Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-5jtmz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-29T07:45:37.033Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Human development, inequality, and their associations with brain structure across 29 countries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 July 2025

Vicente Medel
Affiliation:
Latin American Brain Health Institute (BrainLat), https://ror.org/0326knt82Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile
Luz M. Alliende
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/000e0be47Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA
Richard Bethlehem
Affiliation:
Autism Research Centre, Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/013meh722University of Cambridge, Cambridge UK Department of Psychology, https://ror.org/013meh722University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK
Jakob Seidlitz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/00b30xv10University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, Philadelphia, PA, USA Penn-Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia Lifespan Brain Institute, https://ror.org/00b30xv10University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Grace Ringlein
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, https://ror.org/00za53h95Johns Hopkins Bloomberg School of Public Health, Baltimore, MD USA
Celso Arango
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Gregorio Marañón (IISGM), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
Aurina Arnatkevičiūtė
Affiliation:
The Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia Monash Biomedical Imaging, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Laila Asmal
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, https://ror.org/05bk57929 Stellenbosch University , Cape Town, South Africa
Mark Bellgrove
Affiliation:
The Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Vivek Benegal
Affiliation:
Centre for Addiction Medicine, https://ror.org/0405n5e57National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences (NIMHANS), Bengaluru, Karnataka, India
Miquel Bernardo
Affiliation:
Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Departament de Medicina, Institut de Neurociències (UBNeuro), https://ror.org/02a2kzf50Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques, August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII), Barcelona, Spain
Pablo Billeke
Affiliation:
Laboratorio de Neurociencia Social y Neuromodulación, Centro de Investigación en Complejidad Social (neuroCICS), Facultad de Gobierno, https://ror.org/05y33vv83Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile
Jorge Bosch-Bayard
Affiliation:
McGill Centre for Integrative Neuroscience, Ludmer Centre for Neuroinformatics and Mental Health, Montreal Neurological Institute, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Applied Neurocognitive Psychology Lab. Carl von Ossietzky Universitaet Oldenburg, Oldenburg, Germany
Rodrigo Bressan
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Laboratory in Clinical Neuroscience (LiNC), Department of Psychiatry, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Geraldo Busatto
Affiliation:
Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Mariana Castro
Affiliation:
Grupo de Investigación en Neurociencias Aplicadas a las Alteraciones de la Conducta (INAAC), Fleni-CONICET Neurosciences Institute (INEU), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, School of Medicine, University of Buenos Aires, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Tiffany Chaim-Avancini
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Psychiatric Neuroimaging (LIM-21), Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de Sao Paulo (HCFMUSP), Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Monise Costanzi
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular Psychiatry, Centro de Pesquisa Clínica, https://ror.org/010we4y38Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil
Leticia Czepielewski
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular Psychiatry, Centro de Pesquisa Clínica, https://ror.org/010we4y38Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil Programa de Pós-Graduação em Psicologia, Instituto Psicologia, https://ror.org/041yk2d64Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Porto Alegre, Brazil
Paola Dazzan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London, UK
Camilo de la Fuente-Sandoval
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychiatry, Direction of Research, https://ror.org/01c5r7j06Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía, Mexico City, Mexico
Covadonga M. Diaz-Caneja
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Gregorio Marañón (IISGM), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
Marta Di Forti
Affiliation:
Social, Genetic and Developmental Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London SE5 8AF, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Maudsley Biomedical Research Centre, South London and Maudsley National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College London, London SE5 8AZ, UK
Ana Maria Diaz-Zuluaga
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, https://ror.org/03bp5hc83University of Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia Center for Neurobehavioral Genetics, Jane and Terry Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior Los Angeles, https://ror.org/046rm7j60University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, USA
Stefan Du Plessis
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, https://ror.org/05bk57929 Stellenbosch University , Cape Town, South Africa South African Medical Research Council (SAMRC) Genomics of Brain Disorders Unit, Cape Town, South Africa
Fabio Duran
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Psychiatric Neuroimaging (LIM-21), Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de Sao Paulo (HCFMUSP), Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Sol Fittipaldi
Affiliation:
Latin American Brain Health Institute (BrainLat), https://ror.org/0326knt82Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), https://ror.org/04f7h3b65Universidad de San Andres, Victoria, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin (TCD), Dublin, Ireland Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California San Francisco (UCSF), San Francisco, California, USA
Alex Fornito
Affiliation:
The Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia Monash Biomedical Imaging, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Nelson Freimer
Affiliation:
Center for Neurobehavioral Genetics, Jane and Terry Semel Institute for Neuroscience and Human Behavior Los Angeles, https://ror.org/046rm7j60University of California Los Angeles (UCLA), Los Angeles, USA
Ary Gadelha
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Laboratory in Clinical Neuroscience (LiNC), Department of Psychiatry, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Clarissa Gama
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular Psychiatry, Centro de Pesquisa Clínica, https://ror.org/010we4y38Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil Department of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Hospital de Clnicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil
Ranjini Garani
Affiliation:
Integrated Program in Neuroscience, https://ror.org/01pxwe438McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada
Clemente Garcia-Rizo
Affiliation:
Barcelona Clinic Schizophrenia Unit, Hospital Clínic de Barcelona, Departament de Medicina, Institut de Neurociències (UBNeuro), https://ror.org/02a2kzf50Universitat de Barcelona (UB), Institut d’Investigacions Biomèdiques, August Pi i Sunyer (IDIBAPS), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII), Barcelona, Spain
Cecilia Gonzalez Campo
Affiliation:
Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), https://ror.org/04f7h3b65Universidad de San Andres, Victoria, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Alfonso Gonzalez-Valderrama
Affiliation:
Early Intervention Program, Instituto Psiquiátrico Dr. J. Horwitz Barak, Santiago, Chile School of Medicine, https://ror.org/0225snd59 Universidad Finis Terrae , Santiago, Chile
Salvador Guinjoan
Affiliation:
https://ror.org/05e6pjy56 Laureate Institute for Brain Research , Tulsa, OK, USA
Bharath Holla
Affiliation:
Department of Integrative Medicine, https://ror.org/0405n5e57NIMHANS, Bengaluru, Karnataka, India Accelerator Program for Discovery in Brain disorders using Stem cells (ADBS), Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/0405n5e57NIMHANS, Bengaluru, Karnataka, India
Agustin Ibañez
Affiliation:
Latin American Brain Health Institute (BrainLat), https://ror.org/0326knt82Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Cognitive Neuroscience Center (CNC), https://ror.org/04f7h3b65Universidad de San Andres, Victoria, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), Trinity College Dublin (TCD), Dublin, Ireland Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI), University of California San Francisco (UCSF), San Francisco, California, USA
Daniza Ivanovic
Affiliation:
Laboratorio de Neurociencia Social y Neuromodulación, Centro de Investigación en Complejidad Social (neuroCICS), Facultad de Gobierno, https://ror.org/05y33vv83Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile
Andrea Jackowski
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/02k5swt12Universidade Federal de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil Department of Education, Information and Communications Technology (ICT) and Learning, Østfold University College, Halden, Norway
Pablo Leon-Ortiz
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychiatry, Direction of Research, https://ror.org/01c5r7j06Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía, Mexico City, Mexico
Christine Lochner
Affiliation:
South African Medical Research Council (SA MRC) Unit on Risk and Resilience in Mental Disorders, Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/05bk57929 Stellenbosch University , Stellenbosch, South Africa
Carlos Lopez-Jaramillo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, https://ror.org/03bp5hc83University of Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia
Hilmar Luckhoff
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, https://ror.org/05bk57929 Stellenbosch University , Cape Town, South Africa
Raffael Massuda
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/05syd6y78Universidade Federal do Paraná (UFPR), Curitiba, Brazil
Philip McGuire
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/052gg0110University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Oxford Centre for Integrative Neuroimaging (OxCIN), https://ror.org/052gg0110University of Oxford, Oxford, UK NIHR Oxford Health Biomedical Research Centre, Oxford, UK Oxford Health National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Jun Miyata
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Aichi Medical University, Aichi, Japan Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, https://ror.org/02kpeqv85Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan
Romina Mizrahi
Affiliation:
Integrated Program in Neuroscience, https://ror.org/01pxwe438McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, Canada Clinical and Translational Sciences Lab, McGill University, Douglas Mental Health University Institute, Montreal, QC, Canada Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/01pxwe438McGill University, Montreal, Canada
Robin Murray
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College, London, UK
Aysegul Ozerdem
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Psychology, Mayo Clinic, Rochester, USA
Pedro Pan
Affiliation:
Interdisciplinary Laboratory in Clinical Neuroscience (LiNC), Department of Psychiatry, Federal University of São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil National Institute of Developmental Psychiatry for Children and Adolescents (INPD), São Paulo, Brazil
Mara Parellada
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry and Mental Health, Hospital General Universitario Gregorio Marañón, Instituto de Investigación Sanitaria Gregorio Marañón (IISGM), Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, School of Medicine, Universidad Complutense, Madrid, Spain
Lebogang Phahladira
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine and Health Sciences, https://ror.org/05bk57929 Stellenbosch University , Cape Town, South Africa
Juan P. Ramirez-Mahaluf
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, https://ror.org/04teye511Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
Ramiro Reckziegel
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Molecular Psychiatry, Centro de Pesquisa Clínica, https://ror.org/010we4y38Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil
Tiago Reis Marques
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College, London, UK
Francisco Reyes-Madrigal
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Experimental Psychiatry, Direction of Research, https://ror.org/01c5r7j06Instituto Nacional de Neurología y Neurocirugía, Mexico City, Mexico
Annerine Roos
Affiliation:
South African Medical Research Council (SA MRC) Unit on Risk and Resilience in Mental Disorders, Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience Institute, https://ror.org/03p74gp79University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
Pedro Rosa
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Psychiatric Neuroimaging (LIM-21), Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de Sao Paulo (HCFMUSP), Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Giovanni Salum
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul (UFRGS), Hospital de Clnicas de Porto Alegre, Porto Alegre, Brazil National Institute of Developmental Psychiatry for Children and Adolescents (INPD), São Paulo, Brazil Child Mind Institute, New York, NY, USA
Freda Scheffler
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa Neuroscience Institute, University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
Gunter Schumann
Affiliation:
Centre for Population Neuroscience and Stratified Medicine (PONS), Institute for Science and Technology for Brain-inspired Intelligence, Fudan University, Shanghai, China PONS-Centre, Charité Mental Health, Dept of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Charité Campus Mitte, Berlin, Germany
Mauricio Serpa
Affiliation:
Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clínicas da Faculdade de Medicina da Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil
Dan J. Stein
Affiliation:
South African Medical Research Council (SA MRC) Unit on Risk and Resilience in Mental Disorders, Department of Psychiatry and Neuroscience Institute, https://ror.org/03p74gp79University of Cape Town, Cape Town, South Africa
Angeles Tepper
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, https://ror.org/04teye511Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
Jeggan Tiego
Affiliation:
The Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, School of Psychological Sciences, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia Monash Biomedical Imaging, https://ror.org/02bfwt286Monash University, Melbourne, Australia
Tsukasa Ueno
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Graduate School of Medicine, https://ror.org/02kpeqv85Kyoto University, Kyoto, Japan Integrated Clinical Education Center, Kyoto University Hospital, Kyoto, Japan
Juan Undurraga
Affiliation:
Department of Neurology and Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, Clínica Alemana Universidad del Desarrollo, Vitacura, Santiago, Chile
Eduardo A Undurraga
Affiliation:
Escuela de Gobierno, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile Research Center for Integrated Disaster Risk Management (CIGIDEN), Santiago, Chile Canadian Institute for Advanced Research (CIFAR) Azrieli Global Scholars Program, CIFAR, Toronto, Canada
Pedro Valdes-Sosa
Affiliation:
The Clinical Hospital of Chengdu Brain Science Institute, University of Electronic Science and Technology of China, Chengdu, China Centro de Neurociencias de Cuba, La Habana, Cuba
Isabel Valli
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosis Studies, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, https://ror.org/0220mzb33King’s College, London, UK FIDMAG Germanes Hospitalaries Research Foundation, Barcelona, Spain
Mirta Villarreal
Affiliation:
Grupo de Investigación en Neurociencias Aplicadas a las Alteraciones de la Conducta (INAAC), Fleni-CONICET Neurosciences Institute (INEU), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina Department of Physics, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad Autónoma de Buenos Aires, Argentina
Toby T Winton-Brown
Affiliation:
Department of Neuroscience, Central Clinical School, Monash University, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia Department of Psychiatry, Alfred Health, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia
Nefize Yalin
Affiliation:
Centre for Affective Disorders, Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK South London and Maudsley National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust, London, UK
Francisco Zamorano
Affiliation:
Unidad de Imágenes Cuantitativas Avanzadas, Departamento de Imágenes, Clínica Alemana de Santiago, Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile Facultad de Ciencias para el Cuidado de la Salud, Campus Los Leones, https://ror.org/04jrwm652 Universidad San Sebastián , Santiago, Chile
Marcus Zanetti
Affiliation:
Laboratory of Psychiatric Neuroimaging (LIM-21), Departamento e Instituto de Psiquiatria, Hospital das Clinicas Faculdade de Medicina Universidade de Sao Paulo (HCFMUSP), Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade de São Paulo, São Paulo, Brazil Hospital Sírio-Libanês, São Paulo, Brazil
Anderson M Winkler
Affiliation:
Department of Human Genetics, University of Texas Rio Grande Valley, Brownsville, USA
Sara Evans-Lacko
Affiliation:
Care Policy and Evaluation Centre, London School of Economics and Political Science, London, UK
Nicolas A. Crossley*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Faculty of Medicine, https://ror.org/03bp5hc83University of Antioquia, Medellín, Colombia Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/052gg0110University of Oxford, Oxford, UK Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, https://ror.org/04teye511Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile
*
Corresponding author: Nicolas A. Crossley; Email: ncrossley@uc.cl

Abstract

Background

The macro-social and environmental conditions in which people live, such as the level of a country’s development or inequality, are associated with brain-related disorders. However, the relationship between these systemic environmental factors and the brain remains unclear. We aimed to determine the association between the level of development and inequality of a country and the brain structure of healthy adults.

Methods

We conducted a cross-sectional study pooling brain imaging (T1-based) data from 145 magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies in 7,962 healthy adults (4,110 women) in 29 different countries. We used a meta-regression approach to relate the brain structure to the country’s level of development and inequality.

Results

Higher human development was consistently associated with larger hippocampi and more expanded global cortical surface area, particularly in frontal areas. Increased inequality was most consistently associated with smaller hippocampal volume and thinner cortical thickness across the brain.

Conclusions

Our results suggest that the macro-economic conditions of a country are reflected in its inhabitants’ brains and may explain the different incidence of brain disorders across the world. The observed variability of brain structure in health across countries should be considered when developing tools in the field of personalized or precision medicine that are intended to be used across the world.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

The conditions in which people are born, grow, live, and age, also known as the social determinants of health [Reference Braveman and Gottlieb1], may increase the risk of developing brain-related disorders [Reference Lund, Brooke-Sumner, Baingana, Baron, Breuer and Chandra2Reference Santamaria-Garcia, Sainz-Ballesteros, Hernandez, Moguilner, Maito and Ochoa-Rosales4]. Furthermore, it has been proposed that these environmental factors are biologically embedded in the brain [Reference Hertzman5]. Studying the link between the brain and environmental risk factors using approaches such as neuroimaging is an established way to shed light on the neural underpinnings of the vulnerability to brain disorders in adverse conditions [Reference Tost, Champagne and Meyer-Lindenberg6Reference Hair, Hanson, Wolfe and Pollak8]. Arguably, this research has the potential to shape novel interventions and inform public health policies [Reference Weissman, Hatzenbuehler, Cikara, Barch and McLaughlin9, Reference Farah10].

The environment influences health at different interacting organizational levels, from the individual to the global [Reference Jordán11, Reference Rose12]. All levels within this complex system must be studied, since they cannot be reduced to a single (lower or higher) tier. The individual’s environment is shaped by their circumstances and personal history, which require assessments tailored to the uniqueness of their situation [Reference Reichert, Gan, Renz, Braun, Brüßler and Timm13]. People also share common environmental exposures among different subgroups, communities, or even countries, which impact their outcomes [Reference Berkman, Kawachi and Glymour14]. Individuals may not be fully aware of their influence, and therefore, a study based on the individual experience of a specific exposure might miss its impact, as it has been widely discussed for racism [Reference Gee and Ford15]. Such group-level influences are seldom addressed in imaging research, with studies mostly focusing on individual exposures. For example, worse macro-economic conditions as those from low- and middle-income countries (LMIC), are associated with a higher proportion of dementia cases [Reference Livingston, Huntley, Sommerlad, Ames, Ballard and Banerjee16] and explain disparities in ageing processes [Reference Legaz, Altschuler, Gonzalez-Gomez, Hernández, Baez and Migeot17, Reference Moguilner, Baez, Hernandez, Migeot, Legaz and Gonzalez-Gomez18]. Despite some controversy [Reference Jorm and Mulder19], people from LMIC have a higher likelihood of depression [Reference Lim, Tam, Lu, Ho, Zhang and Ho20, Reference Errazuriz, Avello-Vega, Ramirez-Mahaluf, Torres, Crossley and Undurraga21]. With regards to the potential brain mechanisms underlying these effects, previous studies have consistently found associations between poverty, hippocampal volume, and total cortical surface area [Reference Taylor, Cooper, Jackson and Barch22Reference Noble, Houston, Brito, Bartsch, Kan and Kuperman25].

Some properties of the social environment, such as exposure to inequality, are better defined for groups of people rather than individuals, which makes it difficult to study in a typical individual-based MRI study. Income inequality has been associated with worse health [Reference Pickett and Wilkinson26], including mental health [Reference Ribeiro, Bauer, Andrade, York-Smith, Pan and Pingani27]. The proposed mechanism of income inequality has been attributed to “social stress” related to relative status hierarchies rather than absolute wealth [Reference Kawachi and Kennedy28]. Social stress related to stigma has been associated with hippocampal volume decreases [Reference Hatzenbuehler, Weissman, McKetta, Lattanner, Ford and Barch29], and general stress with cortical thinning of frontoparietal regions [Reference Bartlett, Klein, Li, DeLorenzo, Kotov and Perlman30, Reference Treadway, Waskom, Dillon, Holmes, Park and Chakravarty31].

The macro-social and environmental factors that impact health have been mostly studied using epidemiological approaches based on large surveys or administrative data, which typically do not provide detailed biological information such as brain scans. Recently, we and others [Reference Weissman, Hatzenbuehler, Cikara, Barch and McLaughlin9, Reference Zugman, Alliende, Medel, Bethlehem, Seidlitz and Ringlein32] have shown that the macro-social organization of the state or country where people live is reflected in their brain structure [Reference Sheridan33]. Here we sought to advance our understanding of how the brain is impacted by the social determinants of health at the country-level. We therefore examined the associations between the brain structure of healthy adults with country-level indices of development and inequality across 29 different countries using a meta-regression approach. We used the United Nations’ Human Development Index as a measure of country-level development, a composite measure which includes economic activity (income), education (years of education), and health (lifespan) of the population [Reference Sagar and Najam34]. To index inequality, we used the United Nations’ metric based on the distributions of income, education, and health. Our novel approach could be considered an ecological imaging approach, in which properties of groups of people are associated with their average brain structure. We hypothesized that human development across countries would be positively associated with total cortical area surface and hippocampal volume. Echoing stress-related changes reported in the literature, we hypothesized that exposure to inequality would be negatively correlated with hippocampal volumes and frontoparietal cortical thickness.

Methods and materials

Search strategy

Our main aim was to include MRI data from a wide range of countries as required by our method. We therefore included open access data reporting MRI images from healthy adults, collating several databases until November 2021 (Supplementary Figure S1). We were particularly interested in studies performed in under-represented countries and included data from collaborators across the world [Reference Zugman, Alliende, Medel, Bethlehem, Seidlitz and Ringlein32].

Inclusion criteria

We included samples approved by the local ethics committee that reported T1-weighted MRIs from at least 15 adults reported as healthy aged 18–40 years (inclusive). Images were acquired on 1.5 T and 3 T MRI scanners. We excluded samples obtained from 7 T MRI scanners because they require adjustments to the data processing pipeline, rendering their results less comparable [Reference Lüsebrink, Wollrab and Speck35]. Additionally, their prevalence in countries with high human development levels could introduce significant bias.

Individual-level information extracted

At the individual level, we collected age and gender for every participant. Considering that pseudo-anonymized data acquired for diverse purposes and released to the public have little other information, we were not able to extract important aspects such as the socioeconomic characteristics (income or level of education) of the participants.

Pre-processing of imaging data

All imaging data were processed using FreeSurfer’s cortical reconstruction pipeline recon-all (see Supplementary Table S1 for details of the specific version used). We examined associations with total intracranial volume (eTIV), hippocampal volume, and morphometric properties of the cortex, including thickness and surface area from the two hemispheres and the 68 cortical regions of interest (ROI) of the Desikan–Killiany atlas [Reference Desikan, Ségonne, Fischl, Quinn, Dickerson and Blacker36]. Quality control was based on an initial visual inspection, which was in some samples performed locally by collaborating groups (Supplementary Table S1) or by two reviewers. It was then followed up by an automatic quality control procedure in which participants were excluded if any of the morphometric properties including any ROIs, either in thickness or surface, were outliers in their study sample as defined by Tukey’s fence:

([1]) $$ \left[{Q}_1- k\left({Q}_3-{Q}_1\right),{Q}_3+ k\left({Q}_3-{Q}_1\right)\right] $$

where Q refers to the respective quartile and using a k = 3 so that values that are “far out” were identified [Reference Tukey37].

Considering the consistent difference observed between genders in head size [Reference Eliot, Ahmed, Khan and Patel38], we also examined a scatterplot of the difference between women and men in intracranial volume as quality control of data labeling, excluding one dataset that was an extreme outlier.

Country-level measures of development and inequality

Country-level characteristics were obtained from the United Nations Development Program from data published for the year 2019. These included the Human Development Index (HDI) and an inequality factor. The human development index was created to emphasize that people and their capabilities should be the criteria to assess the development of a country, and not only the national income. It is built using the geometric mean from indices in three key dimensions of human development: a long and healthy life (measured using the life expectancy at birth), access to knowledge (using the mean between the expected years of schooling and average years of schooling) and a decent standard of living (gross national income per capita) [Reference Stanton39, 40]. It first defines minimum and maximum values for each dimension, and a normalized value is calculated using the following formula:

([2]) $$ \mathrm{Dimension}\ \mathrm{index}=\frac{\mathrm{actual}\ \mathrm{value}-\mathrm{minimum}\ \mathrm{value}}{\mathrm{maximum}\ \mathrm{value}-\mathrm{minimum}\ \mathrm{value}} $$

The coefficient of human inequality draws on the Atkinson’s inequality measures [Reference Atkinson41], which are widely used in the Social Sciences. Alongside a measure of distribution, it also includes an aversion parameter, which is adjusted to reflect society’s sensitivity to inequality. For the HDI, the United Nations sets this aversion ε parameter to 1 (i.e., equal weight is given to everyone’s welfare in society), leading to:

([3]) $$ A=1-\frac{g}{\mu} $$

where g is the geometric mean of the distribution and μ is the arithmetic mean. These inequality indices A are measured for each dimension, and pooled using their arithmetic mean. Since inequality is negatively associated with HDI (a correlation of −0.9 (P < 0.0001) across all countries for which the indices are reported), we regressed out the HDI. In other words, we are looking at brain associations with varying degrees of inequality at the same level of development. Most previous health studies examining inequality refer to income inequality. In this dataset, the multidimensional inequality index is highly correlated with income inequality (correlation of 0.86, P < 0.0001), so we could not examine if associations were driven by specific dimensions.

Meta-regression analyses

We performed meta-regression analyses examining the associations between HDI and the brain, and separately between inequality and the brain. These analyses were performed on morphometric properties that in previous studies, at the individual level, have been most consistently associated with poverty and inequality: the eTIV, the cortex, and the hippocampi [Reference Rakesh and Whittle24, Reference Hatzenbuehler, Weissman, McKetta, Lattanner, Ford and Barch29]. For the cortex analyses, we focused on cortical thickness and surface area from the two hemispheres, and performed a regional analysis of the 68 regions of interest from the Desikan–Killiany atlas. For these latter analyses addressing localized differences in subregions within hemispheres, we corrected results for multiple testing using false-discovery-rate (fdr). The choice of cortical thickness and surface area over a combined metric such as volume was based on their differential genetic control and environmental influence [Reference Zugman, Alliende, Medel, Bethlehem, Seidlitz and Ringlein32, Reference Winkler, Kochunov, Blangero, Almasy, Zilles and Fox42]. We used a random-effects model with weights based on the inverse of the variance of the imaging metric examined, modeling the between-study variance using the Paule and Mandel estimator [Reference Veroniki, Jackson, Viechtbauer, Bender, Bowden and Knapp43]. To ensure that differences were not driven by sex or age differences, we included the mean age and the proportion of men as extra regressors.

Examining the reliability of the results

Brain differences between countries could be due to differences in the scanner, or otherwise differences in the genetic or ethnic background of the population studied. We performed a jackknife analysis to examine if results were driven by a single site (leave-one- sample-out approach) and excluding all studies from a country (leave-one-country-out approach). We also performed analyses excluding all Western nations here defined as European countries, Canada, USA, and Australia.

To quantify the potential impact of data acquired with MRI scanners with different magnetic fields, namely 3 T and 1.5 T, we conducted sensitivity analyses incorporating the use of 1.5 T MRI scanner as an additional dummy regressor in our meta-regression analyses.

Countries and their populations undergo changes in time, including shifts in their level of development and inequality. To examine the potential impact of these changes, we used the HDI and inequality (adjusted) indices in the model corresponding to the year in which the images were obtained. Since open-access data do not have this information, we imputed it assuming it was three years prior to the publication date of the article associated with the dataset, when such a publication was available (see Supplementary Table S1). Note that inequality data for the Human Development Index was only published after 2010 by the United Nations, which restricts the scope of this analysis.

Reporting effect sizes

To facilitate the interpretation of results, beyond betas that may be difficult to understand in isolation, we report findings related to percent changes of brain structure using the average brain of the whole sample. We describe associations related to a 0.1 change in HDI and 5 points in the Inequality index adjusted for HDI (see Figure 1A to get an idea of this effect size).

Figure 1. Geographical location of the samples included, their size, and characteristics of their countries. Interval highlighted in the graph (0.1 points for HDI and 5 points for inequality) are the intervals used to express effect sizes. Values of HDI and Inequality (adjusted) for each country included are also reported in Supplementary Table S2.

Analyses were performed in R (4.3.1) using the Metafor package [Reference Viechtbauer44].

Results

We included 145 samples of MRI images from 29 different countries from all groups of regions in the world (Figure 1; Supplementary Table S2). The total number of participants was 7,962, including 3,852 men and 4,110 women. Thirty-five percent of participants lived in a low-and-middle-income country (LMIC). The median average age of participants across samples was 24.2 years (range 18.3 to 31.7). Nearly 80% of the datasets were likely acquired after 2010 (Supplementary Table S1). Supplementary Figure S2 plots the changes in the development and inequality indices in those countries with datasets acquired in more than one year.

Brain associations with human development

The country-level HDI was associated with total brain volume (eTIV or estimated total intracranial volume), with an increase of 0.1 points in the HDI associated with an increase of 1.57% (95% CI 0.07 to 3.06%, P = 0.041 and R 2 = 5.92%) (Figure 2A). Jackknife analyses showed that this result was significant 93.8% of the time when a study was excluded, and 85.7% when a country was excluded (Figure 2A). Subgroup analysis of non-Western countries was also significant, but the association was not present when considering Western countries.

Figure 2. Brain associations with the Human Development Index. (A) Associations with estimated total intracranial volume. Reliability graph describes jack-knife analyses leaving-one- sample-out and leave-one-country-out. (B) Associations with hemispheric and regional surface. (C) Associations with hippocampal volume.

The cortical surface area estimates for both hemispheres were positively correlated with HDI [1.14% increases (95% CI 0.34–1.94) for 0.1 change in HDI in the right hemisphere, P = 0.005; 1.22% (95% CI 0.45–1.99) in the left, P = 0.0018] (Figure 2B). These results were robust to leave-one-study-out, or one country out. They were also significant when controlling for the average total intracranial volume of each sample. The results remained unchanged for the left hemisphere only when considering non-Western countries, and they were not significant for Western countries only. Regional analyses corrected for fdr showed that the relationship of cortical surface area with both indices was widespread across the cortex but was particularly concentrated in frontal regions when accounting for the total intracranial volume (Figure 2B).

Hemispheric cortical thickness was not associated with HDI (left P = 0.86; right P = 0.88). Analyses on brain sub-regions showed a significant association between thickness and HDI in both right and left post-central gyri.

Hippocampal volume was positively associated with HDI in the left (1.36% increase per 0.1 change in HDI, 95% CI 0.52–2.21, P < 0.002) and right hemisphere (0.99%, 95% CI 0.14–1.84, P = 0.022) (Figure 2C). Results were robust to jackknife analyses as shown in Figure 2C. These associations were still significant when correcting for total brain volume for the left, but not the right, hippocampus. It was also present for studies including only non-Western countries, but not for Western countries.

Table 1A Provides a summary of all the findings related to HDI.

Table 1. Summary of main results

- Brain associations with inequality

We then examined the associations between brain structure and inequality (HDI-adjusted). We report effect sizes in percentage changes of the average structural brain associated to changes of 5 points in the Inequality index (see Figure 1A).

Total intracranial volume was negatively associated with inequality (decreases of 2.1% associated with increases of 5 points in inequality, 95% CI −4.1 to −0.06%, P = 0.044) (Figure 3A). However, this result was reproduced in 86% of the leave-one-sample-out analyses, and 60.7% of the leave-one-country-out analyses. It was also not replicated in the analyses including only non-Western countries’ samples or Western countries’ samples.

Figure 3. Brain associations with HDI-adjusted inequality. Associations with (A) estimated total intracranial volume, (B) hemispheric cortical surface area, as well as regional associations, (C) hemispheric and regional cortical thickness, and (D) hippocampal volume. In all panels heat maps represent leave-one-sample-out analyses and leave-one-country-out analyses (excluding all studies of a particular country). Note that no surface area results were significant after controlling for total intracranial volume.

There was a significant negative association with brain cortical hemispheric surface (right decreases of −1.44% for every 5-point increases, 95% CI −2.55 to −0.33, P = 0.011; left decreases of −1.37%, 95% CI −2.44 to −0.3, P = 0.012) (Figure 3B). Results were stable when performing leave-one-sample-out (100% in both hemispheres) and leave-one-country-out (96% in both hemispheres). When controlling for total intracranial volume, results were significant on the right but not left side. Results were significant when looking only at Western samples, with no significant associations in non-Western samples. This association was significant at the regional fdr-corrected level in areas such as bilateral entorhinal cortex and inferior temporal area; left superior temporal, temporal pole, supramarginal gyrus, and rostral anterior cingulate; and right insula and inferior parietal (Pfdr < 0.05; Figure 3A). When controlling for total intracranial volume, none of these regional results were significant.

We also found a significant negative association between inequality and the right hemisphere mean cortical thickness (decreases of 1.22% per 5 points increase, 95% CI −1.98 to −0.45, P-value = 0.0018), as well as in the left hemisphere (decreases of 1.24% per 5 points increase, 95% CI −2.01 to −0.46, P-value = 0.0017; Figure 3B). These results were consistent in the leave-one-sample-out and leave-one-country-out analyses. It was also observed in samples including non-Western countries only, but not for Western countries. As shown in Figure 3B, this relationship was widespread in the brain, particularly in temporal and parietal regions.

Bilateral hippocampal volume was also associated with inequality (left hippocampus, decrease of 1.78% per 5 points increase, 95% CI −2.9 to −0.65, P = 0.002; right hippocampus, decrease of 1.98% per 5 points increase, 95% CI −3.13 to −0.84, P = 0.0007). This result was significant when controlling for total intracranial volume. It was robust to leave-one-sample-out (100% significant bilateral), and leave-one-country-out (96% significant on the left, 100% on the right). It was also present bilaterally in samples from non-Western countries, but not in Western samples.

Table 1B Provides a summary of all the findings related to inequality.

- Sensitivity analyses

The use of 1.5 T MRI scanner instead of 3 T had no significant effect in the main analyses, as shown in Supplementary Figure S3.

Modeling the level of development and inequality of a country according to the imputed year in which MRI images were obtained did not change substantially the main results (Supplementary Figure S4). Some results were no longer significant for the inequality analyses, particularly hippocampal volume or right hemispheric thickness, which could be related to the lower power of the inequality analysis.

Discussion

Our study provides further evidence of how macro-social and environmental factors are associated with brain structure in healthy adults. We found that higher country-level development was consistently and positively associated with hippocampal volume bilaterally and cortical surface area, even when controlling for total intracranial volume. Inequality on the other hand, was most consistently associated with thinner cortical thickness across the brain and smaller hippocampi volume, and less consistently with lower total intracranial volume and surface area, particularly in temporal and parietal surfaces.

These results could be compared to the many studies that have examined the effects of low socioeconomic status on the developing brain [Reference Taylor, Cooper, Jackson and Barch22, Reference Rakesh and Whittle24, Reference Noble, Houston, Brito, Bartsch, Kan and Kuperman25]. The country-level results echo similar findings from individuals within a community: higher levels of development are consistently associated with larger cortical surface area and larger hippocampi. This similarity is expected, given that the method used involves the average brain values across communities with different levels of development. Nonetheless, the consistency of these findings, combined with our robustness analyses, help to validate our novel approach. Our findings related to total brain size can also be read alongside the observed increases of head circumferences in children across the world in the last decades, which mirrors the increasing country development [Reference Zhang and Li45, Reference Bergerat, Heude, Taine, Tich, Werner and Frandji46]. The fact that better conditions lead to larger head circumferences in the next generations provides us with some clues about the causal direction of our observed association. More importantly, if we were to consider some of these brain morphometrics as integral part of brain health, they highlight that our indicators of worse brain health in some populations can be improved.

A similar expansion of brain area surface was seen in adults who participated in a randomized controlled trial of an educational intervention during childhood [Reference Farah, Sternberg, Nichols, Duda, Lohrenz and Luo47]. Therefore, one of the potential underlying mechanisms might be the greater availability of early educational opportunities in more developed countries. A larger head size after controlling for sex differences, often indicative of larger total intracranial volumes in healthy people, is also associated with lower dementia risks [Reference Perneczky, Wagenpfeil, Lunetta, Cupples, Green and Decarli48]. Considering the high burden of dementia in LMIC, smaller average brain size could be one aspect of a population-level marker of decreased brain reserve [Reference Stern, Albert, Barnes, Cabeza, Pascual-Leone and Rapp49].

Lower human development is defined by the presence of worse material conditions, education and health. The different measures used to build the human development index are highly correlated across countries. It is therefore difficult to disentangle between the effects of material conditions, education or health with our neuroimaging ecological approach. At the same time, these dimensions are also associated with other environmental conditions that could affect brain health and development, such as poor nutrition, pollution or exposure to violence. Our current approach could not address their impact either. Future studies could examine communities where these dimensions and associated factors are less correlated, using natural or quasi-experimental designs that exploit external shocks, policy changes, or natural variations creating plausibly exogenous differences in exposure. For example, examining the impact of increasing the length of compulsory education, the implementation of direct cash transfers to improve material conditions, or the introduction of pollution-control technologies.

The novel ecological imaging approach adopted in this study allowed the examination of brain associations with inequality, which is difficult to address in an individual-based imaging study. We found that country-level inequality was robustly associated with lower hippocampal volume. This result is in line with animal models of social defeat and its impact on the hippocampus [Reference Buwalda, Kole, Veenema, Huininga, de Boer and Korte50], as well as associations of smaller hippocampal volume in people who belong to a minority group [Reference Hatzenbuehler, Weissman, McKetta, Lattanner, Ford and Barch29]. We also found consistent associations with a thinner global cortical thickness, particularly in temporo-parietal regions. It remains unclear whether these associations could be attributed to a social stress mechanism, and further studies will have to examine further these suggestive findings. As has been proposed for the relationship between mortality and inequality [Reference Gravelle51], it is plausible that some of these findings are due to the use of aggregate data (ecological fallacy) in morphological characteristics that are solely influenced by development. This is particularly the case for our hippocampal results, where the observed association with development is unlikely to follow a simple linear association. It is less likely for thickness since this morphometric property was not associated with development.

The observed variability of brain structure in different environments echoes warnings from epidemiological science highlighting the existence of risk factors that affect populations and individuals [Reference Rose12]. In this context, case–control studies within a population might fail to elicit the population-level risks, which we have shown occurs in the typical case–control imaging studies in psychiatry [Reference Crossley, Zugman, Reyes-Madrigal, Czepielewski, Castro and Diaz-Zuluaga52]. Our results are also relevant for the field of personalized or precision medicine and psychiatry [Reference Coutts, Koutsouleris and McGuire53]. There are clear advantages of creating normative models of brain development for the definition of pathological states [Reference Bethlehem, Seidlitz, White, Vogel, Anderson and Adamson54, Reference Rutherford, Fraza, Dinga, Kia, Wolfers and Zabihi55]. However, while there is a recognized effort to include samples from diverse world regions to create those brain charts, particularly considering possible ethnic variations in brain shape [Reference Holla, Taylor, Glen, Lee, Vaidya and Mehta56, Reference Tang, Hojatkashani, Dinov, Sun, Fan and Lin57], current approaches have modelled site differences using age and sex and assumed that the rest of the variance is due to the inter-scanner difference. Little attention has been paid to the need to consider brain development unaffected by socio-environmental conditions when defining standards, as was done by the World Health Organization when creating their growth curves [58]. As we show here, brain metrics also reflect the population-level exposure to risk factors such as lower development. For machine-learning approaches, we should be careful to prevent the introduction of a representation bias when building these algorithms in a limited number of high-income countries [Reference Mehrabi, Morstatter, Saxena, Lerman and Galstyan59]. Such a bias would unintendedly perpetuate existing health inequalities across the world. The results shown highlight the importance of integrating neuroscience with global mental health [Reference Stein, Phillips, Sahakian, Williams and Patel60].

Our study relied on finding differences across sites and countries. We did not apply any harmonization procedure accounting for differences in acquisition across sites, as is now commonly done [Reference Fortin, Cullen, Sheline, Taylor, Aselcioglu and Cook61]. Controlling for scanner variability would have potentially masked site-specific differences, which were central to our study. Inter-site variability would have little impact on our findings as long as it was not correlated with HDI. As our sensitivity analyses showed, using a 1.5 T MRI scanner instead of a 3 T had a small impact on the results.

We should mention some limitations of this study. Previous studies have suggested that international differences in brain structure could be related to cultural factors shaping the brain [Reference Han and Northoff62] or genetic background [Reference Xu, Guo, Cheng, Wang, Geng and Zhu63], although the evidence suggest their effect is restricted to specific brain regions and are not associated with a global effect as we found [Reference Tang, Zhao, Lou, Shi, Fang and Lin64]. Our confirmatory analyses on Western and non-Western countries aimed to explore this possibility. Our findings do not rule out the influence of cultural or genetic factors on brain structure, since much of this variance is independent from levels of development. For the correlated variance, particularly relating a possible impact of genetics on development and brain structure, we argue that the previously mentioned temporal trends within populations, linking larger head size with advances in development [Reference Zhang and Li45, Reference Bergerat, Heude, Taine, Tich, Werner and Frandji46], challenge this interpretation. We included only adults, a life stage where the brain is less influenced by development or aging processes. However, this choice limited our ability to study how macro-economic conditions affect these processes, such as identifying potential critical periods. Additionally, our results were not always replicated in subgroup analyses, especially in Western-only samples. This is likely due to the lower variance existing in HDI and inequality metrics in those countries, which reduced statistical power.

A further important point is that our study is based on samples that may not be representative of their broader country population, and we lacked individual-level data to address this limitation. Populations within countries are diverse, and imaging studies, even those using epidemiological sampling [Reference Fry, Littlejohns, Sudlow, Doherty, Adamska and Sprosen65], often do not fully represent all communities. Despite these limitations, our study emphasizes the importance of considering the conditions in which people lived when data was collected, even using a broad classification such as country, when interpreting their findings [Reference Henrich, Heine and Norenzayan66]. We hope it will also encourage imaging researchers to provide information about the socioeconomic context in which their studies were performed, which we would argue has important effects on their findings.

In summary, we here show suggestive evidence that the macroeconomic conditions of a country are reflected in its inhabitants’ brains. Our results suggest that human development is associated with larger brains with greater cortical surface and hippocampi, while inequality is most consistently associated with lower hippocampal volume and thinner global cortical thickness.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2025.10060.

Data availability statement

Group-level data and scripts can be requested from authors.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank Dr Andre Zugman and Dr Daniel Pine who facilitated data interpretation. Previous collaborative work with them formed the basis for the current work. Increases in the complexity of the process for obtaining approval for government scientists to participate in collaborative research prevented their ability to participate in timely review of the final manuscript.

NAC, JU and AG-V are supported by ANID-PIA-ACT192064, and ANID-FONDECYT Regular 1240426. MB is supported by the Instituto de Salud Carlos III, the Spanish Ministry of Science, Innovation and Universities, the European Regional Development Fund (ERDF/FEDER)(PI08/0208, PI11/00325, PI14/00612); CIBERSAM; CERCA Program; Catalan Government, the Secretariat of Universities and Research of the Department of Enterprise and Knowledge (2017SGR1355) and Institut de Neurociències, Universitat de Barcelona. GFB is supported by FAPESP grant number 14/50873- 3. CDC has received grant support from Instituto de Salud Carlos III (PI17/00481, JR19/00024, PI20/00721). AI is partially supported by grants from ReDLat [National Institutes of Health and the Fogarty International Center (FIC), National Institutes of Aging (R01 AG057234, R01 AG075775, R01 AG21051, CARDS-NIH), Alzheimer’s Association (SG-20-725707), Rainwater Charitable Foundation – The Bluefield project to cure FTD, and Global Brain Health Institute)], ANID-FONDECYT Regular (1210195, 1210176 and 1220995); and ANID-FONDAP 15150012. CGR is supported by project PI20/00661 from Instituto de Salud Carlos III and co-financed by the European Union (Feder)” a way of making Europe “. GS is supported by the Horizon 2020 funded ERC Advanced Grant ‘STRATIFY’ (695313), RFIS-NSFC grant (82150710554) and the DFG FKZ 458317126. CA and CDC are supported by the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, Instituto de Salud Carlos III (ISCIII), co-financed by the European Union, ERDF Funds from the European Commission, “A way of making Europe” (PI19/01024, PI20/00721, JR19/00024), financed by the European Union - NextGenerationEU (PMP21/00051), CIBERSAM, Madrid Regional Government (B2017/BMD-3740 AGES-CM-2), European Union Structural Funds, European Union Seventh Framework Program, European Union H2020 Program under the Innovative Medicines Initiative 2 Joint Undertaking: Project PRISM-2 (Grant agreement No.101034377), Project AIMS-2-TRIALS (Grant agreement No 777394), Horizon Europe (Grant agreement No 101057182, project Youth-GEMs), the National Institute of Mental Health of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number 1U01MH124639-01 (Project ProNET) and Award Number 5P50MH115846-03 (project FEP-CAUSAL), Fundación Familia Alonso, and Fundación Alicia Koplowitz. JT is supported by a Turner Impact Fellowship from the Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University. MNC and MFV are supported by PICT 2017-0955 from Agencia Nacional de Promoción Científica y Tecnológica Argentina.

The cVEDA authors include Pratima Murthy, Amit Chakrabarti, Debasish Basu, B.N. Subodh, Lenin Singh, Roshan Singh, Kartik Kalyanram, Kamakshi Kartik, Kalyanaraman Kumaran, Ghattu Krishnaveni, Rebecca Kuriyan, Sunita Simon Kurpad, Gareth J. Barker, Rose D. Bharath, Sylvane Desrivieres, Meera Purushottam, Dimitri P. Orfanos, Eesha Sharma, Matthew Hickman, Jon Heron, Mireille B. Toledano and Nilakshi Vaidya.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: NAC; Methodology and Analyses: NAC; Collating open-access data: LMA, VM; Writing – original draft: NAC; Writing – review & editing: all authors.

Competing interests

CA has been a consultant to or has received honoraria or grants from Acadia, Angelini, Biogen, Boehringer, Gedeon Richter, Janssen Cilag, Lundbeck, Medscape, Menarini, Minerva, Otsuka, Pfizer, Roche, Sage, Servier, Shire, Schering Plow, Sumitomo Dainippon Pharma, Sunovion and Takeda. CDC has received honoraria from Angelini and travel support from Janssen and Angelini. VM, LMA, RB, JS, GR, AA, LA, MBellgrove, VB, MBernardo, PB, JBB, RB, GB, MCastro, TCA, MCostanzi, LC, PD, CDFS, AMDZ, SDP, FD, SF, AF, NF, AG, CG, RG, CGR, CGC, AGV, SG, BH, AI, DI, AJ, PLO, CL, CLJ, HL, RM, PM, JM, RMizrahi, RMurray, AO, PP, MP, LP, JRM, RR, TRM, FRM, AR, PR, GSalum, FS, GSchumann, MS, DS, AT, JT, TU, JU, EU, PVS, IV, MV, TWB, NY, FZ, MZ, AW, SEL and NAC report no conflict of interests related to this work.

The contents of this publication are solely the authors’ responsibility and do not represent the official views of these institutions.

Footnotes

Vicente Medel and Luz M. Alliende: these authors contributed equally to this work.

References

Braveman, P, Gottlieb, L. The social determinants of health: it’s time to consider the causes of the causes. Pub Health Rep. 2014;129:1931.Google Scholar
Lund, C, Brooke-Sumner, C, Baingana, F, Baron, EC, Breuer, E, Chandra, P, et al. Social determinants of mental disorders and the sustainable development goals: a systematic review of reviews. Lancet Psychiatr. 2018;5:357–69.Google Scholar
Feigin, VL, Nichols, E, Alam, T, Bannick, MS, Beghi, E, Blake, N, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of neurological disorders, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet Neurol. 2019;18:459–80.Google Scholar
Santamaria-Garcia, H, Sainz-Ballesteros, A, Hernandez, H, Moguilner, S, Maito, M, Ochoa-Rosales, C, et al. Factors associated with healthy aging in Latin American populations. Nat Med. 2023:111.Google Scholar
Hertzman, C. Putting the concept of biological embedding in historical perspective. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2012;109:17160–7.Google Scholar
Tost, H, Champagne, FA, Meyer-Lindenberg, A. Environmental influence in the brain, human welfare and mental health. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:4121–31.Google Scholar
Ghoneem, A, Osborne, MT, Abohashem, S, Naddaf, N, Patrich, T, Dar, T, et al. Association of socioeconomic status and infarct volume with functional outcome in patients with ischemic stroke. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5:e229178.Google Scholar
Hair, NL, Hanson, JL, Wolfe, BL, Pollak, SD. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 2015;169:822–9.Google Scholar
Weissman, DG, Hatzenbuehler, ML, Cikara, M, Barch, DM, McLaughlin, KA. State-level macro-economic factors moderate the association of low income with brain structure and mental health in US children. Nat Commun. 2023;14:2085.Google Scholar
Farah, MJ. Socioeconomic status and the brain: Prospects for neuroscience-informed policy. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2018;19:428–38.Google Scholar
Jordán, F. The network perspective: vertical connections linking organizational levels. Ecol Modell. 2022;473:110112.Google Scholar
Rose, G. Sick individuals and sick populations. Int J Epidemiol. 2001;30: 427432.Google Scholar
Reichert, M, Gan, G, Renz, M, Braun, U, Brüßler, S, Timm, I, et al. Ambulatory assessment for precision psychiatry: foundations, current developments and future avenues. Exp Neurol. 2021;345:113807.Google Scholar
Berkman, L, Kawachi, I, Glymour, MM. Social epidemiology. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2014.Google Scholar
Gee, GC, Ford, CL. Structural racism and health inequities: old issues, new directions. Du Bois Rev 2011;8:115–32.Google Scholar
Livingston, G, Huntley, J, Sommerlad, A, Ames, D, Ballard, C, Banerjee, S, et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet. 2020;396:413–46.Google Scholar
Legaz, A, Altschuler, F, Gonzalez-Gomez, R, Hernández, H, Baez, S, Migeot, J, et al. Structural inequality linked to brain volume and network dynamics in aging and dementia across the Americas. Nat Aging. 2024;30:3646–57. https://doi.org/10.1038/s43587-024-00781-2.Google Scholar
Moguilner, S, Baez, S, Hernandez, H, Migeot, J, Legaz, A, Gonzalez-Gomez, R, et al. Brain clocks capture diversity and disparities in aging and dementia across geographically diverse populations. Nat Med. 2024; 30(12): 36463657.Google Scholar
Jorm, AF, Mulder, RT. Cross-national differences in the prevalence of mental illness symptoms: evidence against the vulnerability paradox. Aust N Z J Psychiatr. 2021;55:620–6.Google Scholar
Lim, GY, Tam, WW, Lu, Y, Ho, CS, Zhang, MW, Ho, RC. Prevalence of depression in the community from 30 countries between 1994 and 2014. Sci Rep 2018;8:110.Google Scholar
Errazuriz, A, Avello-Vega, D, Ramirez-Mahaluf, JP, Torres, R, Crossley, NA, Undurraga, EA, et al. Prevalence of depressive disorder in the adult population of Latin America: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Reg Health Am 2023;26:100587.Google Scholar
Taylor, RL, Cooper, SR, Jackson, JJ, Barch, DM. Assessment of neighborhood poverty, cognitive function, and prefrontal and hippocampal volumes in children. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3:e2023774.Google Scholar
McDermott, CL, Seidlitz, J, Nadig, A, Liu, S, Clasen, LS, Blumenthal, JD, et al. Longitudinally mapping childhood socioeconomic status associations with cortical and subcortical morphology. J Neurosci. 2019;39:1365–73.Google Scholar
Rakesh, D, Whittle, S. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain–A systematic review of neuroimaging findings in youth. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;130:379407.Google Scholar
Noble, KG, Houston, SM, Brito, NH, Bartsch, H, Kan, E, Kuperman, JM, et al. Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:773–8.Google Scholar
Pickett, KE, Wilkinson, RG. Income inequality and health: a causal review. Soc Sci Med. 2015;128:316–26.Google Scholar
Ribeiro, WS, Bauer, A, Andrade, MCR, York-Smith, M, Pan, PM, Pingani, L, et al. Income inequality and mental illness-related morbidity and resilience: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatr. 2017;4:554–62.Google Scholar
Kawachi, I, Kennedy, BP. Socioeconomic determinants of health: Health and social cohesion: why care about income inequality? Br Med J. 1997;314:1037.Google Scholar
Hatzenbuehler, ML, Weissman, DG, McKetta, S, Lattanner, MR, Ford, JV, Barch, DM, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume among black and latinx youth living in high-stigma contexts. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr. 2022;61:809–19.Google Scholar
Bartlett, EA, Klein, DN, Li, K, DeLorenzo, C, Kotov, R, Perlman, G. Depression severity over 27 months in adolescent girls is predicted by stress-linked cortical morphology. Biol Psychiatr. 2019;86:769–78.Google Scholar
Treadway, MT, Waskom, ML, Dillon, DG, Holmes, AJ, Park, MTM, Chakravarty, MM, et al. Illness progression, recent stress, and morphometry of hippocampal subfields and medial prefrontal cortex in major depression. Biol Psychiatr. 2015;77:285–94.Google Scholar
Zugman, A, Alliende, LM, Medel, V, Bethlehem, RAI, Seidlitz, J, Ringlein, G, et al. Country-level gender inequality is associated with structural differences in the brains of women and men. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2023;120:e2218782120.Google Scholar
Sheridan, MA. Measuring the impact of structural inequality on the structure of the brain. Proc Nat Acad Sci. 2023;120:e2306076120.Google Scholar
Sagar, AD, Najam, A. The human development index: a critical review. Ecol Econ. 1998;25:249–64.Google Scholar
Lüsebrink, F, Wollrab, A, Speck, O. Cortical thickness determination of the human brain using high resolution 3 T and 7 T MRI data. Neuroimage 2013;70:122–31.Google Scholar
Desikan, RS, Ségonne, F, Fischl, B, Quinn, BT, Dickerson, BC, Blacker, D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage 2006;31:968–80.Google Scholar
Tukey, JW. Exploratory data analysis. Vol. 2. Reading, MA; 1977.Google Scholar
Eliot, L, Ahmed, A, Khan, H, Patel, J. Dump the “dimorphism”: comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;125:667–97.Google Scholar
Stanton, EA. The human development index: a history. PERI Working Papers;2007, p. 85.Google Scholar
UNDP. 2019 HDR technical note. 2020.Google Scholar
Atkinson, AB. On the measurement of inequality. J Econ Theory. 1970;2:244–63.Google Scholar
Winkler, AM, Kochunov, P, Blangero, J, Almasy, L, Zilles, K, Fox, PT, et al. Cortical thickness or grey matter volume? The importance of selecting the phenotype for imaging genetics studies. Neuroimage. 2010;53:1135–46.Google Scholar
Veroniki, AA, Jackson, D, Viechtbauer, W, Bender, R, Bowden, J, Knapp, G, et al. Methods to estimate the between-study variance and its uncertainty in meta-analysis. Res Synth Method. 2016;7:5579.Google Scholar
Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36:148.Google Scholar
Zhang, YQ, Li, H. Changes in weight, length, head circumference, and ponderal index at birth of healthy term newborns in nine cities in China during the period of rapid social development 1985–2005. Econ Hum Biol. 2015;19.Google Scholar
Bergerat, M, Heude, B, Taine, M, Nguyen The Tich, S, Werner, A, Frandji, B, et al. Head circumference from birth to five years in France: new national reference charts and comparison to WHO standards. Lancet Reg Health Eur. 2021;5.Google Scholar
Farah, MJ, Sternberg, S, Nichols, TA, Duda, JT, Lohrenz, T, Luo, Y, et al. Randomized manipulation of early cognitive experience impacts adult brain structure. J Cogn Neurosci. 2021;33:1197–209.Google Scholar
Perneczky, R, Wagenpfeil, S, Lunetta, KL, Cupples, LA, Green, RC, Decarli, C, et al. Head circumference, atrophy, and cognition: implications for brain reserve in Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2010;75:137–42.Google Scholar
Stern, Y, Albert, M, Barnes, CA, Cabeza, R, Pascual-Leone, A, Rapp, PR. A framework for concepts of reserve and resilience in aging. Neurobiol Aging. 2023;124:100–3.Google Scholar
Buwalda, B, Kole, MHP, Veenema, AH, Huininga, M, de Boer, SF, Korte, SM, et al. Long-term effects of social stress on brain and behavior: a focus on hippocampal functioning. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29:8397.Google Scholar
Gravelle, H. How much of the relation between population mortality and unequal distribution of income is a statistical artefact? Br Med J. 1998;316:382–5.Google Scholar
Crossley, NA, Zugman, A, Reyes-Madrigal, F, Czepielewski, LS, Castro, MN, Diaz-Zuluaga, AM, et al. Structural brain abnormalities in schizophrenia in adverse environments: examining the effect of poverty and violence in six Latin American cities. Br J Psychiatr. 2021;218:112–8.Google Scholar
Coutts, F, Koutsouleris, N, McGuire, P. Psychotic disorders as a framework for precision psychiatry. Nat Rev Neurol. 2023:114.Google Scholar
Bethlehem, RAI, Seidlitz, J, White, SR, Vogel, JW, Anderson, KM, Adamson, C, et al. Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature. 2022:111.Google Scholar
Rutherford, S, Fraza, C, Dinga, R, Kia, SM, Wolfers, T, Zabihi, M, et al. Charting brain growth and aging at high spatial precision. Elife. 2022;11:e72904.Google Scholar
Holla, B, Taylor, PA, Glen, DR, Lee, JA, Vaidya, N, Mehta, UM, et al. A series of five population-specific Indian brain templates and atlases spanning ages 6–60 years. Hum Brain Mapp. 2020;41:5164–75.Google Scholar
Tang, Y, Hojatkashani, C, Dinov, ID, Sun, B, Fan, L, Lin, X, et al. The construction of a Chinese MRI brain atlas: a morphometric comparison study between Chinese and Caucasian cohorts. Neuroimage 2010;51:3341.Google Scholar
World Health Organisation. WHO child growth standards: length/height for age, weight-for-age, weight-for-length, weight-for-height and body mass index-for-age, methods and development. Vol. 51. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2006.Google Scholar
Mehrabi, N, Morstatter, F, Saxena, N, Lerman, K, Galstyan, A. A survey on bias and fairness in machine learning. ACM Comput Surv (CSUR) 2021;54:135.Google Scholar
Stein, DJ, He Y, Phillips, A, Sahakian, BJ, Williams, J, Patel, V. Global mental health and neuroscience: potential synergies. Lancet Psychiatr. 2015;2:178–85.Google Scholar
Fortin, JP, Cullen, N, Sheline, YI, Taylor, WD, Aselcioglu, I, Cook, PA, et al. Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. Neuroimage. 2018;167:104–20.Google Scholar
Han, S, Northoff, G. Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: a transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9.Google Scholar
Xu, Q, Guo, L, Cheng, J, Wang, M, Geng, Z, Zhu, W, et al. CHIMGEN: a Chinese imaging genetics cohort to enhance cross-ethnic and cross-geographic brain research. Mol Psychiatr. 2020;25.Google Scholar
Tang, Y, Zhao, L, Lou, Y, Shi, Y, Fang, R, Lin, X, et al. Brain structure differences between Chinese and Caucasian cohorts: a comprehensive morphometry study. Hum Brain Mapp. 2018;39.Google Scholar
Fry, A, Littlejohns, TJ, Sudlow, C, Doherty, N, Adamska, L, Sprosen, T, et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. Am J Epidemiol. 2017;186:1026–34.Google Scholar
Henrich, J, Heine, SJ, Norenzayan, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci. 2010;33.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Geographical location of the samples included, their size, and characteristics of their countries. Interval highlighted in the graph (0.1 points for HDI and 5 points for inequality) are the intervals used to express effect sizes. Values of HDI and Inequality (adjusted) for each country included are also reported in Supplementary Table S2.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Brain associations with the Human Development Index. (A) Associations with estimated total intracranial volume. Reliability graph describes jack-knife analyses leaving-one- sample-out and leave-one-country-out. (B) Associations with hemispheric and regional surface. (C) Associations with hippocampal volume.

Figure 2

Table 1. Summary of main results

Figure 3

Figure 3. Brain associations with HDI-adjusted inequality. Associations with (A) estimated total intracranial volume, (B) hemispheric cortical surface area, as well as regional associations, (C) hemispheric and regional cortical thickness, and (D) hippocampal volume. In all panels heat maps represent leave-one-sample-out analyses and leave-one-country-out analyses (excluding all studies of a particular country). Note that no surface area results were significant after controlling for total intracranial volume.

Supplementary material: File

Medel et al. supplementary material

Medel et al. supplementary material
Download Medel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 229.4 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.