction, with early attachment disruptions and trauma hypothesized to impair the development of reflective functioning, a conceptual framework that led to the development of Mentalization-Based Treatment (MBT), an evidence-based intervention for this disorder (Bateman & Fonagy, Reference Bateman and Fonagy2008). In the context of psychosis, mentalization deficits have been investigated as a part of a broader disruption in social cognition, with relevant implications for daily functioning, contributing to social withdrawal and isolation (Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Carcione, Dimaggio, Johannesen, Nicolò, Procacci and Semerari2005).

Although these two populations have received the most attention, growing evidence suggests that impaired mentalization may also play a role in other psychiatric conditions, providing the basis for a transdiagnostic perspective.

Recent studies showed that mentalization deficits are more prevalent among individuals with psychiatric disorders than in the general population (Nazarov et al., Reference Nazarov, Frewen, Parlar, Oremus, MacQueen, McKinnon and Lanius2014; Healey, Bartholomeusz, & Penn, Reference Healey, Bartholomeusz and Penn2016; Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Trier, Haahr and Simonsen2016; Nemeth et al., Reference Nemeth, Matrai, Hegyi, Czeh, Czopf, Hussain, Pammer, Szabo, Solymar, Kiss, Hartmann, Szilagyi, Kiss and Simon2018). Notably, these deficits do not seem to result from the disorder itself, as they are unaffected by symptom severity or disease stage, and may be evident as early as the prodromal phases or even prior to the onset of mental illness (Addington et al., Reference Addington, Penn, Woods, Addington and Perkins2008; Green et al., Reference Green, Penn, Bentall, Carpenter, Gaebel, Gur, Kring, Park, Silverstein and Heinssen2008; Green et al., Reference Green, Bearden, Cannon, Fiske, Hellemann, Horan, Kee, Kern, Lee, Sergi, Subotnik, Sugar, Ventura, Yee and Nuechterlein2012). Consistent with these findings, there is preliminary evidence that deficits in mentalization may constitute a transdiagnostic risk factor closely associated with broader psychopathology, potentially underlying vulnerability across various psychiatric disorders (Bateman, Reference Bateman2012; Luyten, Campbell, Allison, & Fonagy, Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020).

Mentalization deficits not only signal vulnerability to various psychiatric disorders but also play a critical role in shaping social and functional outcomes across these conditions (Sharp & Venta, Reference Sharp and Venta2013). In this sense, several studies have shown that mentalization capacity plays a crucial role in social functioning across various psychiatric disorders; individuals with mentalization impairments often experience significant challenges in social interactions and overall functioning (Couture, Penn, & Roberts, Reference Couture, Penn and Roberts2006; Green et al., Reference Green, Penn, Bentall, Carpenter, Gaebel, Gur, Kring, Park, Silverstein and Heinssen2008; Bell, Tsang, Greig, & Bryson, Reference Bell, Tsang, Greig and Bryson2009). In conditions like psychosis, mentalization – alongside other aspects of social cognition – emerges as a key determinant of functional outcomes, suggesting that improving mentalization deficits could meaningfully enhance general functionality (Fares-Otero, Alameda et al., Reference Fares-Otero, Alameda, Pfaltz, Martinez-Aran, Schafer and Vieta2023).

It is well established that traumatic events during childhood, typically classified as physical and emotional neglect and physical, sexual, and emotional abuse, represent a well-known and highly prevalent risk factor for the onset of mental illness (McKay et al., Reference McKay, Cannon, Chambers, Conroy, Coughlan, Dodd, Healy, O’Donnell and Clarke2021). Indeed, evidence indicates that the prevalence of childhood trauma in psychiatric populations ranges from 70% to 85% (Battle et al., Reference Battle, Shea, Johnson, Yen, Zlotnick, Zanarini, Sanislow, Skodol, Gunderson, Grilo, McGlashan and Morey2004; Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Andreassen, Aas, Rossberg, Mork, Steen, Barrett, Lagerberg, Peleikis, Agartz, Melle and Lorentzen2013; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso, Degenhardt, de Girolamo, Dinolova, Ferry, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Huang, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lepine, Levinson and Koenen2017), while in the general population, it is approximately 30% (Larsson et al., Reference Larsson, Andreassen, Aas, Rossberg, Mork, Steen, Barrett, Lagerberg, Peleikis, Agartz, Melle and Lorentzen2013; Whitten, Tzoumakis, Green, & Dean, Reference Whitten, Tzoumakis, Green and Dean2024). Although childhood trauma is consistently associated with various mental health conditions, the mechanisms underlying their link are not fully understood yet (Alameda et al., Reference Alameda, Rodriguez, Carr, Aas, Trotta, Marino, Vorontsova, Herane-Vives, Gadelrab, Spinazzola, Di Forti, Morgan and Murray2020), and the role of mentalization in the interplay between childhood trauma and psychopathology requires further exploration.

In recent years, research has indeed identified trauma, in the form of abuse and neglect, as a significant risk factor for impaired social cognition, including mentalizing abilities (Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Begin, Normandin, Godbout and Fonagy2017; Rokita, Dauvermann, & Donohoe, Reference Rokita, Dauvermann and Donohoe2018) in both clinical and nonclinical populations. It is suggested that traumatic events occurring during early development interfere with the formation of mentalizing abilities during childhood and adolescence (Fonagy & Luyten, Reference Fonagy and Luyten2009; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Begin, Normandin, Godbout and Fonagy2017; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021; Martin-Gagnon, Normandin, Fonagy, & Ensink, Reference Martin-Gagnon, Normandin, Fonagy and Ensink2023). Moreover, mentalization deficits identified in populations exposed to traumatic events during childhood have been linked both to trauma by abuse (Cicchetti et al., Reference Cicchetti, Rogosch, Maughan, Toth and Bruce2003; Pears & Fisher, Reference Pears and Fisher2005; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Normandin, Target, Fonagy, Sabourin and Berthelot2015) and to trauma by neglect (Shipman & Zeman, Reference Shipman and Zeman1999; Edwards, Shipman, & Brown, Reference Edwards, Shipman and Brown2005).

Despite evidence linking mentalization abilities to trauma exposure, only two studies cover this area (Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann and Donohoe2018; Yang & Huang, Reference Yang and Huang2024). Rokita’s systematic review concluded an association between the social environment experienced during childhood, including traumatic experiences, and poorer social performance as an adult, including social cognition. However, specific insights on the link between childhood trauma and mentalization were not provided, and the review’s search was conducted in May 2018, with considerable research emerging since then (Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson, Francis-Naylor and Mulholland2018; Quide et al., Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O’Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018; Rnic et al., Reference Rnic, Sabbagh, Washburn, Bagby, Ravindran, Kennedy, Strauss and Harkness2018; Weijers et al., Reference Weijers, Fonagy, Eurelings-Bontekoe, Termorshuizen, Viechtbauer and Selten2018; Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis, van Beveren, Bruggeman, Cahn, de Haan, Delespaul, Meijer, Myin-Germeys, Kahn, Schirmbeck, Simons, van Haren and van Winkel2019; Simon et al., Reference Simon, Nemeth, Galber, Lakner, Csernela, Tenyi and Czeh2019; Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019; Guhn et al., Reference Guhn, Merkel, Hubner, Dziobek, Sterzer and Kohler2020; Li, Carracher, & Bird, Reference Li, Carracher and Bird2020; Eidenmueller et al., Reference Eidenmueller, Grimm, Hermann, Frischknecht, Montag, Dziobek, Kiefer and Bekier2021; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello, Cullen, Kane, McKernan, Morris, Kelly, Gill, Corvin, Hallahan, McDonald and Donohoe2021; Vaskinn, Melle, Aas, & Berg, Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021; Branas et al., Reference Branas, Lahera, Barrigon, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022). Yang’s meta-analysis evidenced a negative correlation between childhood trauma and mentalization capacity. However, this work only included five studies conducted in clinical samples, mainly focusing on the general population. While similar reviews have examined social cognition in psychotic disorders (Fares-Otero, Alameda et al., Reference Fares-Otero, Alameda, Pfaltz, Martinez-Aran, Schafer and Vieta2023), and in mood disorders (Fares-Otero, De Prisco et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023), none, to our knowledge, has taken a transdiagnostic approach with a focus on mentalization. This review aims to address that gap by concentrating on mentalization, now recognized as a promising target for psychological treatment. Disentangling the relationships between childhood trauma, mentalization, and subsequent psychopathology is essential for developing new prevention and treatment strategies to reduce the incidence and social functioning burden of mental illness.

Methodology

This systematic review was registered with the International Prospective Register of Systematic Reviews (PROSPERO; registration number: CRD42023455602). The review was reported according to the updated Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines (Page et al., Reference Page, McKenzie, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann, Mulrow, Shamseer, Tetzlaff, Akl, Brennan, Chou, Glanville, Grimshaw, Hrobjartsson, Lalu, Li, Loder, Mayo-Wilson, McDonald and Moher2021) (see Table S1 in the supplement) and the Enhancing the Quality and Transparency of Health Research (EQUATOR) reporting guidelines (Altman et al., Reference Altman, Simera, Hoey, Moher and Schulz2008).

Search strategy

A systematic search was conducted in Ovid (Medline, Embase, PsycINFO), covering studies from database inception to December 2022. The search strategy included keywords related to (1) psychiatric populations, (2) childhood trauma (CT), and (3) mentalization domains. The Boolean operator ‘AND’ was used (see search strategy and terms inserted in the supplement). After a consensus between different mentalization-based therapy experts (MA and MD), we decided to include in our search all terms relating to mentalization, namely ‘Theory of Mind’, ‘metacognition’, ‘reflective functioning’, ‘social cognition’, and ‘mindfulness’. This was necessary as these concepts are overlapping, sometimes used interchangeably, and many clinical trials use the same assessment tools to measure different constructs. Given the variability in terminology across studies, we opted for a broad search strategy to minimize the risk of overlooking potentially relevant papers.

Filters were applied to remove duplicate entries, research that was not conducted on human samples, and papers that were not published in the English language. After retrieving the articles, two researchers (GK and YP) independently screened the titles and abstracts of identified articles for relevancy. There was a 92% agreement consistency between both researchers; any discrepancies were resolved by two senior researchers (LA and MA). Subsequently, two researchers (GK and YP) independently conducted the full-text screening to assess eligibility; any discrepancies were resolved by a third researcher (MG) at a group meeting. Additionally, a manual literature search was performed to identify further eligible articles from the reference lists of previously identified reviews and included studies. This included: (i) screening the references of included studies, and (ii) screening the references of articles cited by the most relevant reviews on this topic (Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann and Donohoe2018; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021; Fares-Otero, De Prisco et al., Reference Fares-Otero, De Prisco, Oliva, Radua, Halligan, Vieta and Martinez-Aran2023; Fares-Otero, Alameda et al., Reference Fares-Otero, Alameda, Pfaltz, Martinez-Aran, Schafer and Vieta2023; Yang & Huang, Reference Yang and Huang2024).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

In relation to the PECO(S) (Participants, Exposition, Comparators, Outcomes, Study Design) framework (Morgan, Whaley, Thayer, & Schünemann, Reference Morgan, Whaley, Thayer and Schünemann2018), studies required (1) (P) for clinical studies of any psychiatric diagnosis based upon validated diagnostic manuals and scales, which includes the Statistical Manual for Mental Disorders (DSM-IV and V) and the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10 and 11), or psychiatric symptoms within the general population (GP); (2) (E) examined the presence of CT, defined as having occurred before the age of 18 and measured as composite CT or specific CT subtypes (sexual, physical and emotional abuse, physical and emotional neglect); (3) (C) compared with individuals without CT; (4) (O) assessed for mentalization outcomes (mentalization, Theory of Mind, metacognition, reflective functioning, social cognition, cognitive empathy, and mindfulness) with validated instruments and its relationship with CT; (5) reported quantitative results; (6) reported original results from a peer-reviewed journal. Predictors, Outcomes, and Table S3 in the supplement include the definitions and operationalization outcomes for each domain. Studies were excluded if (1) they were reviews, clinical cases, conference proceedings, study protocols, dissertations, or gray literature; (2) included a sample of participants over the age of 65 years.

Data extraction and quality assessment

Researchers (GK and YP) extracted data from all included studies into a Microsoft Excel database. The data was then cross-checked by a third researcher (MG) to ensure high data extraction quality. The descriptive variables extracted consisted of the following: (1) first author and year of publication and country; (2) sample size, ICD-defined/ DSM-defined or general population (GP), mean age (with range), and percentage of female; (3) CT measure used and subtype of trauma reported; (4) mentalization domain and outcome measure; (5) covariates with the analysis; (6) results in the association between CT and mentalization. Two independent reviewers (GK and YP) utilized the modified version Newcastle–Ottawa Scale (NOS) (Wells et al., Reference Wells, Shea, O’connell, Peterson, Welch, Losos and Tugwell2014) to assess for quality and risk of bias. This was cross-checked by a third reviewer (LA). Detailed information regarding the quality assessment process is presented in Table S4 of the supplementary material.

Results

Summary of search results

Of 3018 eligible articles, 179 were full-text screened and 29 were included in the qualitative synthesis. The details of the selection process have been visually displayed through the PRISMA flow diagram in Figure 1. The final selection and a summary of the findings are presented in Table 1.

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flowchart of studies selected for systematic review.

Table 1. Characteristics of the studies included in the review

Abbreviations: ACE, Adverse Childhood Experience questionnaire; BLERT, Bell-Lysaker Emotional Recognition Task; BD, Bipolar Disorders; BPD, Borderline Personality Disorder; CAT, Child Abuse and Trauma scale; CECA, Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse; CEAS, Childhood Emotional Abuse Scale; CHR, Clinical High Risk; CT, Childhood Trauma; CTQ, Childhood Trauma Questionnaire; EA, Emotional Abuse; EAT, Expression Attribution Test; EN, Emotional Neglect; ETI, Early Trauma Inventory; FEEST, Facial Expressions of Emotion: Stimuli and Tests; FEP, First Episode Psychosis; GP, General Population; HC, Healthy Controls; HT, Hinting Task; IPII, Indiana Psychiatric Illness Interview; IPT-15, Interpersonal Perception Task-15; IRI, Interpersonal Reactivity Index; MACE, Maltreatment and Abuse Chronology of Exposure scale; MAS, Metacognition Assessment Scale; MAS-A, Metacognition Assessment Scale-Abbreviated; MCQ-30, Metacognitions Questionnaire-30 items-Uncontrollability and Danger of thoughts; MDD, Major Depressive Disorder; MET, Multifaceted Empathy Task; MASC, The Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition; MZQ, Mentalization Questionnaire; PA, Physical Abuse; PN, Physical Neglect; PTSD, Post Traumatic Stress Disorder; RFQ, Reflective Functioning Questionnaire; RFS, Reflective Functioning Scale; RMET, Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test; SA, Sexual Abuse; SCZ, Schizophrenia; SUD, Substance Use Disorder; TAA, Trauma Assessment for Adults; TASIT, The Awareness of Social Inference Test; TEQ, Toronto Empathy Questionnaire; ToM, Theory of Mind.

The studies were all published between 2011 and 2022, with 22 out of 29 being published between 2016 and 2020. The different clinical populations that the studies focused on can be seen in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Clinical populations represented in the selected studies.

All studies employed a cross-sectional design and recruited clinical populations from inpatient and outpatient mental health services. All diagnoses were based on the International Classification of Diseases (ICD-10) or the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV and V) diagnostic criteria. The sample sizes of the clinical populations ranged from 19 to 757 patients. Of the 29 studies, 19 included healthy control groups, while only two studies reported well-matched controls (Chiesa & Fonagy, Reference Chiesa and Fonagy2014; Brune, Walden, Edel, & Dimaggio, Reference Brune, Walden, Edel and Dimaggio2016).

The majority of the studies (n = 22, 76%) assessed CT with well-established measures, namely the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire (CTQ) (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia, Stokes, Handelsman, Medrano, Desmond and Zule2003) and the Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA) Scale (Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran, & Jacobs, Reference Bifulco, Bernazzani, Moran and Jacobs2005). A total of nine studies (31%) employed a composite measure of CT, while most studies (n = 20, 69%) discriminated between the different subtypes of CT, namely physical abuse, emotional abuse, sexual abuse, physical neglect, and emotional neglect.

Details on mentalization domains across the studies are found in Supplementary Table S3. Mentalization abilities were assessed using a diverse array of tests, as also listed in Table 1. The Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) (Baron-Cohen et al., Reference Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Hill, Raste and Plumb2001) and the Hinting Task (HT) (Corcoran, Mercer, & Frith, Reference Corcoran, Mercer and Frith1995) were the most frequently employed tests, each being used in six studies, while the Metacognition Assessment Scale-Abbreviated (MAS-A) (Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Carcione, Dimaggio, Johannesen, Nicolò, Procacci and Semerari2005) and the Mayer–Salovey–Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test-Managing Emotions (MSCEIT-ME) (Brackett & Salovey, Reference Brackett and Salovey2006) were used in four studies each.

Only six studies conducted a mediation analysis with mentalization as a mediator between CT and clinical outcome (Chiesa & Fonagy, Reference Chiesa and Fonagy2014; Belvederi Murri et al., Reference Belvederi Murri, Ferrigno, Penati, Muzio, Piccinini, Innamorati, Ricci, Pompili and Amore2017; Ostefjells et al., Reference Ostefjells, Lystad, Berg, Hagen, Loewy, Sandvik, Melle and Rossberg2017; Weijers et al., Reference Weijers, Fonagy, Eurelings-Bontekoe, Termorshuizen, Viechtbauer and Selten2018; Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis, van Beveren, Bruggeman, Cahn, de Haan, Delespaul, Meijer, Myin-Germeys, Kahn, Schirmbeck, Simons, van Haren and van Winkel2019; Li et al., Reference Li, Carracher and Bird2020). They focused on various clinical populations, and they all found that mentalization impairments partially mediated the effect of CT on the psychiatric disorders, especially on negative and depressive symptoms. Additionally, the study by Mansueto et al. (Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis, van Beveren, Bruggeman, Cahn, de Haan, Delespaul, Meijer, Myin-Germeys, Kahn, Schirmbeck, Simons, van Haren and van Winkel2019) is the only one that performed a moderation analysis, finding no moderating effect of mentalization on the impact of CT on clinical conditions in individuals with a psychotic disorder.

Study quality assessment

The methodological quality of the included studies was assessed through the NOS scale and is presented in detail in Supplementary Table S4. The 29 included studies had scores ranging from 5 to 7 (poor to good), with 9 (31%) being rated as good quality, 14 (48%) as fair, and 6 as poor (21%). Most studies scored well on the ‘Selection’ domain, particularly in regard to the representativeness of the exposed cohorts and the ascertainment of trauma exposure. Nonetheless, many studies had limited comparability, with only few controlling for key confounders or including samples over 100 participants. Moreover, most of the included studies did not report a priori power analyses or otherwise justify their sample sizes. Finally, when considering the ‘outcome’ domains, if all the studies used validated measures, due to their cross-sectional design, they all had insufficient follow-up periods, which limited the ability to infer causality or long-term outcomes.

Transdiagnostic association between childhood trauma and mentalization capacity

Over the 29 studies included in this systematic review, 20 (69%) found at least one analysis, examining any form of CT (composite or subtype), supporting the presence of a negative association between CT and mentalization. One single study found evidence of a positive association (Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019), while eight (28%) studies reported no association between CT and mentalization. Notably, among the eight studies with no association, five utilized a composite measure of CT (Parlar et al., Reference Parlar, Frewen, Nazarov, Oremus, MacQueen, Lanius and McKinnon2014; Andreou et al., Reference Andreou, Kelm, Bierbrodt, Braun, Lipp, Yassari and Moritz2015; Palmier-Claus et al., Reference Palmier-Claus, Berry, Darrell-Berry, Emsley, Parker, Drake and Bucci2016; Addington et al., Reference Addington, Shakeel, Braun, Bonneville and Stowkowy2019; Guhn et al., Reference Guhn, Merkel, Hubner, Dziobek, Sterzer and Kohler2020), one focused solely on abuse (Branas et al., Reference Branas, Lahera, Barrigon, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022), and two assessed the five subtypes of CT (Jimenez et al., Reference Jimenez, Sole, Arias, Mitjans, Varo, Reinares, Bonnin, Ruiz, Saiz, Garcia-Portilla, Buron, Bobes, Amann, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Vieta and Benabarre2017; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello, Cullen, Kane, McKernan, Morris, Kelly, Gill, Corvin, Hallahan, McDonald and Donohoe2021).

Figure 3 illustrates the percentage of studies that reported negative associations between various forms of CT and levels of mentalization. The subtypes of neglect – specifically physical and emotional neglect – were most commonly linked to mentalization deficits, with 59% of studies indicating a negative association. In contrast, the evidence for a negative relationship between abuse subtypes and mentalization was more limited; only 22% of studies on physical and sexual abuse and 35% of studies on emotional abuse found a negative association. When examining CT as a composite measure, 44% of studies reported a negative association with mentalization capacity.

Figure 3. Association between CT and mentalization by subtypes of CT.

Diagnostic-specific associations between childhood trauma and mentalization capacity

Schizophrenia spectrum disorders

A significant portion of the studies (n = 14, 48%) focused on the relationship between CT and mentalization in the broad psychosis spectrum disorders, spanning from Clinical High-Risk for Psychosis to Schizophrenia Spectrum Disorders. Among these, nine studies identified a negative correlation, revealing that higher levels of CT were associated with reduced mentalization capacity. Specifically, mentalization deficits were associated with neglect (physical and emotional) in six studies (Garcia et al., Reference Garcia, Montalvo, Creus, Cabezas, Sole, Algora, Moreno, Gutierrez-Zotes and Labad2016; Kilian et al., Reference Kilian, Asmal, Chiliza, Olivier, Phahladira, Scheffler, Seedat, Marder, Green and Emsley2018; Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson, Francis-Naylor and Mulholland2018; Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus, & Rockstroh, Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus and Rockstroh2018; Mansueto et al., Reference Mansueto, Schruers, Cosci, van Os, Alizadeh, Bartels-Velthuis, van Beveren, Bruggeman, Cahn, de Haan, Delespaul, Meijer, Myin-Germeys, Kahn, Schirmbeck, Simons, van Haren and van Winkel2019; Vaskinn et al., Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021), sexual abuse in two studies (Lysaker Paul et al., Reference Lysaker Paul, Gumley, Brune, Vanheule, Buck and Dimaggio2011; Vaskinn et al., Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021), and emotional abuse in one study (Aydin et al., Reference Aydin, Balikci, Tas, Aydin, Danaci, Brune and Lysaker2016); notably, no study established a specific link with physical abuse. In contrast, four studies reported no significant association between CT and mentalization abilities (Palmier-Claus et al., Reference Palmier-Claus, Berry, Darrell-Berry, Emsley, Parker, Drake and Bucci2016; Addington et al., Reference Addington, Shakeel, Braun, Bonneville and Stowkowy2019; Rokita et al., Reference Rokita, Dauvermann, Mothersill, Holleran, Holland, Costello, Cullen, Kane, McKernan, Morris, Kelly, Gill, Corvin, Hallahan, McDonald and Donohoe2021; Branas et al., Reference Branas, Lahera, Barrigon, Canal-Rivero and Ruiz-Veguilla2022). It is interesting to notice that two utilized a composite measure of CT, and one focused solely on abuse without assessing neglect. Notably, Trauelsen et al. (Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019) reported a positive correlation between different subtypes of CT and metacognition abilities.

Major depressive disorder

Four studies explored the link between CT and mentalization in patients with major depressive disorder (MDD). Among these, three studies reported a negative correlation (Rnic et al., Reference Rnic, Sabbagh, Washburn, Bagby, Ravindran, Kennedy, Strauss and Harkness2018; Simon et al., Reference Simon, Nemeth, Galber, Lakner, Csernela, Tenyi and Czeh2019; Li et al., Reference Li, Carracher and Bird2020), while one study found no significant association (Guhn et al., Reference Guhn, Merkel, Hubner, Dziobek, Sterzer and Kohler2020).

Bipolar disorder

Research on bipolar disorder is limited, with only Jimenez et al. (Jimenez et al., Reference Jimenez, Sole, Arias, Mitjans, Varo, Reinares, Bonnin, Ruiz, Saiz, Garcia-Portilla, Buron, Bobes, Amann, Martinez-Aran, Torrent, Vieta and Benabarre2017) investigating the connection between childhood adversities and mentalization capacity. This study did not find a significant association.

Personality disorders

Three studies (Chiesa & Fonagy, Reference Chiesa and Fonagy2014; Brune et al., Reference Brune, Walden, Edel and Dimaggio2016; Petersen, Brakoulias, & Langdon, Reference Petersen, Brakoulias and Langdon2016) examined mentalization abilities in individuals diagnosed with personality disorders, all of which found a significant association with experiences of CT, indicating that early trauma adversely affects mentalization capacity in this group.

Post-traumatic stress disorder

Two studies addressed the relationship between mentalization abilities and CT in individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD). The findings were conflicting: Nazarov et al. (Reference Nazarov, Frewen, Parlar, Oremus, MacQueen, McKinnon and Lanius2014) found a negative correlation, whereas Parlar et al. (Reference Parlar, Frewen, Nazarov, Oremus, MacQueen, Lanius and McKinnon2014) reported no significant association.

Opioid addiction

Eidenmueller et al. (Reference Eidenmueller, Grimm, Hermann, Frischknecht, Montag, Dziobek, Kiefer and Bekier2021) investigated ToM abilities in individuals diagnosed with opioid addiction who were undergoing substitution treatment. Their findings indicated a significant negative correlation between CT and mentalization capacity.

Heterogeneous disorders

Four studies examined the CT-mentalization association within a heterogeneous sample of psychiatric diagnoses. Andreou et al. (Reference Andreou, Kelm, Bierbrodt, Braun, Lipp, Yassari and Moritz2015) compared individuals with borderline personality disorder and schizophrenia to a healthy control group, finding no evidence of an association between CT and mentalization. In contrast, Ostefjells et al. (Reference Ostefjells, Lystad, Berg, Hagen, Loewy, Sandvik, Melle and Rossberg2017) compared individuals with psychotic disorders to those with bipolar disorder, while Belvederi Murri et al. (Reference Belvederi Murri, Ferrigno, Penati, Muzio, Piccinini, Innamorati, Ricci, Pompili and Amore2017) analyzed a general psychiatric population against healthy controls. Quide et al. (Reference Quide, Cohen-Woods, O’Reilly, Carr, Elzinga and Green2018) also compared individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder to healthy controls. All three studies identified a significant negative correlation between CT and mentalization in psychiatric populations.

Association between childhood trauma and overlapping concepts used to refer to mentalization

Theory of mind

Similarly, seven studies (24%) (Nazarov et al., Reference Nazarov, Frewen, Parlar, Oremus, MacQueen, McKinnon and Lanius2014; Palmier-Claus et al., Reference Palmier-Claus, Berry, Darrell-Berry, Emsley, Parker, Drake and Bucci2016; Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson, Francis-Naylor and Mulholland2018; Rnic et al., Reference Rnic, Sabbagh, Washburn, Bagby, Ravindran, Kennedy, Strauss and Harkness2018; Simon et al., Reference Simon, Nemeth, Galber, Lakner, Csernela, Tenyi and Czeh2019; Eidenmueller et al., Reference Eidenmueller, Grimm, Hermann, Frischknecht, Montag, Dziobek, Kiefer and Bekier2021; Vaskinn et al., Reference Vaskinn, Melle, Aas and Berg2021) examined ToM, typically assessed via tasks such as the RMET and the HT. Six of these studies demonstrated a significant association between CT and ToM performances, while Palmier-Claus et al. found no link.

Metacognition

Metacognition was the focus of five studies (17%) (Lysaker Paul et al., Reference Lysaker Paul, Gumley, Brune, Vanheule, Buck and Dimaggio2011; Aydin et al., Reference Aydin, Balikci, Tas, Aydin, Danaci, Brune and Lysaker2016; Ostefjells et al., Reference Ostefjells, Lystad, Berg, Hagen, Loewy, Sandvik, Melle and Rossberg2017; Addington et al., Reference Addington, Shakeel, Braun, Bonneville and Stowkowy2019; Trauelsen et al., Reference Trauelsen, Gumley, Jansen, Pedersen, Nielsen, Haahr and Simonsen2019), which mainly used the MAS-A to investigate it. Four studies supported a negative correlation between CT and metacognitive capacity. In contrast, Trauelsen et al. reported a paradoxical finding, with CT being associated with better metacognitive functioning in individuals with first-episode psychosis.

Cognitive empathy

Finally, two studies (7%) (Parlar et al., Reference Parlar, Frewen, Nazarov, Oremus, MacQueen, Lanius and McKinnon2014; Guhn et al., Reference Guhn, Merkel, Hubner, Dziobek, Sterzer and Kohler2020) examined cognitive empathy, employing three different scales (see Table 1), and both reported no association with CT.

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to synthesize the existing literature on the association between childhood trauma and mentalization within psychiatric populations. Our analysis identified 29 relevant studies, reflecting a growing but recent interest in this field, particularly over the last decade. Overall, our results support a significant relationship between childhood trauma exposure, particularly neglect, and mentalization deficits transdiagnostically.

Half of these studies focused on individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders, while the remaining half examined a variety of conditions, including MDD, bipolar disorder, PTSD, personality disorders, and opioid addiction. Although autism spectrum disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, and eating disorders were included in our search strategy, no eligible studies involving these populations were identified, suggesting a potential gap in the literature at the intersection of trauma, mentalization, and these specific diagnoses. Considering the uneven representation of clinical populations across the included studies, the observed strength of associations between childhood trauma, particularly neglect, and mentalization impairments may partially reflect the availability of research rather than true diagnostic differences. This imbalance limits the generalizability of our findings across all psychiatric diagnoses and highlights the need for more research in underrepresented populations.

Nevertheless, the findings mainly indicate a significant negative correlation between childhood trauma and impairments in mentalization abilities across clinical samples, with the strongest associations observed in those with schizophrenia, MDD, and personality disorders, and broadly a greater impact of neglect experiences as against to abuse. In accordance with the literature (Fonagy & Luyten, Reference Fonagy and Luyten2009; Ensink et al., Reference Ensink, Begin, Normandin, Godbout and Fonagy2017; Rodriguez et al., Reference Rodriguez, Aas, Vorontsova, Trotta, Gadelrab, Rooprai and Alameda2021; Martin-Gagnon et al., Reference Martin-Gagnon, Normandin, Fonagy and Ensink2023), this suggests that experiences of childhood trauma may disrupt the normal development of mentalization, a critical social cognitive skill essential for understanding one’s own and others’ mental states.

Our finding of a stronger evidence linking neglect, as compared to abuse, with mentalizing abilities aligns with attachment theory, which posits that neglect can hinder the development of secure attachment necessary for self-reflection and interpersonal understanding (Bowlby, Reference Bowlby1969; Fonagy & Target, Reference Fonagy and Target1997). The diminished frequency of meaningful interactions between caregivers and children can limit opportunities for the child to learn about emotional states and develop a coherent sense of self and others (Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020). Such deficits in early relational experiences may have lasting impacts on social cognitive skills, potentially perpetuating mental health challenges in adulthood. Moreover, neurological evidence suggests distinct effects of different types of trauma on brain development (McLaughlin, Sheridan, & Lambert, Reference McLaughlin, Sheridan and Lambert2014). For instance, neglect is associated with reduced amygdala volume, while abuse can lead to increased amygdala volume (Teicher, Samson, Anderson, & Ohashi, Reference Teicher, Samson, Anderson and Ohashi2016). These structural changes may influence social cognition differently, underscoring the importance of differentiating among the subtypes of childhood adversity when studying their effects on social cognition.

While our review focused on mentalization-related constructs, it is also important to consider the broader cognitive impairments associated with childhood trauma. Recent meta-analyses have demonstrated robust associations between early trauma and deficits in attention, working memory, and executive functioning (Rodriguez et al., 2020; Vargas et al., Reference Vargas, Lam, Azis, Osborne, Lieberman and Mittal2019). These core cognitive domains may play a foundational role in supporting higher-order processes such as perspective-taking, a key component of mentalization. Consequently, impairments in basic cognitive functions could also moderate or mediate the impact of trauma on social cognitive development, and future studies should consider incorporating measures of general cognition to better elucidate their interplay.

The observed relationship between childhood trauma and mentalization impairments across disorders may also be understood in the context of the ‘P factor’ hypothesis (Caspi & Moffitt, Reference Caspi and Moffitt2018), which posits that various mental health conditions are interrelated and often rooted in common etiological pathways – such as adverse childhood experiences – thus aiming to explain the co-occurrence and heterogeneity of psychiatric symptoms. As mentalization plays a central role in self-regulation, interpersonal functioning, and adaptation to social stressors, and its impairments have been observed in multiple psychiatric conditions, sometimes even prior to illness onset, it might represent a transdiagnostic marker of this underlying vulnerability (Nolte et al., Reference Nolte, Bolling, Hudac, Fonagy, Mayes and Pelphrey2013; Luyten et al., Reference Luyten, Campbell, Allison and Fonagy2020). However, the current evidence is still limited, and further longitudinal studies are needed to clarify the role of mentalization within this framework. Future research should test whether mentalization deficits predict a broad spectrum of psychiatric outcomes over time, and whether enhancing mentalization in early intervention contexts could mitigate the long-term impact of early trauma.

While the cross-sectional design of the reviewed studies poses limitations, preventing us from establishing causal relationships, the presence of a mediation effect in all the six studies that investigated this aspect, supports the hypothesis that mentalization may serve as a protective factor for those who have experienced childhood trauma and should be further studied in longitudinal prospective studies. This highlights the importance of considering mentalization in both research and clinical practice, particularly when developing interventions for individuals with a history of trauma. Concerning the assessment of childhood trauma, the majority of the studies used retrospective self-report tools (CTQ, CECA), which might be prone to recall bias. Nevertheless, more and more studies suggest that these reports are reliable (Bernstein et al., Reference Bernstein, Stein, Newcomb, Walker, Pogge, Ahluvalia, Stokes, Handelsman, Medrano, Desmond and Zule2003; Read, van Os, Morrison, & Ross, Reference Read, van Os, Morrison and Ross2005; Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Craig, Fearon, Morgan, Dazzan, Lappin, Hutchinson, Doody, Jones, McGuffin, Murray, Leff and Morgan2009; Simpson et al., Reference Simpson, Phillips, Baksheev, Garner, Markulev, Phassouliotis, Alvarez-Jimenez, McGorry and Bendall2019). Additionally, except for two studies (Kincaid et al., Reference Kincaid, Shannon, Boyd, Hanna, McNeill, Anderson, Francis-Naylor and Mulholland2018; Schalinski et al., Reference Schalinski, Teicher, Carolus and Rockstroh2018), there was no information about the timing or the duration of the childhood adversity exposure, which might limit our understanding of these complex relationships (Fares-Otero & Schalinski, Reference Fares-Otero and Schalinski2024). The variability in methods used to assess mentalization across studies complicates the interpretation of results as well. The same instruments were applied to assess different concepts, and the same concepts were evaluated with varying tests, contributing to the often-conflicting results observed across the literature and preventing from performing a meta-analysis. Furthermore, in most of the studies, different terms such as ‘mentalization’, ‘ToM’, ‘metacognition’, ‘cognitive empathy’, ‘reflective functioning’, and ‘social cognition’ were used interchangeably and as synonyms, contributing to the complexity of the interpretation of their findings. This heterogeneity, partly due to the lack of agreement on the conceptualization of these terms, limited the feasibility and interpretability of meta-analytic aggregation, preventing us from carrying out quantitative analysis. Variability across studies also arose from the wide range of adversities examined, some of which – such as abuse and neglect – are not directly comparable. Future meta-analytical research confirming our findings is needed. Moreover, longitudinal prospective studies are needed to clarify the nature of the relationships between childhood trauma and mentalization. Such studies should also account for neurocognitive functions, medication use, and other confounding factors that may impact mentalization performance, such as socioeconomic status, psychosocial stressors, supportive environment, and resilience. A transdiagnostic approach, as suggested by our findings, could enhance our understanding of mentalization as a universal target for intervention across various psychiatric disorders.

Given the role of mentalization as a common factor underlying effective psychosocial interventions (Fonagy & Allison, Reference Fonagy and Allison2012), strengthening this capacity in therapeutic practices may be crucial for improving outcomes in patients with a history of trauma. One of the most studied approaches is MBT (Bateman, Fonagy, & Allen, Reference Bateman, Fonagy and Allen2009), originally developed for borderline personality disorder. MBT aims to enhance individuals’ ability to understand both their own and others’ mental states, particularly under emotional stress, by fostering reflection on interpersonal experiences and attachment-related responses – often disrupted in those who suffered from early trauma. Its application has since broadened to other clinical populations, namely to antisocial personality disorder (Bateman, Reference Bateman2022; Fonagy et al., Reference Fonagy, Simes, Yirmiya, Wason, Barrett, Frater, Cameron, Butler, Hoare, McMurran, Moran, Crawford, Pilling, Allison, Yakeley and Bateman2025), eating disorders (Robinson et al., Reference Robinson, Hellier, Barrett, Barzdaitiene, Bateman, Bogaardt, Clare, Somers, O’Callaghan, Goldsmith, Kern, Schmidt, Morando, Ouellet-Courtois, Roberts, Skårderud and Fonagy2016), substance use disorders (Fuggle et al., Reference Fuggle, Talbot, Wheeler, Rees, Ventre, Beehan, Hare, Bevington and Cracknell2021), and psychotic disorders (Weijers et al., Reference Weijers, Ten Kate, Viechtbauer, Rampaart, Eurelings and Selten2021), as well as to family (Asen & Fonagy, Reference Asen and Fonagy2012) and group therapy settings (Fonagy, Campbell, & Bateman, Reference Fonagy, Campbell and Bateman2017). Another relevant intervention is the Metacognitive Reflection and Insight Therapy (MERIT) (Lysaker et al., Reference Lysaker, Buck, Carcione, Procacci, Salvatore, Nicolò and Dimaggio2011; Van Donkersgoed et al., Reference Van Donkersgoed, De Jong, Van der Gaag, Aleman, Lysaker, Wunderink and Pijnenborg2014) developed specifically for individuals with schizophrenia (de Jong et al., Reference de Jong, van Donkersgoed, Timmerman, Aan Het Rot, Wunderink, Arends, van Der Gaag, Aleman, Lysaker and Pijnenborg2019; Hasson-Ohayon et al., Reference Hasson-Ohayon, Igra, Lavi-Rotenberg, Goldzweig and Lysaker2024). Unlike MBT, MERIT does not draw from a developmental or attachment-based model but shares the goal of enhancing self-reflection, psychological flexibility, and the ability to form complex and coherent representations of self and others. While originally designed for psychotic disorders, its principles may also benefit other clinical populations with social-cognitive impairments.

The growing implementation of interventions designed to foster mentalization skills – both in individual and group formats – reinforces the idea that enhancing this social cognitive ability may mitigate some of the adverse effects of childhood trauma on mental health.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this review underscores the significant relationship between childhood trauma, particularly neglect, and mentalization deficits transdiagnostically. As research in this area advances, a deeper understanding of these dynamics will be essential for developing targeted interventions that not only address mental health symptoms but also promote healthier interpersonal functioning and resilience in those affected by childhood trauma, ultimately aiming to prevent the emergence of psychiatric disorders.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725100822.

Funding statement

Professor Marco Armando is supported by the Fonds National Suisse fund n. FNS 227936. Dr. Luis Alameda thanks the Foundation Adrian and Simone Frutiger and Carigest SA Foundation for their support.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Footnotes

Marco Armando and Luis Alameda contributed equally to last authorship.

References

Addington, J., Penn, D., Woods, S. W., Addington, D., & Perkins, D. O. (2008). Social functioning in individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 99(1–3), 119124. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2007.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Addington, J., Shakeel, M. K., Braun, A., Bonneville, D., & Stowkowy, J. (2019). Metacognition in youth at-risk for psychosis. Schizophrenia Research, 210, 303305. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.07.005.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alameda, L., Rodriguez, V., Carr, E., Aas, M., Trotta, G., Marino, P., Vorontsova, N., Herane-Vives, A., Gadelrab, R., Spinazzola, E., Di Forti, M., Morgan, C., & Murray, R. M. (2020). A systematic review on mediators between adversity and psychosis: Potential targets for treatment. Psychological Medicine, 50(12), 19661976. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altman, D. G., Simera, I., Hoey, J., Moher, D., & Schulz, K. (2008). EQUATOR: Reporting guidelines for health research. Lancet, 371(9619), 11491150. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(08)60505-X.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Andreou, C., Kelm, L., Bierbrodt, J., Braun, V., Lipp, M., Yassari, A. H., & Moritz, S. (2015). Factors contributing to social cognition impairment in borderline personality disorder and schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 229(3), 872879. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2015.07.057.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asen, E., & Fonagy, P. (2012). Mentalization-based therapeutic interventions for families. Journal of Family Therapy, 34(4), 347370. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6427.2011.00552.x.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aydin, O., Balikci, K., Tas, C., Aydin, P. U., Danaci, A. E., Brune, M., & Lysaker, P. H. (2016). The developmental origins of metacognitive deficits in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 245, 1521.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(2), 241251.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, A., & Fonagy, P. (2008). 8-year follow-up of patients treated for borderline personality disorder: Mentalization-based treatment versus treatment as usual. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(5), 631638. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07040636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateman, A., Fonagy, P., & Allen, J. G. (2009). Theory and practice of mentalization-based therapy. In Textbook of psychotherapeutic treatments (pp. 757780). American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Bateman, A. W., & P. (2012). Handbook of mentalizing in mental health practice. American Psychiatric Publishing, Inc.Google Scholar
Bateman, A. W. (2022). Mentalizing and group psychotherapy: A novel treatment for antisocial personality disorder. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 75(1), 3237. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.20210023.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Battle, C. L., Shea, M. T., Johnson, D. M., Yen, S., Zlotnick, C., Zanarini, M. C., Sanislow, C. A., Skodol, A. E., Gunderson, J. G., Grilo, C. M., McGlashan, T. H., & Morey, L. C. (2004). Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 18(2), 193211. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.18.2.193.32777CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, M., Tsang, H. W., Greig, T. C., & Bryson, G. J. (2009). Neurocognition, social cognition, perceived social discomfort, and vocational outcomes in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35(4), 738747. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belvederi Murri, M., Ferrigno, G., Penati, S., Muzio, C., Piccinini, G., Innamorati, M., Ricci, F., Pompili, M., & Amore, M. (2017). Mentalization and depressive symptoms in a clinical sample of adolescents and young adults. Child and Adolescent Mental Health, 22 ( 2), 6976.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, D. P., Stein, J. A., Newcomb, M. D., Walker, E., Pogge, D., Ahluvalia, T., Stokes, J., Handelsman, L., Medrano, M., Desmond, D., & Zule, W. (2003). Development and validation of a brief screening version of the childhood trauma questionnaire. Child Abuse & Neglect, 27(2), 169190. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0145-2134(02)00541-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bifulco, A., Bernazzani, O., Moran, P. M., & Jacobs, C. (2005). The childhood experience of care and abuse questionnaire (CECA.Q): Validation in a community series. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 44(Pt 4), 563581. https://doi.org/10.1348/014466505X35344CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. Random House.Google Scholar
Brackett, M. A., & Salovey, P. (2006). Measuring emotional intelligence with the Mayer-Salovery-Caruso emotional intelligence test (MSCEIT). Psicothema, 18(Suppl), 3441.Google ScholarPubMed
Branas, A., Lahera, G., Barrigon, M. L., Canal-Rivero, M., & Ruiz-Veguilla, M. (2022). Effects of childhood trauma on facial recognition of fear in psychosis [Efectos del trauma infantil en el reconocimiento de la expresion facial de miedo en psicosis.]. Revista de Psiquiatria y Salud Mental, 15 ( 1), 2937.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brune, M., Walden, S., Edel, M. A., & Dimaggio, G. (2016). Mentalization of complex emotions in borderline personality disorder: The impact of parenting and exposure to trauma on the performance in a novel cartoon-based task. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 64, 2937.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2018). All for one and one for all: Mental disorders in one dimension. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(9), 831844. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.17121383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chiesa, M., & Fonagy, P. (2014). Reflective function as a mediator between childhood adversity, personality disorder and symptom distress. Personality and Mental Health, 8(1), 5266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., Maughan, A., Toth, S. L., & Bruce, J. (2003). False belief understanding in maltreated children. Development and Psychopathology, 15(4), 10671091. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579403000440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Corcoran, R., Mercer, G., & Frith, C. D. (1995). Schizophrenia, symptomatology and social inference: Investigating “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17(1), 513. https://doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(95)00024-G.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Couture, S. M., Penn, D. L., & Roberts, D. L. (2006). The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: A review. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 32(Suppl 1), S44S63. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbl029CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Jong, S., van Donkersgoed, R. J. M., Timmerman, M. E., Aan Het Rot, M., Wunderink, L., Arends, J., van Der Gaag, M., Aleman, A., Lysaker, P. H., & Pijnenborg, G. H. M. (2019). Metacognitive reflection and insight therapy (MERIT) for patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 49(2), 303313. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291718000855.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, A., Shipman, K., & Brown, A. (2005). The socialization of emotional understanding: A comparison of neglectful and nonneglectful mothers and their children. Child Maltreatment, 10(3), 293304. https://doi.org/10.1177/1077559505278452.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eidenmueller, K., Grimm, F., Hermann, D., Frischknecht, U., Montag, C., Dziobek, I., Kiefer, F., & Bekier, N. K. (2021). Exploring influences on theory of mind impairment in opioid dependent patients. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, Article 721690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Begin, M., Normandin, L., Godbout, N., & Fonagy, P. (2017). Mentalization and dissociation in the context of trauma: Implications for child psychopathology. Journal of Trauma & Dissociation, 18(1), 1130. https://doi.org/10.1080/15299732.2016.1172536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ensink, K., Normandin, L., Target, M., Fonagy, P., Sabourin, S., & Berthelot, N. (2015). Mentalization in children and mothers in the context of trauma: An initial study of the validity of the child reflective functioning scale. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 203217. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjdp.12074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fares-Otero, N. E., Alameda, L., Pfaltz, M. C., Martinez-Aran, A., Schafer, I., & Vieta, E. (2023). Examining associations, moderators and mediators between childhood maltreatment, social functioning, and social cognition in psychotic disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 53(13), 59095932. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723001678.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fares-Otero, N. E., De Prisco, M., Oliva, V., Radua, J., Halligan, S. L., Vieta, E., & Martinez-Aran, A. (2023). Association between childhood maltreatment and social functioning in individuals with affective disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 148(2), 142164. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13557.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fares-Otero, N. E., & Schalinski, I. (2024). Social cognition in maltreated individuals: Do type and timing of maltreatment matter? European Neuropsychopharmacology, 81, 3840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2023.12.011.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisher, H. L., Craig, T. K., Fearon, P., Morgan, K., Dazzan, P., Lappin, J., Hutchinson, G., Doody, G. A., Jones, P. B., McGuffin, P., Murray, R. M., Leff, J., & Morgan, C. (2009). Reliability and comparability of psychosis patients’ retrospective reports of childhood abuse. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 37(3), 546553. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbp103.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., & Allison, E. (2012). What is mentalization? The concept and its foundations in developmental research. In Minding the child: Mentalization-based interventions with children, young people and their families (pp. 1134). Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., Campbell, C., & Bateman, A. (2017). Mentalizing, attachment, and epistemic trust in group therapy. International Journal of Group Psychotherapy, 67(2), 176201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Gergely, G., Jurist, E. L., & Target, M. (2002). Affect regulation, mentalization, and the development of the self. Other Press.Google Scholar
Fonagy, P., & Luyten, P. (2009). A developmental, mentalization-based approach to the understanding and treatment of borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 21(4), 13551381. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409990198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fonagy, P., Luyten, P., & Strathearn, L. (2011). Borderline personality disorder, mentalization, and the neurobiology of attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 32(1), 4769. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.20283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., Simes, E., Yirmiya, K., Wason, J., Barrett, B., Frater, A., Cameron, A., Butler, S., Hoare, Z., McMurran, M., Moran, P., Crawford, M., Pilling, S., Allison, E., Yakeley, J., & Bateman, A. (2025). Mentalisation-based treatment for antisocial personality disorder in males convicted of an offence on community probation in England and Wales (Mentalization for offending adult males, MOAM): A multicentre, assessor-blinded, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Psychiatry, 12(3), 208219. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(24)00445-0.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., & Target, M. (1997). Attachment and reflective function: Their role in self-organization. Development and Psychopathology, 9(4), 679700. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579497001399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuggle, P., Talbot, L., Wheeler, J., Rees, J., Ventre, E., Beehan, V., Hare, S., Bevington, D., & Cracknell, L. (2021). Improving lives not just saying no to substances: Evaluating outcomes for a young people’s substance use team trained in the AMBIT approach. Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 26(2), 490504. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359104521994875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia, M., Montalvo, I., Creus, M., Cabezas, A., Sole, M., Algora, M. J., Moreno, I., Gutierrez-Zotes, A., & Labad, J. (2016). Sex differences in the effect of childhood trauma on the clinical expression of early psychosis. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 68, 8696.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Bearden, C. E., Cannon, T. D., Fiske, A. P., Hellemann, G. S., Horan, W. P., Kee, K., Kern, R. S., Lee, J., Sergi, M. J., Subotnik, K. L., Sugar, C. A., Ventura, J., Yee, C. M., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2012). Social cognition in schizophrenia, part 1: Performance across phase of illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(4), 854864. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq171.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, M. F., Penn, D. L., Bentall, R., Carpenter, W. T., Gaebel, W., Gur, R. C., Kring, A. M., Park, S., Silverstein, S. M., & Heinssen, R. (2008). Social cognition in schizophrenia: An NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 34(6), 12111220. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbm145.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guhn, A., Merkel, L., Hubner, L., Dziobek, I., Sterzer, P., & Kohler, S. (2020). Understanding versus feeling the emotions of others: How persistent and recurrent depression affect empathy. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 130, 120127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasson-Ohayon, I., Igra, L., Lavi-Rotenberg, A., Goldzweig, G., & Lysaker, P. H. (2024). Findings from a randomized controlled trial of metacognitive reflection and insight therapy for people with schizophrenia: Effects on metacognition and symptoms. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 97(Suppl 1), 7590. https://doi.org/10.1111/papt.12485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healey, K. M., Bartholomeusz, C. F., & Penn, D. L. (2016). Deficits in social cognition in first episode psychosis: A review of the literature. Clinical Psychology Review, 50, 108137. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.10.001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jimenez, E., Sole, B., Arias, B., Mitjans, M., Varo, C., Reinares, M., Bonnin, C. D. M., Ruiz, V., Saiz, P. A., Garcia-Portilla, M. P., Buron, P., Bobes, J., Amann, B. L., Martinez-Aran, A., Torrent, C., Vieta, E., & Benabarre, A. (2017). Impact of childhood trauma on cognitive profile in bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 19(5), 363374. https://doi.org/10.1111/bdi.12514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kessler, R. C., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., Benjet, C., Bromet, E. J., Cardoso, G., Degenhardt, L., de Girolamo, G., Dinolova, R. V., Ferry, F., Florescu, S., Gureje, O., Haro, J. M., Huang, Y., Karam, E. G., Kawakami, N., Lee, S., Lepine, J. P., Levinson, D., … Koenen, K. C. (2017). Trauma and PTSD in the WHO world mental health surveys. European Journal of Psychotraumatology, 8(sup5), 1353383. https://doi.org/10.1080/20008198.2017.1353383.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilian, S., Asmal, L., Chiliza, B., Olivier, M. R., Phahladira, L., Scheffler, F., Seedat, S., Marder, S. R., Green, M. F., & Emsley, R. (2018). Childhood adversity and cognitive function in schizophrenia spectrum disorders and healthy controls: Evidence for an association between neglect and social cognition. Psychological Medicine, 48 ( 13), 21862193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kincaid, D., Shannon, C., Boyd, A., Hanna, D., McNeill, O., Anderson, R., Francis-Naylor, M., & Mulholland, C. (2018). An investigation of associations between experience of childhood trauma and political violence and theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 270, 293297.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsson, S., Andreassen, O. A., Aas, M., Rossberg, J. I., Mork, E., Steen, N. E., Barrett, E. A., Lagerberg, T. V., Peleikis, D., Agartz, I., Melle, I., & Lorentzen, S. (2013). High prevalence of childhood trauma in patients with schizophrenia spectrum and affective disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 54(2), 123127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2012.06.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, E. T., Carracher, E., & Bird, T. (2020). Linking childhood emotional abuse and adult depressive symptoms: The role of mentalizing incapacity. Child Abuse and Neglect, 99, Article 104253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luyten, P., Campbell, C., Allison, E., & Fonagy, P. (2020). The Mentalizing approach to psychopathology: State of the art and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 297325. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Buck, K. D., Carcione, A., Procacci, M., Salvatore, G., Nicolò, G., & Dimaggio, G. (2011). Addressing metacognitive capacity for self reflection in the psychotherapy for schizophrenia: A conceptual model of the key tasks and processes. Psychology and Psychotherapy, 84(1), 5869; discussion 98–110. https://doi.org/10.1348/147608310x520436CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Carcione, A., Dimaggio, G., Johannesen, J. K., Nicolò, G., Procacci, M., & Semerari, A. (2005). Metacognition amidst narratives of self and illness in schizophrenia: Associations with neurocognition, symptoms, insight and quality of life. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112(1), 6471. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00514.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lysaker, P. H., Gagen, E., Moritz, S., & Schweitzer, R. D. (2018). Metacognitive approaches to the treatment of psychosis: A comparison of four approaches. Psychology Research and Behavior Management, 11, 341351. https://doi.org/10.2147/PRBM.S146446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lysaker Paul, H. P. H., Gumley, A., Brune, M., Vanheule, S., Buck, K. D., & Dimaggio, G. (2011). Deficits in the ability to recognize one’s own affects and those of others: Associations with neurocognition, symptoms and sexual trauma among persons with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Consciousness and Cognition, 20 ( 4), 11831192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mansueto, G., Schruers, K., Cosci, F., van Os, J., Alizadeh, B. Z., Bartels-Velthuis, A. A., van Beveren, N. J., Bruggeman, R., Cahn, W., de Haan, L., Delespaul, P., Meijer, C. J., Myin-Germeys, I., Kahn, R. S., Schirmbeck, F., Simons, C. J. P., van Haren, N. E. M., & van Winkel, R. (2019). Childhood adversities and psychotic symptoms: The potential mediating or moderating role of neurocognition and social cognition. Schizophrenia Research, 206, 183193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin-Gagnon, G., Normandin, L., Fonagy, P., & Ensink, K. (2023). Adolescent mentalizing and childhood emotional abuse: Implications for depression, anxiety, and borderline personality disorder features. Frontiers in Psychology, 14, 1237735. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1237735.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McKay, M. T., Cannon, M., Chambers, D., Conroy, R. M., Coughlan, H., Dodd, P., Healy, C., O’Donnell, L., & Clarke, M. C. (2021). Childhood trauma and adult mental disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal cohort studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 143(3), 189205. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, K. A., Sheridan, M. A., & Lambert, H. K. (2014). Childhood adversity and neural development: Deprivation and threat as distinct dimensions of early experience. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 47, 578591. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.012.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morgan, R. L., Whaley, P., Thayer, K. A., & Schünemann, H. J. (2018). Identifying the PECO: A framework for formulating good questions to explore the association of environmental and other exposures with health outcomes. Environment International, 121(Pt 1), 10271031. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.envint.2018.07.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nazarov, A., Frewen, P., Parlar, M., Oremus, C., MacQueen, G., McKinnon, M., & Lanius, R. (2014). Theory of mind performance in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood abuse. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 129(3), 193201. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nemeth, N., Matrai, P., Hegyi, P., Czeh, B., Czopf, L., Hussain, A., Pammer, J., Szabo, I., Solymar, M., Kiss, L., Hartmann, P., Szilagyi, A. L., Kiss, Z., & Simon, M. (2018). Theory of mind disturbances in borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychiatry Research, 270, 143153. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2018.08.049.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nolte, T., Bolling, D. Z., Hudac, C. M., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2013). Brain mechanisms underlying the impact of attachment-related stress on social cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 816. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00816.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ostefjells, T., Lystad, J. U., Berg, A. O., Hagen, R., Loewy, R., Sandvik, L., Melle, I., & Rossberg, J. I. (2017). Metacognitive beliefs mediate the effect of emotional abuse on depressive and psychotic symptoms in severe mental disorders. Psychological Medicine, 47 ( 13), 23232333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Page, M. J., McKenzie, J. E., Bossuyt, P. M., Boutron, I., Hoffmann, T. C., Mulrow, C. D., Shamseer, L., Tetzlaff, J. M., Akl, E. A., Brennan, S. E., Chou, R., Glanville, J., Grimshaw, J. M., Hrobjartsson, A., Lalu, M. M., Li, T., Loder, E. W., Mayo-Wilson, E., McDonald, S., … Moher, D. (2021). The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. Revista Española de Cardiología, 74(9), 790799. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rec.2021.07.010 (Declaracion PRISMA 2020: una guia actualizada para la publicacion de revisiones sistematicas.)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmier-Claus, J., Berry, K., Darrell-Berry, H., Emsley, R., Parker, S., Drake, R., & Bucci, S. (2016). Childhood adversity and social functioning in psychosis: Exploring clinical and cognitive mediators. Psychiatry Research, 238, 2532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parlar, M., Frewen, P., Nazarov, A., Oremus, C., MacQueen, G., Lanius, R., & McKinnon, M. C. (2014). Alterations in empathic responding among women with posttraumatic stress disorder associated with childhood trauma. Brain and Behavior: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective, 4(3), 381389. https://doi.org/10.1002/brb3.215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pears, K. C., & Fisher, P. A. (2005). Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: Evidence of deficits. Development and Psychopathology, 17(1), 4765. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579405050030.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, R., Brakoulias, V., & Langdon, R. (2016). An experimental investigation of mentalization ability in borderline personality disorder. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 64, 1221.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Quide, Y., Cohen-Woods, S., O’Reilly, N., Carr, V. J., Elzinga, B. M., & Green, M. J. (2018). Schizotypal personality traits and social cognition are associated with childhood trauma exposure. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 57 ( 4), 397419.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Read, J., van Os, J., Morrison, A. P., & Ross, C. A. (2005). Childhood trauma, psychosis and schizophrenia: A literature review with theoretical and clinical implications. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 112(5), 330350. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2005.00634.x.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rnic, K., Sabbagh, M. A., Washburn, D., Bagby, R. M., Ravindran, A., Kennedy, J. L., Strauss, J., & Harkness, K. L. (2018). Childhood emotional abuse, physical abuse, and neglect are associated with theory of mind decoding accuracy in young adults with depression. Psychiatry Research, 268, 501507.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, P., Hellier, J., Barrett, B., Barzdaitiene, D., Bateman, A., Bogaardt, A., Clare, A., Somers, N., O’Callaghan, A., Goldsmith, K., Kern, N., Schmidt, U., Morando, S., Ouellet-Courtois, C., Roberts, A., Skårderud, F., & Fonagy, P. (2016). The NOURISHED randomised controlled trial comparing mentalisation-based treatment for eating disorders (MBT-ED) with specialist supportive clinical management (SSCM-ED) for patients with eating disorders and symptoms of borderline personality disorder. Trials, 17(1), 549. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13063-016-1606-8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez, V., Aas, M., Vorontsova, N., Trotta, G., Gadelrab, R., Rooprai, N. K., & Alameda, L. (2021). Exploring the interplay between adversity, Neurocognition, social cognition, and functional outcome in people with psychosis: A narrative review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 596949. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.596949.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokita, K. I., Dauvermann, M. R., & Donohoe, G. (2018). Early life experiences and social cognition in major psychiatric disorders: A systematic review. European Psychiatry, 53, 123133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2018.06.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokita, K. I., Dauvermann, M. R., Mothersill, D., Holleran, L., Holland, J., Costello, L., Cullen, C., Kane, R., McKernan, D., Morris, D. W., Kelly, J., Gill, M., Corvin, A., Hallahan, B., McDonald, C., & Donohoe, G. (2021). Childhood trauma, parental bonding, and social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy adults. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 77 ( 1), 241253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schalinski, I., Teicher, M. H., Carolus, A. M., & Rockstroh, B. (2018). Defining the impact of childhood adversities on cognitive deficits in psychosis: An exploratory analysis. Schizophrenia Research, 192, 351356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sharp, C., & Venta, A. (2013). Mentalizing problems in children and adolescents. In Minding the child (pp. 3553). Routledge.Google Scholar
Shipman, K. L., & Zeman, J. (1999). Emotional understanding: A comparison of physically maltreating and nonmaltreating mother-child dyads. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 28(3), 407417. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424jccp280313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, M., Nemeth, N., Galber, M., Lakner, E., Csernela, E., Tenyi, T., & Czeh, B. (2019). Childhood adversity impairs theory of mind abilities in adult patients with major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 10, Article 867.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, S., Phillips, L., Baksheev, G., Garner, B., Markulev, C., Phassouliotis, C., Alvarez-Jimenez, M., McGorry, P., & Bendall, S. (2019). Stability of retrospective self-reports of childhood trauma in first-episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 13(4), 908913. https://doi.org/10.1111/eip.12700.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teicher, M. H., Samson, J. A., Anderson, C. M., & Ohashi, K. (2016). The effects of childhood maltreatment on brain structure, function and connectivity. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience, 17(10), 652666. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2016.111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trauelsen, A. M., Gumley, A., Jansen, J. E., Pedersen, M. B., Nielsen, H. G. L., Haahr, U. H., & Simonsen, E. (2019). Does childhood trauma predict poorer metacognitive abilities in people with first-episode psychosis? Psychiatry Research, 273, 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trauelsen, A. M., Gumley, A., Jansen, J. E., Pedersen, M. B., Nielsen, H. L., Trier, C. H., Haahr, U. H., & Simonsen, E. (2016). Metacognition in first-episode psychosis and its association with positive and negative symptom profiles. Psychiatry Research, 238, 1423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2016.02.003.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Donkersgoed, R. J., De Jong, S., Van der Gaag, M., Aleman, A., Lysaker, P. H., Wunderink, L., & Pijnenborg, G. H. (2014). A manual-based individual therapy to improve metacognition in schizophrenia: Protocol of a multi-center RCT. BMC Psychiatry, 14, 27. https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-244x-14-27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vargas, T., Lam, P. H., Azis, M., Osborne, K. J., Lieberman, A., & Mittal, V. A. (2019). Childhood trauma and Neurocognition in adults with psychotic disorders: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(6), 11951208. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sby150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaskinn, A., Melle, I., Aas, M., & Berg, A. O. (2021). Sexual abuse and physical neglect in childhood are associated with affective theory of mind in adults with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research: Cognition, 23, Article 100189.Google ScholarPubMed
Weijers, J., Fonagy, P., Eurelings-Bontekoe, E., Termorshuizen, F., Viechtbauer, W., & Selten, J. P. (2018). Mentalizing impairment as a mediator between reported childhood abuse and outcome in nonaffective psychotic disorder. Psychiatry Research, 259, 463469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weijers, J., Ten Kate, C., Viechtbauer, W., Rampaart, L. J. A., Eurelings, E. H. M., & Selten, J. P. (2021). Mentalization-based treatment for psychotic disorder: A rater-blinded, multi-center, randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 51(16), 28462855. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720001506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wells, G., Shea, B., O’connell, D., Peterson, J., Welch, V., Losos, M., & Tugwell, P. (2014). Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale cohort studies. University of Ottawa.Google Scholar
Whitten, T., Tzoumakis, S., Green, M. J., & Dean, K. (2024). Global prevalence of childhood exposure to physical violence within domestic and family relationships in the general population: A systematic review and proportional meta-analysis. Trauma Violence Abuse, 25(2), 14111430. https://doi.org/10.1177/15248380231179133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, L., & Huang, M. (2024). Childhood maltreatment and mentalizing capacity: A meta-analysis. Child Abuse & Neglect, 149, 106623. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2023.106623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flowchart of studies selected for systematic review.

Figure 1

Table 1. Characteristics of the studies included in the review

Figure 2

Figure 2. Clinical populations represented in the selected studies.

Figure 3

Figure 3. Association between CT and mentalization by subtypes of CT.

Supplementary material: File

Gorgellino et al. supplementary material

Gorgellino et al. supplementary material
Download Gorgellino et al. supplementary material(File)
File 59.7 KB