Hostname: page-component-6bb9c88b65-znhjv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-07-24T12:02:04.300Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Obstetric complications, cortical gyrification, and cognition in first-episode psychosis

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 July 2025

Ana Costas-Carrera
Affiliation:
Hospital Universitario Central de Asturias (HUCA), Oviedo, Spain Medicine Department, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
Norma Verdolini
Affiliation:
Department of Mental Health, Umbria 1 Mental Health Center, Perugia, Italy
Gisela Mezquida
Affiliation:
Serra-Hunter Fellow, Department of Basic Clinal Practice, Pharmacology Unit, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Barcelona Clínic Schizophrenia Unit (BCSU), Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
Maria Florencia Forte
Affiliation:
Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clinic de Barcelona, Institute of Neurosciences, Barcelona, Spain
Joost Janssen
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
Clemente Garcia-Rizo*
Affiliation:
Medicine Department, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Barcelona Clínic Schizophrenia Unit (BCSU), Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
Anabel Martinez-Aran
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain Bipolar and Depressive Disorders Unit, Hospital Clinic de Barcelona, Institute of Neurosciences, Barcelona, Spain
Pablo Andres-Camazon
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, IiSGM, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
Ana Maria Sánchez-Torres
Affiliation:
Department of Health Sciences, Universidad Pública de Navarra, Pamplona, Spain Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria de Navarra (IdiSNA), Pamplona, Spain Institut de Salud Mental, Hospital del Mar, Barcelona, Spain
Daniel Berge
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut de Salud Mental, Hospital del Mar, Barcelona, Spain Hospital del Mar Research Institute, Barcelona, Spain Department of Medicine and Life Sciences, Universitat Pompeu Fabra (UPF), Barcelona, Spain
Elena de la Serna
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychology, Institut Clinic de Neurociències, Hospital Clínic Universitari, Barcelona, Spain
Rafael Penades
Affiliation:
Medicine Department, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Barcelona Clínic Schizophrenia Unit (BCSU), Neuroscience Institute, Hospital Clinic de Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Barcelona, Spain
Isabel Valli
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK FIDMAG, Germanes Hospitalaries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
Silvia Amoretti
Affiliation:
Biomedical Research Networking Center for Mental Health Network (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain Group of Psychiatry, Mental Health and Addictions, Vall d’Hebron Research Institute (VHIR), Barcelona, Spain
*
Corresponding author: Clemente Garcia-Rizo; Email: cgarcia3@clinic.cat
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Obstetric complications (OCs) are associated with cognitive and brain abnormalities observed in patients with schizophrenia. Gyrification, a measure of cortical integrity sensitive to events occurring during the prenatal and perinatal periods, is also altered in first-episode psychosis (FEP). We examined the relationship between OCs and gyrification in FEP, as well as whether gyrification mediates the relationship between OCs and cognition.

Methods

We examined differences in the Local Gyrification Index (LGI) for the frontal, parietal, temporal, occipital, and cingulate cortices between 139 FEP patients and 125 healthy controls (HCs). Regression analyses explored whether OCs and diagnosis interact to explain LGI variation. Parametric mediation analyses were conducted to assess the effect of LGI on the relationship between OCs and cognition for FEP and HC.

Results

Significant LGI differences were observed between FEP patients and HC in the left parietal and bilateral cingulate and occipital cortices. There was a significant interaction between OCs and diagnosis on the left cingulate cortex (LCC) that was specific to males (p = 0.04) and was driven by gestational rather than intrauterine OCs.

In HCs, OCs had a direct effect on working memory (WM) (p = 0.048) in the mediation analysis, whereas in FEP, we observed no significant effect of OCs on either verbal or WM.

Conclusions

OCs interact with diagnosis to predict LCC gyrification, such that males with FEP exposed to OCs exhibit the lowest LGI. OCs influence WM, and LCC gyrification may mediate this relation only in HC, suggesting a differential neurodevelopmental process in psychosis.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Schizophrenia results from the complex interplay of genetic and environmental factors, which can impact the trajectories of brain development, particularly over the prenatal and perinatal stages, due to the important brain changes that occur during the intrauterine and the early postnatal period (Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi, & Suzuki, Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021). Obstetric complications (OCs) have been associated with the neural network abnormalities observed in schizophrenia (Keshavan & Hogarty, Reference Keshavan and Hogarty1999). Exposure to OCs has been reported to influence the subsequent development of schizophrenia with an odds ratio between 1.17 and 3.5, and the largest effect sizes observed for polyhydramnios, premature rupture of membranes, low birth weight, and birth hypoxia (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Segre, Estradé, Radua, De Micheli, Provenzani and Fusar-Poli2020).

Reduced gray matter volume is a well-established feature of schizophrenia (De Peri et al., Reference De Peri, Crescini, Deste, Fusar-Poli, Sacchetti and Vita2012; Ellison-Wright, Glahn, Laird, Thelen, & Bullmore, Reference Ellison-Wright, Glahn, Laird, Thelen and Bullmore2008), especially in regions such as the frontal and temporal cortices, the cingulate, thalamus, and hippocampus (Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018). However, measures of cortical morphology, such as surface area (SA), cortical thickness (CTh), and gyrification, exhibit distinct maturational trajectories (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015). The latter refers to the development of brain surface folding patterns that, based on curvature and sulcal morphology, contribute to cortical SA (Fernández & Borrell, Reference Fernández and Borrell2023; Tonya White, Su, Schmidt, Kao, & Sapiro, Reference White, Su, Schmidt, Kao and Sapiro2010). Gyrification begins between 10 and 15 weeks of gestation and continues through the early postnatal period, remaining relatively stable thereafter (White et al., Reference White, Su, Schmidt, Kao and Sapiro2010). In contrast, maturational changes in SA and CTh exhibit a more protracted timeline, continuing throughout adolescence (Zilles, Palomero-Gallagher, & Amunts, Reference Zilles, Palomero-Gallagher and Amunts2013). Therefore, abnormalities in gyrification may reflect prenatal or perinatal adverse events (Papini et al., Reference Papini, Palaniyappan, Kroll, Froudist-Walsh, Murray and Nosarti2020; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015) and could be used as markers of early neurodevelopment (Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Kraguljac, White, Jindal, Shin and Lahti2020).

In healthy controls (HCs), cortical gyrification is related to general cognitive ability (Papini et al., Reference Papini, Palaniyappan, Kroll, Froudist-Walsh, Murray and Nosarti2020) and with language and working memory (WM) domains (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ringwald, Meller, Stein, Brosch, Pfarr and Kircher2023). Exposure to obstetric risk factors, such as very preterm birth, impacts the Local Gyrification Index (LGI) and is related to poorer general cognitive ability, with cognitive deficits and higher rates of psychopathology (Papini et al., Reference Papini, Palaniyappan, Kroll, Froudist-Walsh, Murray and Nosarti2020; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ringwald, Meller, Stein, Brosch, Pfarr and Kircher2023).

In schizophrenia, abnormal gyrification is considered an endophenotype reflecting aberrant connectivity (White & Gottesman, Reference White and Gottesman2012). Earlier sulci of the brain are thought to be correlated with genetic influences, while tertiary sulci are associated with nongenetic exposures to environmental risk factors (Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi, & Suzuki, Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021). Abnormal gyrification has been described both in the at-risk mental state (ARMS) and first-episode psychosis (FEP), suggesting it could be an early imaging biomarker for psychosis (Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018; Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021; White & Gottesman, Reference White and Gottesman2012).

In chronic schizophrenia, gyrification abnormalities were reported to be very heterogeneous (White & Gottesman, Reference White and Gottesman2012). Some studies have reported increased LGI, which refers to the ratio between the complete superficial contour and the outer contour of the cortex (Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018) in regions such as the prefrontal cortex, insula, and temporo-parieto-occipital cortices (Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021; Spalthoff, Gaser, & Nenadić, Reference Spalthoff, Gaser and Nenadić2018; White & Gottesman, Reference White and Gottesman2012). In contrast, other studies have found decreased LGI (Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018; Nelson et al., Reference Nelson, Kraguljac, White, Jindal, Shin and Lahti2020; Nesvåg et al., Reference Nesvåg, Schaer, Haukvik, Westlye, Rimol, Lange and Eliez2014; Palaniyappan & Liddle, Reference Palaniyappan and Liddle2012; Palaniyappan et al., Reference Palaniyappan, Marques, Taylor, Handley, Mondelli, Bonaccorso and Dazzan2013; Rychagov et al., Reference Rychagov, del Re, Zeng, Oykhman, Lizano, McDowell and Keshavan2024) predominantly in frontotemporal regions (Nanda et al., Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Giakoumatos, Abhishekh, Clementz and Keshavan2014; Palaniyappan, Mallikarjun, Joseph, White, & Liddle, Reference Palaniyappan, Mallikarjun, Joseph, White and Liddle2011; Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021), including the insula, superior temporal gyrus, Broca’s area, left precentral gyrus, right middle temporal gyrus, precuneus, and cingulate cortex (Madeira et al., Reference Madeira, Duarte, Martins, Costa, Macedo and Castelo-Branco2020; Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018; Nanda et al., Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Giakoumatos, Abhishekh, Clementz and Keshavan2014; Nesvåg et al., Reference Nesvåg, Schaer, Haukvik, Westlye, Rimol, Lange and Eliez2014; Wheeler & Harper, Reference Wheeler and Harper2007). Inconsistencies were considered to reflect methodological differences, heterogeneity in demographic and clinical variables, or abnormalities being specific depending on the brain region examined (Si et al., Reference Si, Bi, Yu, See, Kelly, Ambrogi and Kempton2024).

Heterogeneity in gyrification abnormalities has also been reported in the early stages of psychosis, with several studies in individuals with FEP and ARMS reporting hypergyria in various brain regions (Narr et al., Reference Narr, Bilder, Kim, Thompson, Szeszko, Robinson and Toga2004; Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021) and also hypogyria in the frontotemporal and insular cortices (Harris et al., Reference Harris, Yates, Miller, Best, Johnstone and Lawrie2004; Palaniyappan et al., Reference Palaniyappan, Marques, Taylor, Handley, Mondelli, Bonaccorso and Dazzan2013). Zhou et al. (Reference Zhou, Wang, Wang, Xu, Cao and Zhang2021) observed a higher LGI in regions such as the left lateral occipital cortex, and lower LGI in other regions such as the left transverse temporal cortex in drug-naïve patients with FEP (Matsuda & Ohi, Reference Matsuda and Ohi2018; H. Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Wang, Wang, Xu, Cao and Zhang2021). Sasabayashi et al. reported increased LGI in the left medial occipital cortex of individuals with ARMS who subsequently developed psychosis and considered it as a possible endophenotype of subsequent transition (Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takayanagi, Takahashi, Koike, Yamasue, Katagiri and Suzuki2017).

In schizophrenia, several brain abnormalities have been related to OCs exposure (Costas-Carrera, Garcia-Rizo, Bitanihirwe, & Penadés, Reference Costas-Carrera, Garcia-Rizo, Bitanihirwe and Penadés2020), including reduced cortical volume (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, van Erp, Rosso, Huttunen, Lönnqvist, Pirkola and Standertskjöld-Nordenstam2002; Neilson et al., Reference Neilson, Bois, Clarke, Hall, Johnstone, Owens and Lawrie2018; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015), reduced hippocampal volume (McNeil, Cantor-Graae, & Weinberger, Reference McNeil, Cantor-Graae and Weinberger2000; Schulze et al., Reference Schulze, McDonald, Frangou, Sham, Grech, Toulopoulou and Murray2003; Stefanis et al., Reference Stefanis, Frangou, Yakeley, Sharma, O’Connell, Morgan and Murray1999), and an increased ventricle–brain ratio (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, van Erp, Rosso, Huttunen, Lönnqvist, Pirkola and Standertskjöld-Nordenstam2002; Costas-Carrera et al., Reference Costas-Carrera, Verdolini, Garcia-Rizo, Mezquida, Janssen, Valli and Bernardo2024; McNeil et al., Reference McNeil, Cantor-Graae and Weinberger2000; Neilson et al., Reference Neilson, Bois, Clarke, Hall, Johnstone, Owens and Lawrie2018). Only three studies analyzed the relationship between OCs and gyrification in patients with schizophrenia (Falkai et al., Reference Falkai, Honer, Kamer, Dustert, Vogeley, Schneider-Axmann and Tepest2007; Haukvik et al., Reference Haukvik, Schaer, Nesvåg, McNeil, Hartberg, Jönsson and Agartz2012; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015). Falkai et al. observed no effect of OC exposure on gyrification in schizophrenia when using a two-dimensional method, whereas Haukvik et al. (Reference Haukvik, Schaer, Nesvåg, McNeil, Hartberg, Jönsson and Agartz2012) and Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015) reported a relationship between OCs and hypogyria . However, only Smith et al. (Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015) observed that the relationship between OCs and hypogyria was specific to patients with FEP and not in HCs (Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015).

Gyrification has been associated with cognition in both HCs (Gautam, Anstey, Wen, Sachdev, & Cherbuin, Reference Gautam, Anstey, Wen, Sachdev and Cherbuin2015; Green et al., Reference Green, Blackmon, Thesen, DuBois, Wang, Halgren and Devinsky2018; Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ringwald, Meller, Stein, Brosch, Pfarr and Kircher2023) and patients diagnosed with schizophrenia (Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takayanagi, Takahashi, Koike, Yamasue, Katagiri and Suzuki2017). Gautam et al. observed that greater gyrification of the lateral frontal cortex was associated with better performance in terms of executive function in HCs (Gautam et al., Reference Gautam, Anstey, Wen, Sachdev and Cherbuin2015), whereas Green et al. observed a positive relationship between measures in the parietal–frontal regions and WM in HCs (Green et al., Reference Green, Blackmon, Thesen, DuBois, Wang, Halgren and Devinsky2018). In patients with schizophrenia, greater gyrification of the right frontal cortex was associated with worse executive function (Sasabayashi et al., Reference Sasabayashi, Takahashi, Takayanagi and Suzuki2021), supporting an aberrant neurodevelopment in schizophrenia. Fetal growth is also correlated with cognition, and variables such as birth weight and gestational age are considered markers of the intrauterine environment (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ringwald, Meller, Stein, Brosch, Pfarr and Kircher2023). Gestational age moderates the association between gyrification in regions such as the left supramarginal gyrus and the left superior frontal gyrus with attention/WM (Schmitt et al., Reference Schmitt, Ringwald, Meller, Stein, Brosch, Pfarr and Kircher2023). In adults born premature, increased frontal–temporal–parietal gyrification was related to worse cognitive performance (Hedderich et al., Reference Hedderich, Bäuml, Berndt, Menegaux, Scheef, Daamen and Sorg2019). Papini et al. reported widespread hypogyria in adults born preterm, especially in the middle and inferior frontal, superior temporal, and medial occipito-parietal regions, and found a relationship between LGI and cognition, with the spatial distribution of these associations substantially differing between preterm-born individuals and HCs (Papini et al., Reference Papini, Palaniyappan, Kroll, Froudist-Walsh, Murray and Nosarti2020).

OCs exert a differential impact on fetal outcomes regarding sex, suggesting a sexual dimorphism effect, which implies different outcomes regarding the timeframe of the event (Yu, Chen, Ge, & Wang, Reference Yu, Chen, Ge and Wang2021). This effect is particularly relevant in schizophrenia, as sex-related brain structural and cognitive differences have been described (Mendrek & Mancini-Marïe, Reference Mendrek and Mancini-Marïe2016).

The assessment of FEP patients provides valuable insights into how adverse prenatal and perinatal factors may contribute to neurodevelopmental vulnerabilities that increase the risk of psychosis without the potential confounding effect of protracted illness and medication exposure. We, therefore, sought to examine the impact of OCs on gyrification in FEP patients and their relationship with cognitive performance.

We hypothesized that there would be differences in gyrification between FEP patients and HCs, with a lower LGI in FEP patients, and that these differences would be related to OC exposure. We also hypothesized that cortical folding patterns would play a role in the relationship between OCs and cognition, and examined the potential effect of sex and the timeframe of OC exposure.

Methods and materials

This research was conducted as part of a multicenter longitudinal study examining gene–environment interactions on the pathway to psychosis (the PEPs study, ‘Phenotype–genotype and environmental interaction: Application of a predictive model in first psychotic episodes’, Bernardo et al. (Reference Bernardo, Bioque, Parellada, Saiz Ruiz, Cuesta, Llerena and Cabrera2013)).

Participants

The sample of the PEPs study included 335 FEP patients and 253 HCs recruited between January 2009 and December 2011. The inclusion criteria and characteristics of the study have been previously described in detail (Salagre et al., Reference Salagre, Arango, Artigas, Ayuso-Mateos, Bernardo, Castro-Fornieles and Vieta2019). Briefly, subjects with FEP aged 7–35 years, presenting psychotic symptoms for <12 months, were recruited from the inpatient and outpatient units of 16 participating Spanish centers, 14 of which are members of the Center of Biomedical Research Network on Mental Health (CIBERSAM). HCs were recruited at each site through advertisements and matched with patients by age (within ±10%), sex, and parental socioeconomic status, as measured by the Hollingshead–Redlich scale (within ±1 level). For the neuroimaging component of the study, a maximum of 6 months was established from inclusion to scan time. All centers received the approval of their respective Independent Ethics Committee. All procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, as revised in 2008. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants before they participated in the study and from parents/legal guardians for children under 16 years of age (children gave assent). In the present study, from the total sample, we included 139 individuals with FEP and 125 HCs based on the availability of data for magnetic resonance imaging, cognitive assessments, and OC exposure.

History of OC assessment

OCs were assessed using the Lewis–Murray scale through a family interview (Lewis & Murray, Reference Lewis and Murray1987). The scale groups OCs into three categories, A, B, and C, according to the type of complication defined as follows (M. Cannon, Jones, & Murray, Reference Cannon, Jones and Murray2002; Mezquida et al., Reference Mezquida, Fernandez-Egea, Treen, Mané, Bergé, Savulich and Garcia-Rizo2018):

  1. A. Complications of pregnancy: syphilis or rubella, rhesus isoimmunization/Rh incompatibility, severe preeclampsia requiring hospitalization or induction of labor, and bleeding before delivery or threatened abortion.

  2. B. Abnormal fetal growth and development: twin delivery, preterm birth before 37 weeks, or long-term after 42 weeks, weight at birth <2500 g, and any important physical abnormality.

  3. C. Difficulties in delivery: premature rupture of membranes, duration of delivery more than 36 h or less than 3 h, umbilical cord prolapse, complicated cesarean delivery, abnormal fetal presentation, use of forceps, and being in an incubator for more than 4 weeks.

Participants were stratified based on exposure, which the Lewis–Murray scale classifies into definite and dubious based on the quality of the information. We included only definite events (yes/no) grouped as intrauterine (Lewis A and B) and delivery complications (Lewis C), as well as the total score (Lewis T).

Image acquisition and processing

After all DICOM images were converted to NIFTI format using the dcm2niix function from MRIcron software, the FreeSurfer analysis package (v7.1.1, https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) was used to generate measurements of CTh, cortical and subcortical volumes, and LGI. The standard FreeSurfer processing pipeline was employed, which follows the workflow: motion and bias field correction, skull extraction, affine and nonlinear alignment to the Talairach atlas, subcortical division, and cortical segmentation using the Desikan–Killiany atlas. For quality assurance, a visual inspection of the segmentation was performed by a technician specialized in neuroimaging, following the quality control protocol 2.0 of the ENIGMA consortium (https://enigma.ini.usc.edu/protocols/imaging-protocols).

The LGI was calculated using FreeSurfer with default parameters (vertex-wise values were averaged for each lobe [frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital] and area [cingulate cortex], considering the right and left hemispheres separately, Kernel size 10 mm; Madan & Kensinger (Reference Madan and Kensinger2017)).

In this multicenter study, data were collected from six distinct neuroimaging centers using different scanners (Siemens Magnetom Trio Tim 3T, Siemens Symphony 1.5T, Philips Achieva 3T, Philips Intera 1.5T, GE Signa Horizon MX 1.5T, and GE Signa Excite 1.5T). Detailed information on the acquisition parameters from each participating platform can be found in previous work from the collaborative study (Pina-Camacho et al., Reference Pina-Camacho, Del Rey-Mejías, Janssen, Bioque, González-Pinto, Arango and Parellada2016). To adjust for site, we employed the ComBat batch harmonization method (Fortin et al., Reference Fortin, Cullen, Sheline, Taylor, Aselcioglu, Cook and Shinohara2018).

Cognitive assessment

In the PEPs study, the cognitive assessment at baseline was performed in the second month after inclusion to ensure the clinical stability of patients after the FEP (Bernardo et al., Reference Bernardo, Bioque, Parellada, Saiz Ruiz, Cuesta, Llerena and Cabrera2013; Cuesta et al., Reference Cuesta, Sánchez-Torres, Cabrera, Bioque, Merchán-Naranjo, Corripio and Bernardo2015). For our hypothesis, we focused on verbal memory (VM) and WM, two cognitive domains that we had previously observed to be associated with OC exposure in schizophrenia (Amoretti, Rabelo-da-Ponte, et al., Reference Amoretti, Rabelo-da-Ponte, Garriga, Forte, Penadés, Vieta and Garcia-Rizo2022).

VM was assessed using the Verbal Learning Test España-Complutense (Benedet, Reference Benedet and Madrid1998), while WM was evaluated using the Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing subtests of the WAIS-III (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2019). These cognitive domains were derived from our previous work using principal component analysis (Amoretti, Rosa, et al., Reference Amoretti, Rosa, Mezquida, Cabrera, Ribeiro, Molina and Bernardo2022), with higher scores reflecting better performance in both domains. Further details are described in a previous work (Cuesta et al., Reference Cuesta, Sánchez-Torres, Cabrera, Bioque, Merchán-Naranjo, Corripio and Bernardo2015).

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics were calculated for each sociodemographic, neuropsychological, and clinical variable. Continuous variables are presented as mean value ± standard deviation and compared using Student’s t-tests. Categorical variables are expressed as total numbers (or percentages) and compared between groups using χ 2-tests. OCs were reported as dichotomous variables (yes/no).

Student’s t-tests were used to examine differences in LGI between FEP patients and HCs. General linear model (GLM) analyses were performed to test the relationship between each independent variable and the LGI of each brain area. Independent variables were sex, age, and chlorpromazine equivalent antipsychotic dose (Gardner, Murphy, O’Donnell, Centorrino, & Baldessarini, Reference Gardner, Murphy, O’Donnell, Centorrino and Baldessarini2010), diagnostic group (FEP/HCs), OCs (presence/absence), and the interaction between OCs and diagnostic group. We report results before and after false discovery rate (FDR) correction for multiple comparisons.

For cognitive measures, parametric mediation analyses were conducted in both groups (HCs and patients with FEP) to test whether gyrification mediates the effect of OCs on cognitive outcomes. We employed the LGI of areas where we had observed a significant interaction between diagnosis and OCs as the mediation variable between the independent variable (OCs) and cognitive outcomes, including WM and VM. The estimation of average causal mediation effects, average direct effects, total effects, and proportion-mediated effects was computed for each of the 1,000 bootstrapped resamples, and the 95% confidence interval (95% CI) was computed by determining the 2.5th and 97.5th percentiles for the resamples. The mediation analysis was performed with the mediation package version 4.5.03 (Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele, & Imai, Reference Tingley, Yamamoto, Hirose, Keele and Imai2014).

Statistical analyses were performed with Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (Version 25) and R version 4.5.0.

Results

Sociodemographic and clinical variables

The sociodemographic characteristics of the sample are described in Table 1. There were no significant between-group differences in sex or age. However, as expected, the groups differed significantly in terms of educational level (χ 2 33.32, p < 0.001), with higher levels in HCs than in patients. There was a trend toward significance in the prevalence of OCs (Lewis total score) (χ 2 = 2.91, p = 0.089) between FEP and HCs. The median duration of untreated psychosis for the FEP patients was 45 days, and the daily equivalent dose of chlorpromazine was 450 (mean value: 557.62).

Table 1. Clinical and sociodemographic characteristics of the sample

Lewis–Murray total score (any difficulty during delivery and pregnancy).

Lewis–Murray A: Complications of pregnancy; Lewis–Murray B: Abnormal fetal growth and development; Lewis–Murray C: Difficulties in delivery.

Differences in LGI between FEP and HC

We observed significant differences in LGI between FEP patients and HCs in the left cingulate (Wald’s χ 2 = 4.24, p = 0.04), left occipital (Wald’s χ 2 = 6.47, p = 0.01), left parietal (Wald’s χ 2 = 3.93, p = 0.05), right cingulate (Wald’s χ 2 = 3.77, p = 0.05), and right occipital (Wald’s χ 2 = 5.95, p = 0.01) cortices (Table 2).

Table 2. Generalized linear model gyrification index lobes by diagnosis (psychosis/control) and stratified by the presence/absence of difficulties during the intrauterine/delivery period (OC presence/absence)

FDR, false discovery rate; OCs, obstetric complications (during the intrauterine period and delivery; LGI, Local Gyrification Index.

The effect of the estimates (Wald’s χ 2 and p-value) for diagnosis and OCs refers to the outcome of the regression analysis without interaction.

The effect of estimates (Wald’s χ 2 and p-value) for diagnosis by OCs refers to the outcome of the regression analysis where the interaction was included in addition to the other independent variables. Estimated marginal means of predicted gyrification index values are adjusted by covariates, age, sex, and chlorpromazine equivalent mean dose.

*p<.05; ^p=0.05-.10

Differences in LGI between subjects with and without OCs

Within the whole sample, there were significant differences between subjects with and without OCs in the left frontal (t = −2.18, p = 0.03); left cingulate (t = −2.03, p = 0.04); and trend level differences in the right cingulate (t = −1.9, p = 0.06) cortices (data not shown). However, when we adjusted for covariates (age, sex, and chlorpromazine equivalent mean dose), none of these differences maintained statistical significance (Table 2).

Association between LGI, diagnosis, and OCs

A GLM was applied to analyze whether OC exposure (Lewis total score) interacted with diagnosis to predict differences in LGI. We observed a significant interaction between OCs and diagnosis in the left cingulate cortex (LCC) (Wald’s χ 2 = 6.65, p = 0.01), and trend level differences in the right parietal cortex (Wald’s χ 2 = 2.98, p = 0.08). However, there was no significant effect on LGI after adjusting for multiple comparisons.

In the LCC, FEP patients with OCs displayed the lowest LGI (2.28 ± 0.02), followed by FEP patients without OCs (2.30 ± 0.01), HCs without OCs (2.32 ± 0.01), and finally, HCs with OCs (2.41 ± 0.03) (Table 2 and Figure 1).

Figure 1. General linear model mean predicted estimated value for Local Gyrification Index for the left cingulate area covariated by sex, age, chlorpromazine equivalent dose, diagnostic group (FEP patients/HCs), OCs (presence/absence), and the interaction between OCs and the diagnostic group. Note: FEP, first episode psychosis; HC, healthy controls; OCs, obstetric complications.

We then separately analyzed the role of gestational (Lewis A + B) and delivery complications (Lewis C) on LGI and observed that the interaction we had found in LCC was specific to those participants who had suffered complications during gestation (Wald’s χ 2 = 5.31, p = 0.02) rather than delivery (Wald’s χ 2 = 3.231, p = 0.072).

Finally, we examined whether sex influenced this interaction effect on LGI in the LCC. In the MLG model, sex had a significant effect on LGI (Wald’s χ 2 = 14.89, p < 0.001) and the interaction of Lewis T × diagnosis × sex was significant (Wald’s χ 2 = 9.70, p = 0.021). Therefore, we stratified the sample according to sex and observed that the effect of Lewis T significantly interacted with diagnosis only in males (Wald’s χ 2 = 8.12, p = 0.004) and remained significant after FDR correction (p = 0.04) (Supplementary Table 2A). In this male subsample, FEP with OCs displayed the lowest LGI (2.33 ± 0.03), followed by FEP without OCs (2.31 ± 0.03), HCs without OCs (2.36 ± 0.02), and finally, HCs with OCs (2.50 ± 0.04). This effect was not observed in the female group (Wald’s χ 2 = 0.253; p = 0.615) (Supplementary Table 2B). However, the female sex was underrepresented, with only 17 women with OC history.

Association between gyrification, OCs, and cognition

To analyze the relationship between OCs, cognition, and gyrification, we focused on the LCC, where we observed a significant interaction between diagnosis and OCs.

We tested a mediation model, with OCs as the independent variable, cognitive functioning (independently examining VM and WM) as the dependent variable, and the LGI in the LCC as the mediator. We stratified the sample between patients and HCs, as previous studies have shown different profiles.

In HCs (Supplementary Figure S2), the total effect of OCs (Lewis T) on WM was not significant (β = −5.46, 95% CI: −13.45 to 1.86; p = 0.14), nor was the average causal mediation effect (ACME) (β = 1.68, 95% CI: −0.11 to 4.32; p = 0.10), whereas the average direct impact of OCs on WM (ADE) (β = −7.14, 95% CI: −15.16 to −0.26; p = 0.048*) was significant. On the contrary, no significant effects were found for VM: total effect (β = 10.92, 95% CI: −10.94 to 28.81; p = 0.32), ACME (β = −1.79, 95% CI: −6.20 to 2.40; p = 0.38), or ADE (β = 12.71, 95% CI: −9.26 to 30.41; p = 0.29).

In FEP patients (Supplementary Figure 3), none of the effects were significant for WM: total effect (β = −0.66, 95% CI: −7.26 to 6.54; p = 0.88), ACME (β = −0.18, 95% CI: −1.32 to 0.41; p = 0.63), and ADE (β = −0.48, 95% CI: −7.11 to 6.77; p = 0.92). Similarly, no significant effects were found for VM: total effect (β = −10.92, 95% CI: −40.16 to 16.41; p = 0.40), ACME (β = −0.27, 95% CI: −5.80 to 4.31; p = 0.92), or ADE (β = −10.65, 95% CI: −39.99 to 16.98; p = 0.41).

When mediation analyses were stratified by sex (Supplementary Figures 4 and 5), none of the effects were significant for WM in HC males: total effect (β = −2.83, 95% CI: −15.49 to 6.84; p = 0.66), ACME (β = 2.75, 95% CI: −0.28 to 8.41; p = 0.10), and ADE (β = −5.58, 95% CI: −19.25 to 4.48; p = 0.38), while in HC females, a trend was seen in the total effect (β = −7.36, 95% CI: −15.35 to 0.54; p = 0.07). However, the ACME (β = −0.28, 95% CI: −2.69 to 1.44; p = 0.75) and ADE (β = −7-08, 95% CI: −15.31 to 1.24; p = 0.11) in HC females were not significant.

In FEP patients, none of the effects were significant for WM in males: ACME (β = −0.11, 95% CI: −1.10 to 0.80; p = 0.81), ADE (β = 4.15, 95% CI: −3.34 to 12.03; p = 0.32), and total effect (β = 4.04, 95% CI: −3.47 to 11.85; p = 0.33). On the other hand, in female FEP patients, the total effect (β = −12.30, 95% CI: −21.57 to −2.15; p = 0.03) and ADE (β = −11.45, 95% CI: −19.92 to −2.15; p = 0.03) were significant, while the ACME (β = −0.85, 95% CI: −4.95 to −3.32; p = 0.62) was not significant.

Discussion

This study examined the relationship between exposure to OCs, cortical folding, and cognitive function.

We observed significant differences in LGI measures between FEP and HC participants in the left parietal, bilateral cingulate, and bilateral occipital cortices. These results are consistent with studies that reported hypogyria in patients with psychotic disorders compared to controls (Nanda et al., Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Giakoumatos, Abhishekh, Clementz and Keshavan2014; Nesvåg et al., Reference Nesvåg, Schaer, Haukvik, Westlye, Rimol, Lange and Eliez2014), also in the early stages of the illness (Palaniyappan et al., Reference Palaniyappan, Marques, Taylor, Handley, Mondelli, Bonaccorso and Dazzan2013).

In terms of exposure to OCs, regardless of diagnosis, we observed no significant gyrification differences between subjects exposed to OCs and those who were not exposed.

Results of previous studies are inconsistent, with some reporting a relationship between exposure to OCs and a reduction in gyrification (Engelhardt et al., Reference Engelhardt, Inder, Alexopoulos, Dierker, Hill, Van Essen and Neil2015; Haukvik et al., Reference Haukvik, Schaer, Nesvåg, McNeil, Hartberg, Jönsson and Agartz2012; Smith et al., Reference Smith, Thornton, Lang, MacEwan, Kopala, Su and Honer2015; Yehuda et al., Reference Yehuda, Rabinowich, Zilberman, Wexler, Haratz, Miller and Bashat2024) and others describing increased gyrification (Hedderich et al., Reference Hedderich, Bäuml, Berndt, Menegaux, Scheef, Daamen and Sorg2019; Wu, De Asis-Cruz, & Limperopoulos, Reference Wu, De Asis-Cruz and Limperopoulos2024). Such differences may be related to the heterogeneity of OCs (hypoxia, premature birth, fetal growth restriction, etc.).

Therefore, we examined whether prenatal and perinatal complications interacted differently with diagnosis to determine differences in LGI. We observed that a significant interaction with diagnosis to determine LGI differences in the LCC specifically pertained to antepartum complications. This result did not survive correction for multiple comparisons within the entire group but it maintained significance within the male subsample when we stratified our participants based on sex. In this region, male patients exposed to OCs displayed the lowest LGI, while in male HC participants, OCs acted inversely, and exposure was associated with the highest LGI. This finding may reflect how early environmental stressors affect patients and controls differently, suggesting that mere exposure to OCs may not be sufficient to explain the changes observed in psychosis, in keeping with the gene–environment interaction hypothesis. Previous research also reported that exposure to OCs can affect patients with schizophrenia differently compared to HC and specifically identified the smallest hippocampal volumes in patients with OCs (Stefanis et al., Reference Stefanis, Frangou, Yakeley, Sharma, O’Connell, Morgan and Murray1999). Similarly, Cannon et al. observed that OCs interacted with schizophrenia diagnosis to determine brain volumetric abnormalities such as ventricular enlargement (Cannon et al., Reference Cannon, van Erp, Rosso, Huttunen, Lönnqvist, Pirkola and Standertskjöld-Nordenstam2002).

Ducsay et al. (Reference Ducsay, Goyal, Pearce, Wilson, Hu and Zhang2018) suggested that hypoxia, a common mechanism of most OCs, is implicated in the disruption of neural plasticity through epigenomic modifications. These alterations in the epigenetic landscape are believed to underlie phenotypic programming mechanisms that influence long-term health outcomes and disease susceptibility (Ducsay et al., Reference Ducsay, Goyal, Pearce, Wilson, Hu and Zhang2018). Furthermore, evidence from genome-wide association studies indicates that numerous genes implicated in schizophrenia risk are not only active during fetal neurodevelopment (Hall & Bray, Reference Hall and Bray2022) but are also regulated by hypoxic conditions (Semenza, Reference Semenza2001). In our sample, we observed a specific effect of antepartum complications, which we had previously observed to be significantly associated with case–control status (Valli et al., Reference Valli, Gonzalez Segura, Verdolini, Garcia‐Rizo, Berge, Baeza and Vieta2023) and are often characterized by protracted hypoxia-associated placental pathology (Valli & McGuire, Reference Valli and McGuire2023).

Collectively, these findings support the hypothesis that exposure to OCs may induce maladaptive epigenetic changes in individuals with a genetic predisposition, thereby altering neurodevelopmental trajectories and heightening vulnerability to subsequent environmental risk factors. On the contrary, the increased LGI in the LCC that we observed in HC exposed to OCs might represent an adaptive mechanism afforded to those at low genetic risk.

Furthermore, within our sample, male participants appeared to be more susceptible to the effects of OCs, as evidenced by a more pronounced reduction in LGI of the LCC in male FEP patients compared to other groups. This finding is consistent with previous studies demonstrating sex-specific differences in brain gyrification patterns in schizophrenia. For instance, one study reported differential gyrification alterations based on sex (Mancini-Marïe et al., Reference Mancini-Marïe, Yoon, Jiminez, Fahim, Potvin, Grant and Mendrek2018), while another found that hypogyrification occurs more frequently in males with schizophrenia than in females (Vogeley et al., Reference Vogeley, Schneider-Axmann, Pfeiffer, Tepest, Bayer, Bogerts and Falkai2000). In addition, studies focusing on individuals exposed to OCs have also identified sex-dependent effects, with male patients exhibiting greater LGI reductions across several brain regions (Mareckova, Miles, Andryskova, Brazdil, & Nikolova, Reference Mareckova, Miles, Andryskova, Brazdil and Nikolova2020; Papini et al., Reference Papini, Palaniyappan, Kroll, Froudist-Walsh, Murray and Nosarti2020). Overall, males seem to be more vulnerable to intrauterine insults, as also suggested by the differential placental upregulation of genes involved in schizophrenia between males and females exposed to obstetric risk (Sutherland & Brunwasser, Reference Sutherland and Brunwasser2018; Ursini et al., Reference Ursini, Punzi, Chen, Marenco, Robinson, Porcelli and Weinberger2018).

These findings emphasize the importance of considering sex-specific neurodevelopmental trajectories in understanding the impact of OCs on cortical morphology in schizophrenia.

The left cingulate area is involved in affect, attention, memory, and executive function (Haznedar et al., Reference Haznedar, Buchsbaum, Hazlett, Shihabuddin, New and Siever2004) and is often reported to be altered in schizophrenia (Bersani et al., Reference Bersani, Minichino, Fojanesi, Gallo, Maglio, Valeriani and Fitzgerald2014; Feng et al., Reference Feng, Palaniyappan, Robbins, Cao, Fang, Luo and Luo2023). In this region, we observed that FEP with OCs displayed the lowest LGI. The neurodevelopmental model of psychosis (Murray & Lewis, Reference Murray and Lewis1987) postulates that genetic predisposition combined with environmental insults may be involved in the aberrant brain development observed in psychosis. In utero insults were identified as key disruptors of neurodevelopmental processes (Keshavan & Hogarty, Reference Keshavan and Hogarty1999). In particular, cingulate dysfunction has been suggested to disrupt the modulation of prefrontal–temporal integration (Dazzan et al., Reference Dazzan, Lawrence, Reinders, Egerton, van Haren, Merritt and McGuire2021; Nanda et al., Reference Nanda, Tandon, Mathew, Giakoumatos, Abhishekh, Clementz and Keshavan2014) with an effect on cognitive functions such as memory and executive function (Fletcher, McKenna, Friston, Frith, & Dolan, Reference Fletcher, McKenna, Friston, Frith and Dolan1999; L. Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Pu, Wang, Liu, Wu, Liu and Liu2016), and a relationship to poorer WM in individuals with FEP (Feng et al., Reference Feng, Palaniyappan, Robbins, Cao, Fang, Luo and Luo2023; L. Zhou et al., Reference Zhou, Pu, Wang, Liu, Wu, Liu and Liu2016).

In our sample, FEP patients performed significantly worse than HC participants in both the cognitive domains examined (WM and VM). This is consistent with previous studies reporting these deficits from the early phases of the disorder (Catalan et al., Reference Catalan, McCutcheon, Aymerich, Pedruzo, Radua, Rodríguez and Fusar-Poli2024; Rodriguez-Jimenez et al., Reference Rodriguez-Jimenez, Santos, Dompablo, Santabárbara, Aparicio, Olmos and García-Fernández2019).

Finally, we examined whether the LGI of the LCC mediated the relationship between OCs and cognitive functioning. In our FEP sample, we did not observe a significant mediation effect for either WM or VM. Meanwhile, a significant direct effect of OCs on WM was observed in HCs. Similarly, the mediation effect of the LCC LGI showed a positive indirect effect on cognitive performance. Although it could be considered a trend toward significance, there is no strong evidence that LCC LGI explains the relationship between OCs and WM. A possible explanation is that cognitive performance in FEP patients depends on a more complex interaction between genetic and environmental risk factors, with several environmental exposures involved in shaping the relationship between OCs and cognition. For instance, a family history of psychosis confers a higher risk for developing psychosis than OCs (Davies et al., Reference Davies, Segre, Estradé, Radua, De Micheli, Provenzani and Fusar-Poli2020) and can contribute to brain changes observed in patients with psychosis or even high-risk populations (Neilson et al., Reference Neilson, Bois, Clarke, Hall, Johnstone, Owens and Lawrie2018). In addition, other environmental risk factors, such as childhood maltreatment (Sideli et al., Reference Sideli, Schimmenti, La Barbera, La Cascia, Ferraro, Aas and van der Ven2022) and cannabis use (Yucel et al., Reference Yucel, Bora, Lubman, Solowij, Brewer, Cotton and Pantelis2012), are related to cognitive impairment in FEP patients and may attenuate the isolated effect of OC exposure on cognition.

When stratifying our sample by sex, we observed no significant effect of OCs in relation to cognition in males, while in females, a direct effect of OCs on WM was observed in FEP patients. These differential effects of OCs on cognition by sex were unexpected, especially considering that previous evidence suggests that the male fetus may be more vulnerable to the effects of OCs than the female fetus, and that males who go on to develop psychosis may possess fewer compensatory resources compared to other groups (DiPietro & Voegtline, Reference DiPietro and Voegtline2017; Sutherland & Brunwasser, Reference Sutherland and Brunwasser2018). One possible explanation for this unexpected pattern lies in the underrepresentation of women with a history of OCs within our sample. This limitation increases the likelihood of unstable parameter estimates, inflated effect sizes, and spurious significance, especially in stratified models. Thus, this effect should be interpreted with caution, and future studies with larger and sex-balanced samples are necessary to clarify the role of sex in moderating the relationship between early adversity and cognition in psychosis.

This research should be interpreted in the context of several other limitations. First, OCs have a small effect size to explain psychosis, so larger sample sizes are needed to increase the statistical power and detect significant effects (De Prisco & Vieta, Reference De Prisco and Vieta2024). In addition, this was a cross-sectional study conducted at the onset of psychosis (despite patients being minimally treated, they were not antipsychotic-naïve), so we were not able to control other factors, including treatment adherence (Vieta & De Prisco, Reference Vieta and De Prisco2024). Longitudinal studies of the population could track the long-term impact of OCs on brain and cognitive development, enabling measures to prevent and reduce the risk of developing psychosis and cognitive impairment. In our mediation analysis, we did not include potential confounders, such as educational level or chlorpromazine equivalents (which might be associated with both LGI and cognitive functioning) (Ilzarbe & Vieta, Reference Ilzarbe and Vieta2023) to reduce the complexity and avoid multicollinearity in the specific case of educational level.

Several strengths counterbalance the aforementioned limitations, such as a relatively large sample size and the lack of confounders associated with the protracted duration of illness and treatment exposure. To the best of the author’s knowledge, this is the first study to examine the potential impact of OCs on brain gyrification processes and how they can affect cognitive performance in FEP.

In summary, we observed that FEP patients differ from HCs in terms of gyrification in the parietal, occipital, and cingulate cortices. Our findings suggest that OCs are associated with altered cortical gyrification, particularly in the LCC, with a differential effect of OC exposure between patients and HCs, especially evident in males and driven by antepartum complications.

Among the HCs, OCs were negatively associated with WM. This pattern potentially reflects compensatory neurodevelopmental mechanisms in response to early adversity, which is also supported by the differential effects of OCs on the brain. In contrast, neither direct nor mediated effects of OCs on cognition were observed among individuals with FEP, suggesting a more complex interplay between early-life adversity, cortical neurodevelopment, and cognition in psychosis. Sex differences may modulate this complex interplay, and the underrepresentation of female participants in our sample limits the strength of our conclusions. Future studies with larger, sex-balanced samples are essential to clarify the role of sex in shaping the neurodevelopmental impact of OCs in psychosis.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725100974.

PEPs Group

Nora Guasch-Capella5,6, Anaid Perez-Ramos5,6,7,19, Jessica Merchán-Naranjo6,9, Laura Pina-Camacho6,9, Alexandra Roldán6,20, Anna Alonso-Solís6,20, Ana González-Pinto6,21, Iñaki Zorrilla6,21, Pedro M. Ruiz-Lázaro6,22, David Vaquero-Puyuelo6,22, Maria José Escarti6,23,24,25, Marta Perez-Rando6,24,26, Anna Mané6,12,13,14, Angeles Malagon6,12,13,14, Maria Serra-Navarro7,8, Eduard Vieta2,6,7,8, Josefina Castro-Fornieles2,6,7,15, Inmaculada Baeza2,6,7,15, Francesco Dal Santo6,27, Paz García-Portilla6,27,28, Rafael Segarra Echevarria6,29,30, Arantzazu Zabala Rabadán6,29,30, Roberto Rodriguez-Jimenez6,31,32, Isabel Martínez-Gras31,32,33, Judith Usall34, Anna Butjosa6,34, Edith Pomarol-Clotet6,17, Raymond Salvador6,17, Angela Ibañez6,35, Manuel J. Cuesta10,11, Vicent Balanzá-Martínez36

19Neuropsychopharmacology and Psychobiology Research Group, Department of Neuroscience, University of Cadiz, Cadiz, Spain

20Department of Psychiatry, Institut d’Investigació Biomèdica Sant Pau, Hospital de la Santa Creu i Sant Pau, Barcelona, Spain

21Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario de Alava, UPV/EHU, BIOARABA, Vitoria, Spain

22Department of Medicine and Psychiatry, Universidad de Zaragoza, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Aragón (IIS Aragón), Zaragoza, Spain

23Servicio de Psiquiatria, Hospital Clínico Universitario de Valencia, Valencia, Spain

24Fundación Investigación Hospital Clínico de Valencia, INCLIVA, Valencia, Spain

25Department of Medicine, University CEU-UCH, Valencia, Spain

26Neurobiology Unit, Institute for Biotechnology and Biomedicine (BIOTECMED), Universitat de València, Valencia, Spain

27Servicio de Salud del Principado de Asturias (SESPA), Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria del Prinicipado de Aturias (ISPA), Oviedo, Spain

28Área de Psiquiatría, Departamento de Medicina, Universidad de Oviedo, Oviedo, Spain

29Área de Psiquiatría, Departamento de Neurociencias, Universidad del País Vasco UPV/EHU, Bilbao, Spain

30Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria BioCruces Bizkaia, Barakaldo, Spain

31Faculty of Medicine, Complutense University of Madrid (UCM), Madrid, Spain

32Department of Psychiatry, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Hospital 12 de Octubre (imas12), Madrid, Spain

33RETIC (Network of Addictive Conditions), Institute of Health Carlos III, Madrid, Spain

34Institut de Recerca Sant Joan de Déu, Department of Child and Adolescent Mental Health, Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Sant Boi de Llobregat, Spain

35Department of Psychiatry, Hospital Universitario Ramon y Cajal, Universidad de Alcala, Instituto Ramon y Cajal de InvestigacionSanitaria (IRYCIS), Madrid, Spain

36Teaching Unit of Psychiatry and Psychological Medicine, Department of Medicine, University of Valencia, CIBERSAM, INCLIVA, Valencia, Spain

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants for their contribution.

Funding statement

This study is part of a coordinated multicenter Project, PEPs study, funded by the Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad (PI08/0208; PI11/00325; PI14/00612), Instituto de Salud Carlos III – Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional. Unión Europea. Una manera de hacer Europa, Centro de Investigacion Biomédica en Red de salud Mental, CIBERSAM, Instituto de Salud Carlos III, by the CERCA Program/Generalitat de Catalunya and Secretaria d’Universitats i Recerca of the Departament d’Economia I Coneixement (2017SGR1355). Departament de Salut de la Generalitat de Catalunya (2017), funds from the Pla Estratègic de Recerca i Innovacio en Salut (PERIS 2016–2020), Projectes de recerca orientats al’atencio primària, SLT006/17/00345. This study has been funded by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII) through the projects ‘PI20/00661 and PI24/00196’ and co-funded by the European Union. CGR received funding from the Maria i Núria Cunillera legacy (FCRB_CU1_2024). NV and IV were supported by a BITRECS fellowship that received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the grant agreement No. 754 550 and from ‘La Caixa’ Foundation under the agreement LCF/PR/GN18/5031000. IB received funding from the Pons-Bartran legacy (FCRB_IPB2_2023), as well as the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI21/00391, co-funded by the European Union, ‘One way to make Europe’). RRJ is supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Fondo de Investigación Sanitaria (FIS), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Madrid Regional Government (S2010/ BMD-2422 AGES; S2017/BMD-3740; S2022/BMD-7216 AGES 3-CM), the Madrid Regional Government (R&D activities in Biomedicine S2022/BMD-7216 (AGES 3-CM), and CIBERSAM-ISCIII. AI was supported by CIBER – Consorcio Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red – (CB/07/09/0025), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación; by the Madrid Regional Government (S2022/BMD-7216 (AGES 3-CM) and European Union Structural Funds; and by grants PI22/01183 and ICI21/00089, integrated into the Plan Nacional de I + D + I and co-financed by the ISCIII-Subdirección General de Evaluación and the Fondo Europeo de Desarrollo Regional (FEDER).

Competing interests

NV has received financial support for CME activities and travel funds from the following entities: Angelini, Janssen-Cilag, Lundbeck, and Otsuka. CGR has received grants from/or served as a consultant, advisor, or speaker for the following entities: Adamed, Angelini, Casen-Recordati, Janssen-Cilag, Lunbeck, and Newron. IB has received honoraria or travel support from Otsuka-Lundbeck and Angelini. EV has received grants and served as a consultant, advisor, or CME speaker for the following entities: AB-Biotics, AbbVie, Adamed, Alcediag, Angelini, Biogen, Beckley-Psytech, Biohaven, Boehringer-Ingelheim, Celon Pharma, Compass, Dainippon Sumitomo Pharma, Esteve, Ethypharm, Ferrer, Gedeon Richter, GH Research, Glaxo-Smith Kline, HMNC, Idorsia, Johnson & Johnson, Lundbeck, Luye Pharma, Medincell, Merck, Newron, Novartis, Orion Corporation, Organon, Otsuka, Roche, Rovi, Sage, Sanofi-Aventis, Sunovion, Takeda, Teva, and Viatris, outside the submitted work. RRJ has been a consultant for, spoken in activities of, or received grants from JanssenCilag, Lundbeck, Otsuka, Pfizer, Ferrer, Juste, Takeda, Exeltis, Casen-Recordati, Angelini, and Rovi. AI has received research support from or served as a speaker or advisor for JanssenCilag, Lundbeck, Otsuka Pharmaceutical, Alter, Rovi, Casen Recordati, and Viatris.

All authors report no financial relationship relevant to the subject of this article.

Footnotes

*

Both to be considered last authors.

Contributors of PEPs Group are listed at the backmatter section of this article.

References

Amoretti, S., Rabelo-da-Ponte, F. D., Garriga, M., Forte, M. F., Penadés, R., Vieta, E., … Garcia-Rizo, C. (2022). Obstetric complications and cognition in schizophrenia: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Medicine, 52(14), 28742884. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291722002409CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Amoretti, S., Rosa, A. R., Mezquida, G., Cabrera, B., Ribeiro, M., Molina, M., … Bernardo, M. (2022). The impact of cognitive reserve, cognition and clinical symptoms on psychosocial functioning in first-episode psychoses. Psychological Medicine, 52(3), 526537. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291720002226CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benedet, A. (1998). TAVEC: test de aprendizaje verbal España-Complutense (Madrid, Tea, Ed.).Google Scholar
Bernardo, M., Bioque, M., Parellada, M., Saiz Ruiz, J., Cuesta, M. J., Llerena, A., … Cabrera, B. (2013). Criterios de valoración clínicos y de funcionamiento en un estudio de interacción gen-ambiente en primeros episodios psicóticos (PEPs). Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, 6(1), 416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsm.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bersani, F. S., Minichino, A., Fojanesi, M., Gallo, M., Maglio, G., Valeriani, G., … Fitzgerald, P. B. (2014). Cingulate cortex in schizophrenia: Its relation with negative symptoms and psychotic onset. A review study. European Review for Medical and Pharmacological Sciences, 18(22), 33543367.Google ScholarPubMed
Cannon, M., Jones, P. B., & Murray, R. M. (2002). Obstetric complications and schizophrenia: Historical and meta-analytic review. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(7), 10801092. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.159.7.1080CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cannon, T. D., van Erp, T. G. M., Rosso, I. M., Huttunen, M., Lönnqvist, J., Pirkola, T., … Standertskjöld-Nordenstam, C.-G. (2002). Fetal hypoxia and structural brain abnormalities in schizophrenic patients, their siblings, and controls. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(1), 35. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.59.1.35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catalan, A., McCutcheon, R. A., Aymerich, C., Pedruzo, B., Radua, J., Rodríguez, V., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2024). The magnitude and variability of neurocognitive performance in first-episode psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Translational Psychiatry, 14(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-023-02718-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Costas-Carrera, A., Garcia-Rizo, C., Bitanihirwe, B., & Penadés, R. (2020). Obstetric complications and brain imaging in schizophrenia: A systematic review. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(12), 10771084. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.07.018Google ScholarPubMed
Costas-Carrera, A., Verdolini, N., Garcia-Rizo, C., Mezquida, G., Janssen, J., Valli, I., … Bernardo, M. (2024). Difficulties during delivery, brain ventricle enlargement and cognitive impairment in first episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 54(7), 13391349. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291723003185CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuesta, M. J., Sánchez-Torres, A. M., Cabrera, B., Bioque, M., Merchán-Naranjo, J., Corripio, I., … Bernardo, M. (2015). Premorbid adjustment and clinical correlates of cognitive impairment in first-episode psychosis. The PEPsCog study. Schizophrenia Research, 164(1–3), 6573. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2015.02.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, C., Segre, G., Estradé, A., Radua, J., De Micheli, A., Provenzani, U., … Fusar-Poli, P. (2020). Prenatal and perinatal risk and protective factors for psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. The Lancet Psychiatry, 7(5), 399410. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30057-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dazzan, P., Lawrence, A. J., Reinders, A. A. T. S., Egerton, A., van Haren, N. E. M., Merritt, K., … McGuire, P. (2021). Symptom remission and brain cortical networks at first clinical presentation of psychosis: The OPTiMiSE study. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 47(2), 444455. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Peri, L., Crescini, A., Deste, G., Fusar-Poli, P., Sacchetti, E., & Vita, A. (2012). Brain structural abnormalities at the onset of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: A meta-analysis of controlled magnetic resonance imaging studies. Current Pharmaceutical Design, 18(4), 486494. https://doi.org/10.2174/138161212799316253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Prisco, M., & Vieta, E. (2024). The never-ending problem: Sample size matters. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 79, 1718. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2023.10.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DiPietro, J. A., & Voegtline, K. M. (2017). The gestational foundation of sex differences in development and vulnerability. Neuroscience, 342, 420. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.068CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ducsay, C. A., Goyal, R., Pearce, W. J., Wilson, S., Hu, X.-Q., & Zhang, L. (2018). Gestational hypoxia and developmental plasticity. Physiological Reviews, 98(3), 12411334. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00043.2017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellison-Wright, I., Glahn, D. C., Laird, A. R., Thelen, S. M., & Bullmore, E. (2008). The anatomy of first-episode and chronic schizophrenia: An anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165(8), 10151023. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07101562CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engelhardt, E., Inder, T. E., Alexopoulos, D., Dierker, D. L., Hill, J., Van Essen, D., & Neil, J. J. (2015). Regional impairments of cortical folding in premature infants. Annals of Neurology, 77(1), 154162. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.24313CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Falkai, P., Honer, W. G., Kamer, T., Dustert, S., Vogeley, K., Schneider-Axmann, T., … Tepest, R. (2007). Disturbed frontal gyrification within families affected with schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41(10), 805813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.07.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feng, N., Palaniyappan, L., Robbins, T. W., Cao, L., Fang, S., Luo, X., … Luo, Q. (2023). Working memory processing deficit associated with a nonlinear response pattern of the anterior cingulate cortex in first-episode and drug-naïve schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 48(3), 552559. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-022-01499-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández, V., & Borrell, V. (2023). Developmental mechanisms of gyrification. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 80, 102711. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2023.102711CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher, P., McKenna, P. J., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Abnormal cingulate modulation of fronto-temporal connectivity in schizophrenia. NeuroImage, 9(3), 337342. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fortin, J.-P., Cullen, N., Sheline, Y. I., Taylor, W. D., Aselcioglu, I., Cook, P. A., … Shinohara, R. T. (2018). Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. NeuroImage, 167, 104120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.024CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gardner, D. M., Murphy, A. L., O’Donnell, H., Centorrino, F., & Baldessarini, R. J. (2010). International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(6), 686693. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gautam, P., Anstey, K. J., Wen, W., Sachdev, P. S., & Cherbuin, N. (2015). Cortical gyrification and its relationships with cortical volume, cortical thickness, and cognitive performance in healthy mid-life adults. Behavioural Brain Research, 287, 331339. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.03.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, S., Blackmon, K., Thesen, T., DuBois, J., Wang, X., Halgren, E., & Devinsky, O. (2018). Parieto-frontal gyrification and working memory in healthy adults. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 12(2), 303308. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-017-9696-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hall, J., & Bray, N. J. (2022). Schizophrenia genomics: Convergence on synaptic development, adult synaptic plasticity, or both? Biological Psychiatry, 91(8), 709717. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.10.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, J. M., Yates, S., Miller, P., Best, J. J. K., Johnstone, E. C., & Lawrie, S. M. (2004). Gyrification in first-episode schizophrenia: A morphometric study. Biological Psychiatry, 55(2), 141147. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(03)00789-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haukvik, U. K., Schaer, M., Nesvåg, R., McNeil, T., Hartberg, C. B., Jönsson, E. G., … Agartz, I. (2012). Cortical folding in Broca’s area relates to obstetric complications in schizophrenia patients and healthy controls. Psychological Medicine, 42(6), 13291337. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291711002315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haznedar, M. M., Buchsbaum, M. S., Hazlett, E. A., Shihabuddin, L., New, A., & Siever, L. J. (2004). Cingulate gyrus volume and metabolism in the schizophrenia spectrum. Schizophrenia Research, 71(2–3), 249262. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2004.02.025CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hedderich, D. M., Bäuml, J. G., Berndt, M. T., Menegaux, A., Scheef, L., Daamen, M., … Sorg, C. (2019). Aberrant gyrification contributes to the link between gestational age and adult IQ after premature birth. Brain, 142(5), 12551269. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz071CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ilzarbe, L., & Vieta, E. (2023). The elephant in the room: Medication as confounder. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 71, 68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2023.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keshavan, M. S., & Hogarty, G. E. (1999). Brain maturational processes and delayed onset in schizophrenia. Development and Psychopathology, 11(3), 525543. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579499002199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, S. W., & Murray, R. M. (1987). Obstetric complications, neurodevelopmental deviance, and risk of schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 21(4), 413421. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(87)90088-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madan, C. R., & Kensinger, E. A. (2017). Test–retest reliability of brain morphology estimates. Brain Informatics, 4(2), 107121. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40708-016-0060-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Madeira, N., Duarte, J. V., Martins, R., Costa, G. N., Macedo, A., & Castelo-Branco, M. (2020). Morphometry and gyrification in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: A comparative MRI study. NeuroImage: Clinical, 26, 102220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102220CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mancini-Marïe, A., Yoon, U., Jiminez, J., Fahim, C., Potvin, S., Grant, J. A., … Mendrek, A. (2018). Sex, age, symptoms and illness duration and their relation with gyrification index in schizophrenia. Clinical Schizophrenia & Related Psychoses, 12(2), 5768. https://doi.org/10.3371/CSRP.MAYO.070415CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mareckova, K., Miles, A., Andryskova, L., Brazdil, M., & Nikolova, Y. S. (2020). Temporally and sex‐specific effects of maternal perinatal stress on offspring cortical gyrification and mood in young adulthood. Human Brain Mapping, 41(17), 48664875. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25163CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsuda, Y., & Ohi, K. (2018). Cortical gyrification in schizophrenia: Current perspectives. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 14, 18611869. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S145273CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeil, T. F., Cantor-Graae, E., & Weinberger, D. R. (2000). Relationship of obstetric complications and differences in size of brain structures in monozygotic twin pairs discordant for schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(2), 203212. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendrek, A., & Mancini-Marïe, A. (2016, August 1). Sex/gender differences in the brain and cognition in schizophrenia. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 67, 5778. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.10.013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mezquida, G., Fernandez-Egea, E., Treen, D., Mané, A., Bergé, D., Savulich, G., … Garcia-Rizo, C. (2018). Obstetric phenotypes in the heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease, 206(11), 882886. https://doi.org/10.1097/NMD.0000000000000897CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murray, R. M., & Lewis, S. W. (1987). Is schizophrenia a neurodevelopmental disorder? BMJ, 295(6600), 681682. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.295.6600.681CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nanda, P., Tandon, N., Mathew, I. T., Giakoumatos, C. I., Abhishekh, H. A., Clementz, B. A., … Keshavan, M. S. (2014). Local gyrification index in probands with psychotic disorders and their first-degree relatives. Biological Psychiatry, 76(6), 447455. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.11.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narr, K. L., Bilder, R. M., Kim, S., Thompson, P. M., Szeszko, P., Robinson, D., … Toga, A. W. (2004). Abnormal gyral complexity in first-episode schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 55(8), 859867. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.027CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neilson, E., Bois, C., Clarke, T.-K., Hall, L., Johnstone, E. C., Owens, D. G. C., … Lawrie, S. M. (2018). Polygenic risk for schizophrenia, transition and cortical gyrification: A high-risk study. Psychological Medicine, 48(9), 15321539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291717003087CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nelson, E. A., Kraguljac, N. V., White, D. M., Jindal, R. D., Shin, A. L., & Lahti, A. C. (2020). A prospective longitudinal investigation of cortical thickness and gyrification in schizophrenia. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 65(6), 381391. https://doi.org/10.1177/0706743720904598CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nesvåg, R., Schaer, M., Haukvik, U. K., Westlye, L. T., Rimol, L. M., Lange, E. H., … Eliez, S. (2014). Reduced brain cortical folding in schizophrenia revealed in two independent samples. Schizophrenia Research, 152(2–3), 333338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.11.032CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palaniyappan, L., & Liddle, P. (2012). Aberrant cortical gyrification in schizophrenia: A surface-based morphometry study. Journal of Psychiatry & Neuroscience, 37(6), 399406. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.110119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palaniyappan, L., Mallikarjun, P., Joseph, V., White, T. P., & Liddle, P. F. (2011). Folding of the prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia: Regional differences in gyrification. Biological Psychiatry, 69(10), 974979. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palaniyappan, L., Marques, T. R., Taylor, H., Handley, R., Mondelli, V., Bonaccorso, S., … Dazzan, P. (2013). Cortical folding defects as markers of poor treatment response in first-episode psychosis. JAMA Psychiatry, 70(10), 1031. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papini, C., Palaniyappan, L., Kroll, J., Froudist-Walsh, S., Murray, R. M., & Nosarti, C. (2020). Altered cortical gyrification in adults who were born very preterm and its associations with cognition and mental health. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 5(7), 640650. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.01.006Google ScholarPubMed
Pina-Camacho, L., Del Rey-Mejías, Á., Janssen, J., Bioque, M., González-Pinto, A., Arango, C., … Parellada, M. (2016). Age at first episode modulates diagnosis-related structural brain abnormalities in psychosis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(2), 344357. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbv128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rodriguez-Jimenez, R., Santos, J. L., Dompablo, M., Santabárbara, J., Aparicio, A. I., Olmos, R., … García-Fernández, L. (2019). MCCB cognitive profile in Spanish first episode schizophrenia patients. Schizophrenia Research, 211, 8892. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2019.07.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rychagov, N., del Re, E. C., Zeng, V., Oykhman, E., Lizano, P., McDowell, J., … Keshavan, M. S. (2024). Gyrification across psychotic disorders: A bipolar-schizophrenia network of intermediate phenotypes study. Schizophrenia Research, 271, 169178. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2024.07.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salagre, E., Arango, C., Artigas, F., Ayuso-Mateos, J. L., Bernardo, M., Castro-Fornieles, J., … Vieta, E. (2019). Diez años de investigación traslacional colaborativa en enfermedades mentales: el CIBERSAM. Revista de Psiquiatría y Salud Mental, 12(1), 18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rpsm.2018.10.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasabayashi, D., Takahashi, T., Takayanagi, Y., & Suzuki, M. (2021). Anomalous brain gyrification patterns in major psychiatric disorders: A systematic review and transdiagnostic integration. Translational Psychiatry, 11(1), 176. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-021-01297-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasabayashi, D., Takayanagi, Y., Takahashi, T., Koike, S., Yamasue, H., Katagiri, N., … Suzuki, M. (2017). Increased occipital gyrification and development of psychotic disorders in individuals with an at-risk mental state: A multicenter study. Biological Psychiatry, 82(10), 737745. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmitt, S., Ringwald, K. G., Meller, T., Stein, F., Brosch, K., Pfarr, J.-K., … Kircher, T. (2023). Associations of gestational age with gyrification and neurocognition in healthy adults. European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 273(2), 467479. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00406-022-01454-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schulze, K., McDonald, C., Frangou, S., Sham, P., Grech, A., Toulopoulou, T., … Murray, R. M. (2003). Hippocampal volume in familial and nonfamilial schizophrenic probands and their unaffected relatives. Biological Psychiatry, 53(7), 562570. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01910-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Semenza, G. L. (2001). Hypoxia-inducible factor 1: Control of oxygen homeostasis in health and disease. Pediatric Research, 49(5), 614617. https://doi.org/10.1203/00006450-200105000-00002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Si, S., Bi, A., Yu, Z., See, C., Kelly, S., Ambrogi, S., … Kempton, M. J. (2024). Mapping gray and white matter volume abnormalities in early-onset psychosis: An ENIGMA multicenter voxel-based morphometry study. Molecular Psychiatry, 29(2), 496504. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-023-02343-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sideli, L., Schimmenti, A., La Barbera, D., La Cascia, C., Ferraro, L., Aas, M., … van der Ven, E. (2022). Childhood maltreatment, educational attainment, and IQ: Findings from a multicentric case-control study of first-episode psychosis (EU-GEI). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(3), 575589. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, G. N., Thornton, A. E., Lang, D. J., MacEwan, G. W., Kopala, L. C., Su, W., & Honer, W. G. (2015). Cortical morphology and early adverse birth events in men with first-episode psychosis. Psychological Medicine, 45(9), 18251837. https://doi.org/10.1017/S003329171400292XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spalthoff, R., Gaser, C., & Nenadić, I. (2018). Altered gyrification in schizophrenia and its relation to other morphometric markers. Schizophrenia Research, 202, 195202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2018.07.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stefanis, N., Frangou, S., Yakeley, J., Sharma, T., O’Connell, P., Morgan, K., … Murray, R. (1999). Hippocampal volume reduction in schizophrenia: Effects of genetic risk and pregnancy and birth complications. Biological Psychiatry, 46(5), 697702. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00089-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutherland, S., & Brunwasser, S. M. (2018). Sex differences in vulnerability to prenatal stress: A review of the recent literature. Current Psychiatry Reports, 20(11), 102. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-018-0961-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tingley, D., Yamamoto, T., Hirose, K., Keele, L., & Imai, K. (2014). Mediation: Package for causal mediation analysis. Journal of Statistical Software, 59(5). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v059.i05CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ursini, G., Punzi, G., Chen, Q., Marenco, S., Robinson, J. F., Porcelli, A., … Weinberger, D. R. (2018). Convergence of placenta biology and genetic risk for schizophrenia. Nature Medicine, 24(6), 792801. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41591-018-0021-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valli, I., Gonzalez Segura, A., Verdolini, N., Garcia‐Rizo, C., Berge, D., Baeza, I., … Vieta, E. (2023). Obstetric complications and genetic risk for schizophrenia: Differential role of antenatal and perinatal events in first episode psychosis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 148(1), 8190. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.13546CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valli, I., & McGuire, P. (2023). Genetic and obstetric risk in psychosis: Towards a hypothesis-based probe of different noxae. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 72, 68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2023.03.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vieta, E., & De Prisco, M. (2024). Cross-sectional studies: Is pressing the pause button worth it in research? European Neuropsychopharmacology, 85, 3233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.euroneuro.2024.06.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vogeley, K., Schneider-Axmann, T., Pfeiffer, U., Tepest, R., Bayer, T. A., Bogerts, B., … Falkai, P. (2000). Disturbed gyrification of the prefrontal region in male schizophrenic patients: A morphometric postmortem study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 157(1), 3439. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.157.1.34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2019, February 11). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale--Third Edition. PsycTESTS Dataset. https://doi.org/10.1037/t49755-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeler, D. G., & Harper, C. G. (2007). Localised reductions in gyrification in the posterior cingulate: Schizophrenia and controls. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 31(2), 319327. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.09.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, T., & Gottesman, I. (2012). Brain connectivity and gyrification as endophenotypes for schizophrenia: Weight of the evidence. Current Topics in Medicinal Chemistry, 12(21), 23932403. https://doi.org/10.2174/156802612805289953CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, T., Su, S., Schmidt, M., Kao, C.-Y., & Sapiro, G. (2010). The development of gyrification in childhood and adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 3645. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.10.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wu, Y., De Asis-Cruz, J., & Limperopoulos, C. (2024). Brain structural and functional outcomes in the offspring of women experiencing psychological distress during pregnancy. Molecular Psychiatry, 29(7), 22232240. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-024-02449-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yehuda, B., Rabinowich, A., Zilberman, A., Wexler, Y., Haratz, K. K., Miller, E., … Bashat, D. Ben. (2024). Reduced gyrification in fetal growth restriction with prenatal magnetic resonance images. Cerebral Cortex, 34(6). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhae250CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yu, P., Chen, Y., Ge, C., & Wang, H. (2021). Sexual dimorphism in placental development and its contribution to health and diseases. Critical Reviews in Toxicology, 51, 555570. https://doi.org/10.1080/10408444.2021.1977237CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yucel, M., Bora, E., Lubman, D. I., Solowij, N., Brewer, W. J., Cotton, S. M., … Pantelis, C. (2012). The impact of cannabis use on cognitive functioning in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-analysis of existing findings and new data in a first-episode sample. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(2), 316330. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbq079CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zhou, H., Wang, D., Wang, J., Xu, H., Cao, B., & Zhang, X. (2021). Association of altered cortical gyrification and psychopathological symptoms in patients with first-episode drug-naïve schizophrenia. Asian Journal of Psychiatry, 64, 102749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajp.2021.102749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhou, L., Pu, W., Wang, J., Liu, H., Wu, G., Liu, C., … Liu, Z. (2016). Inefficient DMN suppression in schizophrenia patients with impaired cognitive function but not patients with preserved cognitive function. Scientific Reports, 6(1), 21657. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep21657CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zilles, K., Palomero-Gallagher, N., & Amunts, K. (2013). Development of cortical folding during evolution and ontogeny. Trends in Neurosciences, 36(5), 275284. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2013.01.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Clinical and sociodemographic characteristics of the sample

Figure 1

Table 2. Generalized linear model gyrification index lobes by diagnosis (psychosis/control) and stratified by the presence/absence of difficulties during the intrauterine/delivery period (OC presence/absence)

Figure 2

Figure 1. General linear model mean predicted estimated value for Local Gyrification Index for the left cingulate area covariated by sex, age, chlorpromazine equivalent dose, diagnostic group (FEP patients/HCs), OCs (presence/absence), and the interaction between OCs and the diagnostic group. Note: FEP, first episode psychosis; HC, healthy controls; OCs, obstetric complications.

Supplementary material: File

Costas-Carrera et al. supplementary material

Costas-Carrera et al. supplementary material
Download Costas-Carrera et al. supplementary material(File)
File 3.3 MB