Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-plnhv Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2025-08-10T10:28:46.881Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Using the Welfare Quality® framework to develop a welfare assessment protocol for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2025

Niamh Mc Gill
Affiliation:
Dublin Zoo, Phoenix Park, Saint James, Dublin D08 WF88, The Republic of Ireland
Miguel Bueno
Affiliation:
Dublin Zoo, Phoenix Park, Saint James, Dublin D08 WF88, The Republic of Ireland
Neil E. Anderson*
Affiliation:
The Royal (Dick) School of Veterinary Studies and the Roslin Institute, https://ror.org/01nrxwf90University of Edinburgh, Roslin, UK
*
Corresponding author: Neil E. Anderson, Email: neil.anderson@ed.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Good welfare is of inherent value to all captive animals and promotes species conservation objectives. Concern for animal welfare is growing globally, and research shows that animal welfare is a top priority for zoo visitors. There is, therefore, an urgent need for zoos to develop and validate species-specific welfare assessment tools with a shift in focus away from avoiding negative affective states, and towards promoting positive ones. This shift in emphasis requires the development of comprehensive and robust welfare assessment protocols incorporating species-specific indicators. This study aimed to identify and propose welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) that could be used to adapt the EU Welfare Quality® protocol for this species. A literature review was carried out according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) guidelines and the authors followed the principles of a systematic review to identify a comprehensive set of welfare indicators for this species. Overall, 14 animal-based and 16 resource-based indicators are proposed to assess the 12 criteria and four principles of Welfare Quality®. This study represents the first effort to adapt the EU Welfare Quality® protocol to assess captive chimpanzee welfare and illustrates how this protocol can be adapted to develop a taxon-specific welfare assessment tool once species-specific natural history and biology are considered.

Information

Type
Systematic Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Global concern for the welfare of zoo animals has increased dramatically over the last decade, and animal welfare has now become a priority for modern zoos (Blokhuis Reference Blokhuis2008; Phillips et al. Reference Phillips, Izmirli, Aldavood, Alonso, Choe, Hanlon, Handziska, Illmann, Keeling, Kennedy, Lee, Lund, Mejdell, Pelagic and Rehn2012). Despite a commitment to species conservation, it has been argued that housing wild animals in captive environments cannot be justified unless high welfare standards are achieved (Sherwen et al. Reference Sherwen, Hemsworth, Beausoleil, Embury and Mellor2018). To this end, formal evaluation of welfare status is now a core element of zoo accreditation schemes, such as that of the European Association of Zoos and Aquaria.

Animal welfare has been variably defined and is a multidimensional concept that encompasses an animal’s health, physiology, behaviour, and mental state (Mellor et al. Reference Mellor, Patterson-Kane and Stafford2009). Each of these dimensions subsumes several criteria; thus, animal welfare assessment poses a multicriteria evaluation challenge (Botreau et al. Reference Botreau, Bonde, Butterworth, Perny, Bracke, Capdeville and Veissier2007). The Five Domains Model (Mellor & Reid Reference Mellor, Reid, Baker, Jenkin and Mellor1994; Mellor & Beausoleil Reference Mellor and Beausoleil2015) and Welfare Quality® protocol are two frameworks that have been used to assess the welfare of zoo animals and have become the foundation for developing multiple assessment tools in various captive species (McCulloch Reference McCulloch2013). There have been several advances in animal welfare science over the past decade, with two of the most notable being the increasing focus on animal-based indicators and the promotion of a positive affective state (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Sherwen, Robbins, McLelland and Whittaker2022). This shift in emphasis requires the development of comprehensive and robust welfare assessment protocols incorporating species-specific indicators.

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are now listed as ‘Endangered’ on the IUCN red list (Humle Reference Humle, Hill, Wich and Marshall2016), and both in situ and ex situ conservation efforts are increasingly crucial in ensuring stable wild populations and increasing public awareness of the global threats facing these primates. Assessing and improving their welfare in captivity is paramount for several reasons. Firstly, animals have an inherent right to good welfare and should be provided with environments that allow them to live according to their ecological niche and behavioural biology (Rose & Riley Reference Rose and Riley2022). Humans, therefore, have a moral and ethical responsibility to offer animals the opportunity to experience ‘a good life’ as a fundamental need (Mellor Reference Mellor2016). Additionally, poor welfare can reduce an animal’s overall health and reproductive capacity, thus undermining conservation efforts. Captive animals with compromised welfare are also more likely to exhibit abnormal behaviours, thereby weakening their educational value for zoo visitors. (Skovlund et al. Reference Skovlund, Kirchner, Moos, Alsted, Manteca, Tallo-Parra, Stelvig and Forkman2021).

The Welfare Quality® framework is one of the most widely accepted and comprehensive protocols for assessing animal welfare (Botreau et al. Reference Botreau, Bonde, Butterworth, Perny, Bracke, Capdeville and Veissier2007). This framework was developed by a European research project that aimed to provide a standardised, science-based approach for evaluating the welfare of farm animals (Blokhuis Reference Blokhuis2013). Welfare Quality® uses four welfare principles, which reflect four primary areas of concern as agreed upon by the Welfare Quality® scientists. These principles are ‘Good feeding’, ‘Good housing’, ‘Good health’, and ‘Appropriate behaviour’, comparable to the four functional domains of the Five Domains model. Each of these principles comprises several more detailed aspects, and so two to four criteria were defined within each of the four principles, creating the twelve Welfare Quality® criteria that exist today. Great emphasis was placed on this set of criteria being exhaustive, minimal, readable, agreed upon by stakeholders, and independent of one another (Botreau et al. Reference Botreau, Bonde, Butterworth, Perny, Bracke, Capdeville and Veissier2007). Welfare scientists also agreed early in the development process that these twelve welfare criteria should be applicable across all species and situations (Blokhuis Reference Blokhuis2013), although the relative importance of each would likely vary between species and circumstances. The Welfare Quality® protocol was chosen as the framework for this study due to its widely accepted structure, comprehensive design, and potential for application to other species in managed care settings (Clegg et al. Reference Clegg, Borger-Turner and Eskelinen2015; Salas et al. Reference Salas, Manteca, Abáigar, Delclaux, Enseñat, Martínez-Nevado, Quevedo and Fernández-Bellon2018; Benn et al. Reference Benn, McLelland and Whittaker2019). As described by Mononen et al. (Reference Mononen, Møller, Hansen, Hovland, Koistinen, Lidfors, Malmkvist, Vinke and Ahola2012) in their adaptation of Welfare Quality® to farmed foxes (Vulpes lagopus, V. vulpes) and mink (Neovison vison), although some welfare indicators from the farm animal protocols may be suitable for many species, the most appropriate welfare indicators for each species must be identified. Botreau et al. (Reference Botreau, Gaborit and Veissier2012) emphasised the importance of the principles and criteria remaining consistent when applying this protocol to other species, and so the welfare assessment for captive chimpanzees incorporates all four principles and twelve criteria of Welfare Quality®. The current work builds upon the recent comprehensive review by Angley et al. (Reference Angley, Vale and Cronin2024), which examined the relationship between the care provided to captive chimpanzees (resource-based indicators) and the resulting impact on their welfare (animal-based indicators).

Despite the need to assess captive chimpanzee welfare robustly and reliably, there have been few efforts to collectively define a set of validated indicators in this species. This study aimed to identify valid and feasible welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees and represents the first effort to adapt the Welfare Quality® protocol to this species.

Materials and methods

Introduction

A three-step process was employed in this study to adapt the Welfare Quality® protocol for captive chimpanzees:

  • Step 1 – A brief review examined how this protocol was previously adapted for five other species in managed care;

  • Step 2 – A literature review was conducted to identify a comprehensive list of welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees; and

  • Step 3 – Valid, reliable, and feasible welfare indicators for chimpanzees were then assigned to each criterion of the Welfare Quality® protocol.

Step 1 – Review of Welfare Quality® protocols adapted for other zoo species

Welfare Quality® has thus far been adapted for four other species in managed care: bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) (Clegg et al. Reference Clegg, Borger-Turner and Eskelinen2015); dorcas gazelles (Gazella dorcas) (Salas et al. Reference Salas, Manteca, Abáigar, Delclaux, Enseñat, Martínez-Nevado, Quevedo and Fernández-Bellon2018); pygmy blue-tongue skink (Tiliqua adelaidensis) (Benn et al. Reference Benn, McLelland and Whittaker2019); and farmed foxes and mink (Mononen et al. Reference Mononen, Møller, Hansen, Hovland, Koistinen, Lidfors, Malmkvist, Vinke and Ahola2012). The first step in developing a protocol for captive chimpanzees was to review how this has already been adapted for other species. These Welfare Quality® protocols were reviewed, and the indicators chosen to assess each criterion were tabulated. The authors also chose to include the Welfare Quality® indicators outlined for dairy cattle (Welfare Quality® 2009) in this table, as the Welfare Quality® protocol developed for farm animal production served as the starting point for these adapted protocols. This review aimed to examine the developmental structure and considerations described by each author relevant to adapting the protocol to a novel species. Additionally, the authors sought to compare the indicators used to assess each criterion in these protocols to identify potential welfare indicators for consideration for the adapted Welfare Quality® protocol for captive chimpanzees.

Step 2 – Identification of welfare indicators for chimpanzees

Search strategy

The second step in adapting this protocol for captive chimpanzees was to broadly review the literature to identify a comprehensive set of welfare indicators for this species. A literature review using a systematic methodology was undertaken in April 2022. This review was carried out according to the PRISMA (Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analysis) guidelines and followed the principles of a systematic review to carry out an extensive search and apply inclusion and exclusion criteria. However, this review should not be considered ‘systematic’ as the authors worked alone under time constraints. Relevant studies were identified from searches of ‘all years’ on the following bibliographic databases:

  • CAB Abstracts (OVID): 1973 – present;

  • Medline (OVID): 1946 – present;

  • Web of Science Core Collection (Web of Science): 1900 – present;

  • BIOSIS Citation Index (Web of Science): 1926 – present;

  • Zoological Record (Web of Science): 1864 – present; and

  • Scopus (Scopus): 1788 – present.

A three-step search strategy was employed. Firstly, to develop a comprehensive search strategy, relevant keywords were identified by identifying and tabulating the keywords and index terms of relevant literature already in the authors’ possession. Scoping searches were then carried out to test these keywords and identify those which returned the most relevant results. Finally, searches were carried out using a combination of all identified and relevant keyword terms relating to captive chimpanzee welfare as outlined below (Table 1). The same keywords were used throughout all database searches, with minor variations using the relevant database thesaurus — MeSH or Index terms. Boolean operators (“OR,” “AND”) and truncation (*) were used to identify all relevant articles and records (see Appendix 1 in Supplementary material for bibliographic tables).

Table 1. Keywords used in searches of six reference databases to identify relevant articles to construct a comprehensive list of welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees

Eligibility criteria

Inclusion criteria

Only publications that met all the following criteria were reviewed for the extraction of welfare indicators:

  1. (i) the paper was specific to chimpanzees in captivity (to include those in sanctuaries, zoos, and laboratory settings), and chimpanzees were the principal subject of interest;

  2. (ii) the publication was available to the authors in full;

  3. (iii) the publication was in English;

  4. (iv) the publication was in a peer-reviewed journal; and

  5. (v) the publication assessed the welfare, behaviour, physiology or physical condition of a captive chimpanzee at a point in time.

Exclusion criteria

Papers were excluded from the review if they did not meet all the above inclusion criteria or were review articles. Additionally, an exclusion criterion was applied to all studies examining infant and maternal behaviour, health, or nutrition, as although normal maternal and infant behaviour is highly relevant to welfare, these behaviours are likely to be specific to a particular life or developmental stage and are therefore not suitable as general welfare indicators. Finally, an exclusion was also applied to all studies relating to personality assessment due to the complexity of the analysis of such an indicator.

Study selection and indicator extraction

Search results were uploaded to Covidence systematic review software (Veritas Health Innovation, Melbourne, VIC, Australia), and duplicates were removed. The authors then used this software to screen the titles and abstracts of the studies for inclusion against the selection criteria outlined above and the full-text records sought for retrieval were then imported into EndNote 20 (Clarivate Analytics, PA, USA). These were then further assessed through full-text review, and literature not meeting all the eligibility criteria was excluded. The full texts of included articles were then reviewed and identified welfare indicators were captured and categorised using a table created in Microsoft Excel® (Microsoft® Corporation 2018).

Categorisation of indicators

Consistent with the Welfare Quality® approach, the primary aim in adapting this protocol for captive chimpanzees was to prioritise the use of animal-based (or welfare outcome-based) indicators over resource-based (or input-based) indicators, where feasible. Indicators identified from the literature search were therefore categorised as ‘animal-based’ or ‘resource-based’. Following on from the groupings adopted by Truelove et al. (Reference Truelove, Martin, Langford and Leach2020) in their paper on laboratory-housed macaque (Macaca mulatta) welfare, indicators related to the environment, enrichment, and management practices were categorised as ‘resource-based’ indicators, while those related to the animal’s physical health, and the behavioural and physiological response to the environment were categorised as ‘animal-based’ indicators.

Step 3 - Creation of a Welfare Quality® protocol for chimpanzees

All potential welfare indicators identified in Steps one and two were reviewed for validity, feasibility, and reliability by further consulting the literature identified in the review. In selecting the welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees, the authors assessed the relative strength of the available evidence for each indicator by evaluating factors such as the number of supporting studies, the research design, the statistical significance and effect sizes of the findings, and the consistency of results across publications. Indicators backed by a larger body of high-quality, consistent evidence were chosen over those with weaker or more conflicting support in the literature.

In line with Welfare Quality®, one primary aim in adapting this protocol for captive chimpanzees was to incorporate as many animal-based indicators as possible instead of resource-based ones. Where a valid, reliable, and feasible animal-based indicator could not be identified, a feasible input-based indicator that corresponded well with the animal-based indicators was chosen (Welfare Quality® 2009; pp 21–22). As mentioned, Botreau et al. (Reference Botreau, Gaborit and Veissier2012) emphasised the importance of the principles and criteria remaining consistent when applying this protocol to other species, and so the welfare assessment for captive chimpanzees incorporates all four principles and twelve criteria of Welfare Quality®. In their development of the C-Well protocol to assess captive bottlenose dolphin welfare, Clegg et al. (Reference Clegg, Borger-Turner and Eskelinen2015) adapted the Welfare Quality® criterion of ‘Ease of movement’ to instead focus on an ‘Appropriate environment’, and the authors of the current study have taken a similar approach. By renaming this criterion as ‘Appropriate environment’, the authors aim to capture a broader range of environmental and social factors that are particularly relevant to the welfare of captive chimpanzees, given their advanced cognitive and social capabilities. This shift in focus allows the adapted protocol to more comprehensively assess the suitability of the captive setting for meeting the complex needs of this endangered primate species.

Results

Step 1 - Review of Welfare Quality® protocols adapted for other zoo species

See Table 2.

Table 2. The Welfare Quality® protocols for five species in managed care were reviewed, and the indicators chosen to assess each criterion were tabulated below to identify common indicators used across all species

Step 2 - Identification and categorisation of welfare indicators for chimpanzees

Literature review

One hundred and one unique welfare indicators were extracted from 164 peer-reviewed papers Figure 1. The articles were published in 28 different journals, with most articles being found in Zoo Biology, American Journal of Primatology, Applied Animal Behaviour Science, Animals, Primates, and the Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science. The articles were published by researchers in 13 countries, with 60% of the studies conducted by researchers in the USA. The included publications were published between 1982 and 2022.

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram outlining the results of the sourcing of literature for the review of welfare assessment for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).

Categorisation of indicators

A list of 101 potential indicators of captive chimpanzee welfare was generated, and each indicator was then categorised as an ‘animal-’ or ‘resource-based’ indicator. Indicators related to the environment, enrichment, and management practices were categorised as ‘resource’ (input) indicators, while those related to the animal’s physical health and the behavioural and physiological response to the environment were categorised as ‘animal’ (output) indicators. The 101 welfare indicators comprised 48 resource- and 53 animal-based indicators (Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3. Resource-based welfare indicators extracted from full texts of included studies were categorised as relating to the environment, enrichment or management practices

Table 4. Animal -based welfare indicators extracted from full texts of included studies were categorised as relating to health, behaviour or physiology

Step 3 - Creation of a Welfare Quality® protocol for chimpanzees

The rationale for selecting the indicators used to evaluate each of the twelve criteria is explained in later sections. In total, 30 indicators were chosen, 14 of which were animal-based (47%), with eight of the twelve criteria including at least one animal-based indicator (Table 5).

Table 5. The 12 criteria and 30 indicators chosen to create the adapted Welfare Quality® protocol for captive chimpanzees

R Resource-based Indicator.

A Animal-based Indicator.

Good feeding

C1. Absence of prolonged hunger

Hunger is a subjective sensation of appetite and is difficult to assess in non-human animals, but well-designed indicators can suggest probable hunger or satiety (Dawkins Reference Dawkins2006; Clegg et al. Reference Clegg, Borger-Turner and Eskelinen2015). Following the Welfare Quality® model, the indicators proposed to assess this criterion and evaluate both the degree of detail included in dietary records as well as the body condition of the animal, as further discussed below.

The diet of captive chimpanzees has been associated with multiple conditions, including obesity, cardiac, dental, and gastrointestinal disease (Obanda et al. Reference Obanda, Omondi and Chiyo2014), as well as contributing to abnormal behavioural repertoires, including regurgitation and reingestion (Cabana et al. Reference Cabana, Jasmi and Maguire2018). The provision of a suitable diet, as assessed by dietary records, is thus of the utmost importance to captive chimpanzee management and encompasses both appropriate dietary composition and presentation. The nutrient composition of foods consumed by wild chimpanzees is high in fibre and low in water-soluble carbohydrates and lipids (Conklin-Brittain et al. Reference Conklin-Brittain, Wrangham and Hunt1998). Studies have demonstrated that as the proportion of dietary water-soluble carbohydrates decreases, the neutral detergent fibre (NDF) fraction increases, significantly influencing chimpanzee behaviour; inactivity and abnormal behaviour patterns decreased while locomotion, foraging, exploratory and affiliative social behaviours increased (Cabana et al. Reference Cabana, Jasmi and Maguire2018). In addition to diet composition, food presentation is vital to captive chimpanzee management. Wild chimpanzees spend a large proportion of their time (22.5–30.7%) engaged in feeding and foraging behaviour (Hockings et al. Reference Hockings, Anderson and Matsuzawa2009) and providing abundant foraging and feeding opportunities likely represents a source of social and cognitive stimulation, reducing inactivity and the performance of some abnormal behaviours (Celli et al. Reference Celli, Tomonaga, Udono, Teramoto and Nagano2003). Research by Bloomsmith and Lambeth (Reference Bloomsmith and Lambeth1995) has also shown that unpredictable feeding schedules reduced inactivity levels before feeding while feeding whole items may increase feeding time and replicate wild manipulatory behaviours in chimpanzees. A good welfare score would thus be assigned if a chimpanzee’s dietary records reflected the current state of knowledge in this area. While this indicator does not directly assess the animal’s physical condition or feeding behaviour, it evaluates the comprehensiveness and quality of the dietary records maintained by the facility. Importantly, this indicator also serves to assess the appropriateness of the diet offered to the chimpanzees and looks for evidence that the diet provided is nutritionally appropriate and aligned with the natural foraging and feeding behaviours of chimpanzees.

Body Condition Scoring (BCS) is a well-established and widely used welfare tool that can be used to assess animal health. Obesity is a common problem when managing captive chimpanzees (Reamer et al. Reference Reamer, Webb, Jones, Thiele, Haller, Schapiro, Lambeth and Hanley2020), and a study by Obanda et al. (Reference Obanda, Omondi and Chiyo2014) highlighted similarities between humans and chimpanzees concerning obesity and the subsequent risk of developing inflammatory disease. Frequent assessment of body condition should therefore be an integral part of health management in captive chimpanzees, and caretakers should be able to quickly identify and assess overweight or obese individuals. Reamer et al. (Reference Reamer, Webb, Jones, Thiele, Haller, Schapiro, Lambeth and Hanley2020) developed a ten-point scale for non-invasive observational ratings and this has been validated against actual weights and skin fold measurements from sedated chimpanzees.

C2. Absence of prolonged thirst

The continuous availability of water, the number of water-points, and the cleanliness of water-points were considered suitable indicators to assess this criterion, but they reflect the challenge in finding a feasible animal-based indicator (Botreau et al. Reference Botreau, Veissier and Perny2009). Indicators to assess dehydration, such as plasma osmolarity, involve taking a blood sample and thus do not satisfy the requirements for a welfare assessment to be non-invasive, feasible to apply, and undemanding on resources (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Sherwen, Robbins, McLelland and Whittaker2022). Chimpanzees should have easy access to multiple functioning water-points, and the number provided should consider the group size, social dynamics, and habitat design. Water-points should be cleaned regularly and free of crusts of dirt (for example, faeces) and decayed food residue with a good welfare score assigned where water is readily available in clean, functioning water-points to all troop members.

Good housing

C3. Comfort around resting

Chimpanzees build nests for sleeping by combining tree branches and vegetation (Goodall Reference Goodall1962; Koops et al. Reference Koops, McGrew, Matsuzawa and Knapp2012), and these nests serve various functions, including shelter from climatic conditions, defence against predators, and comfort for sleep or rest (Pruetz et al. Reference Pruetz, Fulton, Marchant, McGrew, Schiel and Waller2008; Stewart & Pruetz Reference Stewart and Pruetz2013). Captive chimpanzees may be provided with materials for nest-building, including hay, woodchips, blankets, and paper (Brent Reference Brent1992), or with intact browse materials for constructing a more typical nest (Videan Reference Videan2006). It is worth mentioning at this point that there is a need to distinguish between a behaviour that is ‘natural’ and one that is ‘motivated’ in an individual as there is limited evidence that ‘natural behaviours’ reliably either lead to or signify better well-being, unless an individual values that behaviour (Yeates Reference Yeates2018). For example, it is not expected that animals enjoy flight, sickness or antagonistic behaviours commonly encountered in ‘natural’ environments (Hermansen et al. Reference Hermansen, Strudsholm and Horsted2004; Miao et al. Reference Miao, Glatz and Ru2004), while animals may find enjoyment in unnatural affiliative interactions with human caregivers (Yeates & Main Reference Yeates and Main2008). Thus, it is impossible to determine a priori what ‘natural behaviours’ improve well-being (Yeates Reference Yeates2010).

Nesting behaviour forms a fundamental and valued part of a chimpanzee’s behavioural repertoire (Fruth & Hohmann Reference Fruth and Hohmann1996), and thus the provision of nesting materials has the potential to impact the welfare of captive chimpanzees. Multiple studies have demonstrated this, including Brent (Reference Brent1992), who found that supplying woodchip bedding to juvenile chimpanzees significantly reduced their abnormal behaviours. More recently, Anderson et al. (Reference Anderson, Amarasekaran and Riba2021) demonstrated that an individual’s welfare was highest when presented with destructible, synthetic nesting materials. It is also worth noting that nest-building ability has been shown to depend on early rearing conditions and that bed-building techniques differ between wild- and captive-born chimpanzees (Bernstein Reference Bernstein1962; Videan Reference Videan2006).

Nests can be built on the ground or at a height, and so the provision of multilevel resting sites is the second indicator chosen to assess comfort around resting in this species. Night nests are typically constructed in a tree (Reynolds & Reynolds Reference Reynolds and Reynolds1965), while day nests are often at ground level (Goodall Reference Goodall1962; Brownlow et al. Reference Brownlow, Plumptre, Reynolds and Ward2001). Samson and Hunt (Reference Samson and Hunt2014) investigated the physical comfort levels of tree and ground nests and reported that ground nests could be advantageous owing to reduced energy expenditure and a homeostatic microclimate. Chimpanzees construct a new nest each night, and a study by Lock and Anderson (Reference Lock and Anderson2013) concluded that night-time spatial arrangements and individual preferences for specific sleeping areas were broadly comparable to nesting patterns reported in free-living chimpanzees. This study suggested that in the interests of captive ape welfare, habitats should include multilevel nesting areas and a choice of multiple sleeping sites.

C4. Thermal comfort

Chimpanzees inhabit various environments, from the semi-arid savanna to the warm and humid tropical forests (Pruetz & Bertolani Reference Pruetz and Bertolani2007), implying behavioural flexibility to adjust to various habitats. Two indicators were developed to assess thermal comfort, adapted from the Welfare Quality® model: availability of shelter and shade and appropriate environmental monitoring records.

Chimpanzee activity budget, terrestriality, and sun exposure have been found to be related to environmental temperatures (Kosheleff & Anderson Reference Kosheleff and Anderson2009). Studies investigating how chimpanzees vary their arboreal versus terrestrial budgets due to environmental temperatures have yielded conflicting results, with Kosheleff and Anderson (Reference Kosheleff and Anderson2009) concluding that chimpanzee terrestriality correlated negatively with temperature, while Takemoto (Reference Takemoto2004) found increased terrestrial activities with higher ambient temperatures. Nevertheless, it is apparent that chimpanzees exhibit behavioural flexibility and adapt to diverse conditions by employing various behavioural strategies for thermoregulation, allowing them to respond to thermal challenges associated with housing them away from their natural environment. Examples of such strategies come from studies such as that by Pruetz and Bertolani (Reference Pruetz and Bertolani2007), who demonstrated that savannah chimpanzees use caves to avoid heat in an exposed environment, while Duncan and Pillay (Reference Duncan and Pillay2013) concluded that chimpanzees in captivity adopted a sun-avoidance strategy, with ultraviolet radiation and humidity levels predicting shade utilisation patterns. Other studies assessing space use and environmental complexity have also demonstrated the tendency of captive apes to spend time close to buildings (Ogden et al. Reference Ogden, Finlay and Maple1990), which may relate to the thermoregulatory benefits of microclimates around these structures (Stoinski et al. Reference Stoinski, Hoff and Maple2001). Thus, the best welfare score for thermal comfort would be assigned to enclosures with multiple shaded and sheltered areas available (for example, climbing structures and platforms), as these areas create a choice of differing microclimates for thermoregulation (Swaisgood & Shepherdson Reference Swaisgood and Shepherdson2005).

In colder countries, captive chimpanzees may have to contend with low environmental temperatures and the provision of heated indoor enclosures and ‘hot spots’ are usually necessary. The highest welfare score for the second Welfare Quality® indicator relating to thermal comfort would evaluate environmental monitoring records and protocols to assess and prevent enclosure temperatures outside of chimpanzees’ Preferred Optimal Temperature Zone, with a remediation process outlined should temperatures fall outside of this range.

C5. Appropriate environment

This was the sole criterion altered from the Welfare Quality® protocol and was adapted to account for chimpanzees’ complex and advanced social structure and intellect. Like Clegg et al. (Reference Clegg, Borger-Turner and Eskelinen2015) in the adaption of Welfare Quality® for captive bottlenose dolphins, this criterion was changed from ‘Ease of movement’ to ‘Appropriate environment’.

There has been an increasing focus on enhancing functionally appropriate captive environments (FACEs) for chimpanzees, such that natural ranging behaviours and other valued species-typical behaviours are promoted (Webb et al. Reference Webb, Hau and Schapiro2018a). Environments inhabited by wild animals often serve as the ‘gold standard’ when judging captive environments, and an abiding critique of captivity is the degree to which it differs to a wild environment, particularly concerning the space available for animals to range and travel. Animal density or space per animal is thus an essential focus of these FACEs, and studies on space per chimpanzee have demonstrated that higher animal densities are related to increased social tension and reduced allogrooming (Aureli & Waal Reference Aureli and Waal2000). Consequently, chimpanzees have been shown to use conflict-avoidance and tension-reduction strategies to cope with reduced space (Aureli & De Waal Reference Aureli and FBM1997). Chimpanzees have also been shown to engage in more abnormal and self-directed behaviours when confined at an increased density (Duncan et al. Reference Duncan, Jones, Lierop and Pillay2013). Thus, the best welfare score for spatial density would be assigned where a behaviourally relevant minimum size threshold was considered. This is supported by literature that suggests that welfare is enhanced when space is increased beyond a suboptimal minimum, with increasing space beyond this leading to smaller and smaller changes in behaviour and increases in welfare (Ross et al. Reference Ross, Wagner, Schapiro, Hau and Lukas2011b; Reamer et al. Reference Reamer, Talbot, Hopper, Mareno, Hall, Brosnan, Lambeth and Schapiro2015).

The topography of zoo enclosures has been associated with animal well-being, with novelty and enclosure complexity regarded as crucial to improving welfare (Jensvold et al. Reference Jensvold, Sanz, Fouts and Fouts2001; Hosey Reference Hosey2005). Such complexity is offered through spatial topography, such as vertical space (Caws et al. Reference Caws, Wehnelt and Aureli2008), available spaces (Bettinger et al. Reference Bettinger, Wallis and Carter1994; Ross et al. Reference Ross, Calcutt, Schapiro and Hau2011a; Herrelko et al. Reference Herrelko, Buchanan-Smith and Vick2015), escape routes (Ross & Lukas Reference Ross and Lukas2006), and opportunities to exhibit control (Videan et al. Reference Videan, Fritz, Schwandt, Smith and Howell2005b; Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Hasenau and Bohm2017; Webb et al. Reference Webb, Hau and Schapiro2018b). Access to outdoor space is a further indicator purported to influence captive chimpanzee behaviour, as it results in significant volumetric expansion, increased naturalism, and increased enclosure complexity (Ross et al. Reference Ross, Wagner, Schapiro, Hau and Lukas2011b). Thus, the appropriateness of the environment could be assessed by these three indicators, whereby more topographically complex and dynamic enclosures with access to outdoor space achieve better welfare scores.

Good health

C6. Absence of injuries

Socially housed chimpanzees are at risk of increased frequency of injury owing to intraspecific aggression, as exists in the wild. Therefore, captive management must balance the social needs of the animals with the desire to curtail wounding owing to natural intraspecific aggression (Williams et al. Reference Williams, Nash, Scarry, Videan and Fritz2010). As agonistic behaviour is a natural part of the chimpanzee’s behavioural repertoire, its elimination from the captive setting is an unrealistic and perhaps undesirable objective. It is also worth noting that social conflict provides cognitive stimulation, variability, and problem-solving opportunities (de Waal et al. Reference de Waal, Nishida, McGrew, Marler, Pickford and de Waal1992). However, captive chimpanzees engage in more aggression than their wild counterparts, and a number of studies have identified factors that influence agonistic encounters and wounding in captivity, including: (1) introductions of new individuals (Alford et al. Reference Alford, Bloomsmith, Keeling and Beck1995); (2) the age and sex of the individual (Baker Reference Baker2000); (3) group composition (Ross et al. Reference Ross, Bloomsmith, Bettinger and Wagner2009); (4) social stability (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Lambeth and Alford1992); (5) the presence of in-oestrus females (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Lambeth and Alford1992); and (6) the level of human activity (Lambeth et al. Reference Lambeth, Bloomsmith and Alford1997). Management policies can be formulated to reduce the risk of serious wounding by identifying the main factors influencing wounding aggression in captive chimpanzees. The general recommendation that emerges from the literature cited above is that captive management should focus on maintaining long-term, stable social groups and minimise changes in group composition as Williams et al. (Reference Williams, Nash, Scarry, Videan and Fritz2010) demonstrated that wounding aggression decreased in groups with long-term social stability. Two indicators were chosen to assess the ‘Absence of injuries’ criterion, reflecting the main risk of injury to captive individuals, which is intraspecific wounding. The highest welfare score for the resource-based indicators would be assigned to a stable social grouping, while the highest score for the animal-based indicator would be assigned to a chimpanzee with no evidence of wounding.

C7. Absence of disease

Chimpanzees, like humans, can be affected by a wide range of diseases, and the Welfare Quality® indicators chosen to assess this criterion focus on the most prevalent chronic and acute diseases of captive chimpanzees.

The most frequent cause of death in captive chimpanzees has been repeatedly confirmed as cardiovascular disease (Kumar et al. Reference Kumar, Laurence, Owston, Sharp, Williams, Lanford, Hubbard and Dick2017; Raindi et al. Reference Raindi, Rees, Hirschfeld, Wright, Dobbs, Moitti, White, Stahl, Martin, Redrobe, Hughes, Liptovszky, Baiker and Grant2022; Ross et al. Reference Ross, Joshi, Terio and Gamble2022), with a higher percentage of chimpanzees affected by this condition when compared to humans (Seiler et al. Reference Seiler, Dick, Guardado-Mendoza, Vandeberg, Williams, Mubiru and Hubbard2009). Interstitial fibrosing cardiomyopathy is the most common pathology identified, and sudden collapse and death are thought to result from defective conduction or sudden cardiac arrhythmia (Baldessari et al. Reference Baldessari, Snyder, Ahrens and Murnane2013). Whereas humans typically suffer ischaemic myocardial injury due to occlusive vascular disease built up over a lifetime, chimpanzees develop myocardial fibrosis without the vascular component (Varki et al. Reference Varki, Anderson, Herndon, Pham, Gregg, Cheriyan, Murphy, Strobert, Fritz, Else and Varki2009). Various initiatives, such as the Ape Heart Project based at Twycross Zoo and the International Primate Heart Project, have been established to further our understanding of heart disease in great apes, with causal factors remaining poorly understood despite extensive research (Strong et al. Reference Strong, Moittie, Sheppard, Liptovszky, White, Redrobe, Cobb and Baiker2020).

Our understanding of what other diseases affect captive chimpanzees derives from various studies retrospectively examining mortality in captive chimpanzee groups over long periods. Ross et al. (Reference Ross, Joshi, Terio and Gamble2022) analysed the cause of death of 224 chimpanzees at 42 accredited zoos over 25 years and concluded that younger individuals are particularly at risk of trauma and infectious disease, while degenerative conditions continue to be the leading cause of death for adult chimpanzees. Ross et al. (Reference Ross, Joshi, Terio and Gamble2022) examined fifteen years of records relating to the deaths of 268 chimpanzees at a primate facility in New Mexico and determined that gastrointestinal disease was the primary cause of death in 41% of the cases there. Hubbard et al. (Reference Hubbard, Lee and Eichberg1991) and Kumar et al. (Reference Kumar, Laurence, Owston, Sharp, Williams, Lanford, Hubbard and Dick2017) reviewed post mortem examination records for a colony of chimpanzees at The Southwest Biomedical Foundation in Texas and identified cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, and respiratory disease as the leading causes of death in this colony. An additional study conducted by Nunamaker et al. (Reference Nunamaker, Lee and Lammey2012) sought to characterise the prevalence of chronic disease in a colony of sixteen geriatric female chimpanzees and documented a 23% incidence of renal disease. Thus, five animal-based indicators were chosen to assess an ‘Absence of disease’, reflecting the most commonly recognised health concerns of captive chimpanzees: cardiovascular disease, respiratory disease, gastrointestinal disease, musculoskeletal and renal disease. A single resource-based indicator reflects the formulation of a comprehensive preventative healthcare programme, with routine parasitology, regular review of medical histories, daily observations, and opportunistic physical and diagnostic examinations as core components of this.

C8. Absence of pain induced by management procedures

Welfare Quality® includes a criterion for assessing pain associated with management procedures, such as blood draws, injections, and capture for immobilisation. In addition, captive chimpanzees are commonly moved within and among enclosures for feeding, training, veterinary care, or in emergencies where rapid access or separation is needed; thus, the ability to achieve this with voluntary animal participation is critical to animal health, safety, and welfare. The indicator proposed to assess this criterion is whether a behavioural management programme (BMP), such as positive reinforcement training (PRT), is in operation.

Eliciting co-operation from captive animals during necessary routine procedures through PRT is desirable, rather than forcing compliance using restraints or aversive measures (Reinhardt Reference Reinhardt2003). Multiple studies have now been published which have variably focused on training captive chimpanzees to co-operate during routine medical procedures (Videan et al. Reference Videan, Fritz, Murphy, Borman, Smith and Howell2005a), moving cages (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Stone and Laule1998), blood and urine collection (Schapiro et al. Reference Schapiro, Bloomsmith and Laule2003; Reamer et al. Reference Reamer, Haller, Thiele, Freeman, Lambeth and Schapiro2014; Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Neu, Franklin, Griffis and McMillan2015), as well as behavioural management that can reduce social tension during feeding (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Laule, Alford and Thurston1994). In addition to the benefits offered to the animal in terms of stress reduction, the involuntary collection of blood from distressed animals can result in blood samples that are not physiologically representative of the animal’s normal hormone levels during stress-free periods (Hassimoto et al. Reference Hassimoto, Harada and Harada2004). A study by Lambeth et al. (Reference Lambeth, Hau, Perlman, Martino and Schapiro2006) also demonstrated that chimpanzees trained to present for an anaesthetic injection had lower white blood cell counts and blood glucose levels, indicating a reduced physiological stress response. Thus, PRT programmes allow animals to participate actively in interactions that improve veterinary care (Fernandez Reference Fernandez2022).

PRT has additionally been shown to benefit animals by influencing social interactions with conspecifics, serving as a form of enrichment, and reducing undesirable behaviours. Bloomsmith et al. (Reference Bloomsmith, Laule, Alford and Thurston1994) effectively reduced aggression during feeding times using PRT in group-housed chimpanzees. Pomerantz and Terkel (Reference Pomerantz and Terkel2009) used PRT to increase chimpanzee affiliative behaviours outside of training sessions, while they also demonstrated reduced abnormal and stress-related behaviours during these PRT sessions. Thus, the highest welfare scores for this criterion would be assigned to facilities with an established and active behavioural management programme in operation.

Appropriate behaviour

C9. Expression of social behaviours

Chimpanzees live in large, highly complex, multi-male, multi-female social communities with fission-fusion dynamics (Goodall Reference Goodall1986). Thus, they require social interactions with conspecifics to promote the expression of natural social behaviour. Group dynamics are somewhat artificial in captivity due to behavioural management programmes, operational decisions regarding housing, and the limited scope for escape or avoidance. Therefore, the indicators chosen to assess this criterion included group size and systematically measuring for agonistic and affiliative behaviours, like those exhibited by wild chimpanzees.

Humane standards for the long-term maintenance of captive chimpanzees mandate that they are socially housed, with a multitude of studies demonstrating that aggression and abnormal behaviours are higher in socially impoverished conditions (Spijkerman et al. Reference Spijkerman, Dienske, van Hooff and Jens1994; Williams et al. Reference Williams, Nash, Scarry, Videan and Fritz2010; Khan Reference Khan2013). In terms of group size, the National Institutes of Health (NIH) and the Association of Zoos and Aquariums (AZA) recommend that captive chimpanzees are housed in multi-male, multi-female, age-diverse groups of no less than seven individuals (NIH 2013). Neal Webb et al. (Reference Neal Webb, Hau and Schapiro2019) investigated this recommendation by examining the behavioural effects of group size, group age, and percentage of males on the group and found evidence to support the existing group size and composition recommendations. They also demonstrated in this study that chimpanzees living in more age-diverse social groups, with at least 50% males, showed the highest levels of affiliation.

It has been suggested that behavioural assessment, specifically social interactions, may inform welfare assessment more than physiological indicators (Hill & Broom Reference Hill and Broom2009). The comparison of captive chimpanzee behaviour to that of wild conspecifics is often used as a proxy for animal welfare assessment, although Howell and Cheyne (Reference Howell and Cheyne2019) emphasise that all behaviours are stimulus-driven and that good welfare does not necessarily mean that behavioural budgets must reflect those of wild populations. A better approach may be determining if the animal is displaying affiliative behaviours such as allogrooming and social play, indicative of a positive emotional state (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007). Boissy et al. (Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007) suggest that such behaviours may help infer good welfare as they often occur only when other physical needs are met. Furthermore, affiliative interactions have been shown to help alleviate stress in primates (Aureli & Yates Reference Aureli and Yates2010), while Yamanashi et al. (Reference Yamanashi, Nogami, Teramoto, Morimura and Hirata2018) demonstrated how adult-adult social play was used to reduce social tension in multi-male groups.

Conversely, there is little evidence to suggest the ‘normal’ frequency of agonistic behaviour among chimpanzees, but multiple studies demonstrate declining welfare states with the expression of agonistic behaviour in high frequencies (Alford et al. Reference Alford, Bloomsmith, Keeling and Beck1995; Videan & Fritz Reference Videan and Fritz2007; Ross et al. Reference Ross, Bloomsmith, Bettinger and Wagner2009; Fultz et al. Reference Fultz, Jackson-Jewett, Higginbotham and Lane2015; Hosey et al. Reference Hosey, Melfi, Formella, Ward, Tokarski, Brunger, Brice and Hill2016). Intra-specific aggression also often leads to injuries and social stress and has therefore been included to assess this criterion in all the adapted zoo animal Welfare Quality® protocols outlined in Table 2. While agonistic behaviour is an expected and natural part of the chimpanzee’s behavioural repertoire, a higher welfare score would be assigned where agonistic behaviour is observed at lower frequencies.

C10. Expression of other behaviours

For captive chimpanzees, ‘abnormal behaviours’ are considered species-atypical behaviours that are observed either exclusively in captivity or at higher rates in captivity than in the wild (Hopper et al. Reference Hopper, Freeman and Ross2016a). Both types are used as benchmarks for evaluating captive chimpanzee welfare, and several stereotypic behaviours have been empirically linked to elevated stress and suboptimal social and physical environments in primates, thus validating them as reliable indicators of poor welfare (Garner Reference Garner2005). A study by Birkett and Newton-Fisher (Reference Birkett and Newton-Fisher2011) examined the behaviour of forty socially housed captive chimpanzees from six collections in the USA and the UK and concluded that abnormal behaviour is endemic in captive chimpanzee populations, despite enrichment efforts. Spijkerman et al. (Reference Spijkerman, Dienske, van Hooff and Jens1994) followed the behavioural development of ninety chimpanzees and demonstrated that body rocking resulted from the inability to cope with frustrating social circumstances, as well as early maternal separation. Baker and Aureli (Reference Baker and Aureli1997) demonstrated that yawning, a self-directed behaviour related to anxiety, increased during periods of social tension in captive chimpanzees, while self-scratching has been observed to increase in chimpanzees in stressful situations, such as immediately after a conflict (Leavens et al. Reference Leavens, Hopkins and Thomas2004; Koski & Sterck Reference Koski and Sterck2007), and to decrease after reconciliation (Fraser et al. Reference Fraser, Stahl and Aureli2008). However, it is worth noting that a clear link between abnormal behaviour and compromised welfare cannot be found in all studies. Recent research on coprophagy (deliberately eating faeces) by both Jacobson et al. (Reference Jacobson, Ross and Bloomsmith2016b) and Hopper et al. (Reference Hopper, Freeman and Ross2016a), suggested an ambiguous relationship between this ‘abnormal behaviour’ and welfare, as it is associated more frequently with desirable conditions, such as mother-rearing. The first Welfare Quality® indicator chosen to assess the ‘expression of other behaviour’ criterion assesses for the presence or absence of ‘abnormal’ behaviours, with the highest welfare scores assigned to animals not displaying species-atypical behaviour at the time of assessment.

As discussed in the Introduction, welfare science has shifted away from the avoidance of negative affective states, towards the promotion of positive ones (Yeates & Main Reference Yeates and Main2008; Mellor Reference Mellor2012), with Boissy et al. (Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007) even suggesting that the presence of positive affective states may be more relevant to welfare assessment. Therefore, the second indicator chosen to assess this criterion looks for the presence of ‘play and exploratory behaviours’, thus acknowledging the need for new welfare assessment tools to accommodate this shift and include positive welfare indicators where possible. Play behaviour is a valid indicator of positive welfare as it only occurs when all other needs are met (Held & Špinka Reference Held and Špinka2011), while curiosity-driven exploration is a goal-directed behaviour, proposed to be indicative of a positive affective state (Mellor Reference Mellor2015a,Reference Mellorb).

The operation of an environmental enrichment strategy was chosen as the third indicator to assess this criterion. Chimpanzees are highly social and cognitively complex animals, often living in environments that differ substantially from their wild counterparts, and thus appropriate enrichment is required to ensure their psychological well-being (de Groot & Cheyne Reference de Groot and Cheyne2016). The provision of enrichment seeks to encourage behaviours indicative of positive welfare, such as play, affiliation, and exploration (Boissy et al. Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007) while either reducing or decreasing the occurrence of negative behaviours (Tarou & Bashaw Reference Tarou and Bashaw2007; Wallace et al. Reference Wallace, Altschul, Korfer, Benti, Kaeser, Lambeth, Waller and Slocombe2017). Such enrichment can be sensory (Grunauer & Walguarnery Reference Grunauer and Walguarnery2018; Obsen et al. Reference Obsen, Madsen, Sloth, Gintberg, Jensen, Pertoldi, Pagh and Alstrup2021), tactile (Cerrone Reference Cerrone2019), cognitive (Yamanashi et al. Reference Yamanashi, Matsunaga, Shimada, Kado and Tanaka2016), physical (Hopper et al. Reference Hopper, Shender and Ross2016b), or food-related (Padrell et al. Reference Padrell, Amici, Cordoba, Giberga, Broekman, Almagro and Llorente2021).

C11. Good human-animal relationship

The human-animal relationship (HAR) was outlined by Hosey (Reference Hosey2008) as the history of interactions between the human and the animal, which allows each to make predictions about the behaviour of the other. Negative interactions are generally aversive, unpredictable, and rough (Waiblinger et al. Reference Waiblinger, Boivin, Pedersen, Tosi, Janczak, Visser and Jones2006), and Hosey and Melfi (Reference Hosey and Melfi2015) highlighted the negative impact of such interactions on zoo animal welfare. They demonstrated the negative impact of keepers with deficient handling skills and the stressing consequences of poor human-animal relationships when multiple keepers care for the same animal. Conversely, recent work has shown that positively reinforcing and affiliative relationships can enhance welfare, established through positive reinforcement training, considerate handling, familiarity, and increased interactions (Hosey Reference Hosey2008; Brando Reference Brando2012). Positive interactions involve reassuring verbal and considered physical effort (Hemsworth Reference Hemsworth2003), and a significant body of evidence shows that positive human-animal relationships broadly improve captive chimpanzee welfare (Jensvold Reference Jensvold2008; Chelluri et al. Reference Chelluri, Ross and Wagner2013). Baker (Reference Baker2004) demonstrated how increased human interaction led to increased affiliative encounters and reduced abnormal behaviours in a group of zoo-housed chimpanzees, while multiple authors have proposed that these positive interactions can further serve as social enrichment with positive welfare benefits (Jensvold et al. Reference Jensvold, Buckner and Stadtner2010; Claxton Reference Claxton2011; Chelluri et al. Reference Chelluri, Ross and Wagner2013).

Chimpanzees actively participate in daily husbandry schedules, such as shifting enclosures to facilitate cleaning, and these frequent animal-keeper interactions make HAR assessment particularly pertinent as a welfare metric, with two indicators chosen to assess this criterion for Welfare Quality®. The indicator proposed to assess this criterion is the response of a chimpanzee to a caregiver when not under stimulus control, with the highest welfare scores assigned to a positive response and the lowest welfare score assigned in cases of apparent fear avoidance, stress, or overt human-directed aggression.

C12. Positive emotional state

Increasing focus on the area of positive emotions, in combination with the assertion that captive wild animals should experience good welfare and ‘a good life’ (Mellor Reference Mellor2016), is leading to a greater aspiration among welfare scientists to promote positive experiences and enhance long-term positive emotional states. However, as highlighted by Boissy et al. (Reference Boissy, Manteuffel, Jensen, Moe, Spruijt, Keeling, Winckler, Forkman, Dimitrov, Langbein, Bakken, Veissier and Aubert2007), such positive emotional states are usually more subtle, often less expressive, and more difficult to reliably distinguish when compared to negative ones. Qualitative Behavioural Assessment (QBA) is a scientific method that was initially developed by Wemelsfelder et al. (Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2000, Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2001) and has been validated to contribute to the identification of the main dimensions of animal emotional states (Mendl et al. Reference Mendl, Burman and Paul2010; Rutherford et al. Reference Rutherford, Donald, Lawrence and Wemelsfelder2012; Temple et al. Reference Temple, Manteca, Dalmau and Velarde2013; Carreras et al. Reference Carreras, Mainau, Arroyo, Moles, González, Bassols, Dalmau, Faucitano, Manteca and Velarde2016). This tool captures the expressive qualities of animal behaviour by observing an animal’s body language and then scores several behavioural descriptors such as ‘tense’, ‘calm’, ‘playful’, or ‘fearful’. The emotional connotations of these behavioural terms directly relate to animal welfare in that they describe the animals’ experience of their own state (Wemelsfelder et al. Reference Wemelsfelder, Hunter, Mendl and Lawrence2001; Wemelsfelder & Farish Reference Wemelsfelder and Farish2004). Ultimately, this method enables a highly experienced caretaker to capture subtle shifts in behaviour, posture, attitude, expression, or movement and to translate them into quantitative measures that can be analysed statistically.

Discussion

Assigning indicators to the Welfare Quality® criteria

The use of animal-based indicators is preferred over resource-based indicators, as these are assumed to possess a higher validity, given that they are more closely related to the current welfare state of the animal (Blokhuis et al. Reference Blokhuis, Veissier, Miele and Jones2010). The time required to assess animal-based indicators is, however, their primary constraint concerning feasibility (Mulleder et al. Reference Mulleder, Troxler, Laaha and Waiblinger2007; Knierim & Winckler Reference Knierim and Winckler2009). This is exemplified by the Welfare Quality® protocol for dairy cattle, where 60% of the indicators are animal-based but take about 90% of the total on-farm assessment time (de Vries et al. Reference de Vries, Engel, Uijl, van Schaik, Dijkstra, de Boer and Bokkers2013).

As already described, welfare assessment tools should be feasible to apply, non-invasive, and undemanding on resources (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Sherwen, Robbins, McLelland and Whittaker2022). Thus, a welfare assessment tool must strike a balance by incorporating as many animal-based indicators as possible while also including more feasible resource-based indicators that have been validated as reflective of an animal’s welfare state at a point in time (Truelove et al. Reference Truelove, Martin, Langford and Leach2020). As stated by Wolfensohn et al. (Reference Wolfensohn, Shotton, Bowley, Davies, Thompson and Justice2018), from a practical viewpoint, a combination of both resource- and animal-based indicators is likely to promote a more comprehensive assessment of animal welfare while being reasonably practical to implement. Of the 30 indicators chosen for inclusion in the adapted protocol for captive chimpanzees, 14 are animal-based (47%), with eight of the twelve criteria including at least one animal-based indicator. The authors chose exclusively resource-based indicators for the remaining four criteria for reasons relating to the feasibility and validity of the considered animal-based indicators as outlined below.

The second Welfare Quality® criterion focuses on the ‘Absence of prolonged thirst’ and animal-based indicators which were considered to assess this criterion include the ‘pinch test’, the appearance of sunken eyes, or the collection of a blood sample for plasma osmolarity and haematocrit counts. The ‘pinch test’ on a captive chimpanzee would require anaesthesia for handling, so this was excluded for reasons relating to feasibility. The appearance of sunken eyes can only be used to detect dehydration in extreme cases and thus is not appropriate as an indicator to detect the suboptimal provision of water. Finally, plasma osmolarity and haematocrit counts increase when animals are deprived of water (Knowles et al. Reference Knowles, Warriss, Brown, Edwards and Mitchell1995; Pritchard et al. Reference Pritchard, Barr and Whay2006), but their assessment requires the collection of a blood sample which is not consistent with a non-invasive welfare assessment. Thus, favouring resource-based indicators to assess this criterion was justified, given the lack of a well-validated, feasible animal-based alternative.

The indicators proposed to assess the three criteria of ‘Good housing’ were also exclusively resource-based in the adapted chimpanzee Welfare Quality® protocol. For the ‘Thermal comfort’ criterion, animal-based indicators, such as visible shivering and panting, could be regarded as valid signs of compromised welfare, but these indicators depend on the ambient temperatures at the time of assessment and, as such, do not reflect an ongoing welfare state. Consequently, the indicators chosen to assess this criterion focus on offering multiple opportunities for animals to maximise their thermal comfort in extreme weather conditions (i.e. the provision of shade and shelter), as well as an environmental monitoring policy that ensures temperatures in indoor habitats remain in the animals Preferred Optimal Temperature Zone. Similarly, for the ‘Comfort around resting’ criterion, an animal-based indicator related to sleep duration and quality could have been chosen, but again the assessment of this indicator is inconsistent with the stated aims of a welfare assessment tool being feasible and undemanding on resources (Jones et al. Reference Jones, Sherwen, Robbins, McLelland and Whittaker2022).

Additionally, exclusively resource-based indicators were chosen to assess the ‘Appropriate environment’ criterion, although it is worth mentioning that some overlap exists between this criterion and the ‘Expression of social behaviour’ and ‘Expression of other behaviour’ criteria. The ‘Appropriate environment’ relates to the welfare ‘input’, whereby the expression of behaviours is the welfare ‘outcome’ in many situations pertinent to chimpanzee welfare. We know from the literature that abnormal behaviours have been linked empirically to suboptimal physical environments in primates (Nash et al. Reference Nash, Fritz, Alford and Brent1999; Garner Reference Garner2005; Birkett & Newton-Fisher Reference Birkett and Newton-Fisher2011), while the expression of social behaviours is directly correlated with social density (Webb et al. Reference Webb, Hau and Schapiro2019) and space allowance (Neal Webb et al. Reference Neal Webb, Hau and Schapiro2018). Thus, resource-based ‘input’ indicators to assess the environment were assigned to ‘Appropriate environment’ while the ‘welfare outcome’ indicators of behaviour were assessed under the criteria examining the expression of social and other behaviours. Although it could be argued that this contradicts the aim of Welfare Quality® to have minimal and mutually independent criteria, all three criteria were included as there are many situations whereby the expression of social and other behaviours in this species may relate to factors unconnected to the environment (for example, the presence of new personnel).

Finally, the authors acknowledge that the strategy of relying upon comprehensive record and management programme evaluations as indicators may introduce some limitations in terms of practicality and accuracy, compared to more direct animal-based measures. However, these indicators can still provide valuable insights into how well the captive chimpanzees are cared for, which has been shown in the literature to have a direct impact on their welfare. For example, numerous studies have demonstrated that the quality and appropriateness of the diet provided, as reflected in dietary records, can significantly influence the physical health, behaviour, and overall welfare of chimpanzees in captivity (Conklin-Brittain et al. Reference Conklin-Brittain, Wrangham and Hunt1998; Cabana et al. Reference Cabana, Jasmi and Maguire2018). Similarly, the presence and implementation of a comprehensive behavioural management programme, including positive reinforcement training, has been linked to reduced stress, increased affiliative interactions, and other indicators of positive welfare in this species (Bloomsmith et al. Reference Bloomsmith, Laule, Alford and Thurston1994; Pomerantz & Terkel Reference Pomerantz and Terkel2009). By evaluating the content and implementation of these care-related records and programmes, the welfare assessment protocol can provide a more holistic understanding of the captive environment and management practices, and how they contribute to the animals’ overall well-being.

The use of ‘abnormal behaviour’ as a welfare indicator

It is worth also discussing the use of ‘abnormal behaviour’ as an indicator of negative welfare, as this relationship is often complicated due to the complex mechanisms underlying many stereotypies (Jacobson et al. Reference Jacobson, Ross and Bloomsmith2016b). Mason and Latham (Reference Mason and Latham2004) suggest that these abnormal behaviours can reflect behavioural flexibility, as animals attempt to satisfy a motivation to perform natural behaviours in captivity. They also suggest that, over time, this behaviour could be reduced to a habit that is no longer linked to the harmful context during which it developed, thus not necessarily reflecting the current welfare state of an animal. Additionally, certain stereotypic behaviours are thought to be spread via social learning (Jacobson et al. Reference Jacobson, Ross and Bloomsmith2016a), with factors such as relatedness and rank implicated (Hopper et al. Reference Hopper, Freeman and Ross2016a). Recent research on coprophagy by Hopper et al. (Reference Hopper, Freeman and Ross2016a) suggests its link to poor welfare is ambiguous, while the classification of self-plucking and ‘regurgitation and reingestion’ as ‘stereotypic’ behaviours is also contested (Baker & Easley Reference Baker and Easley1996; Hosey & Skyner Reference Hosey and Skyner2007). Thus, the extent to which chimpanzee behaviours deemed as ‘abnormal’ are actual manifestations of stress is debated, and a holistic understanding of the aetiologies of these behaviours is critical to determining whether they can be used as valid and reliable indicators of compromised welfare in captive chimpanzees. Furthermore, it highlights the importance of understanding the individual’s abnormal behavioural repertoire as if individuals can reliably vary in this repertoire, it seems plausible that such variation may also be apparent in the abnormal behaviours linked to behavioural signals of stress.

Scoring system

The Welfare Quality® scoring system requires the sequential aggregation of welfare indicators into 12 criteria scores, grouped further into four principle scores, and finally into an overall welfare categorisation as outlined in Figure 2. This scoring is a highly complex task for several reasons. Firstly, scoring the various indicators requires interpretation and balancing, and a degree of subjectivity is inevitable when weighting their relative importance (Spoolder et al. Reference Spoolder, de Rosa, Hörning, Waiblinger and Wemelsfelder2003). De Graaf et al. (Reference de Graaf, Ampe, Winckler, Radeski, Mounier, Kirchner, Haskell, van Eerdenburg, des Roches, Andreasen, Bijttebier, Lauwers, Verbeke and Tuyttens2017) looked at the sensitivity of the integrated Welfare Quality® scores in dairy cattle and suggested that the integration method of this protocol be revised to ensure that the relative contribution of the welfare indicators to the integrated scores more accurately reflected their relevance for dairy cattle welfare. Secondly, a decision must be made regarding whether compensation between criteria is allowed, such that good scores on specific criteria can compensate for bad scores on others (Botreau et al. Reference Botreau, Bonde, Butterworth, Perny, Bracke, Capdeville and Veissier2007). Thirdly, a scoring system must balance society’s expectation of welfare standards with the expert opinion of animal scientists and what is achievable in practice. Finally, it has been argued that value judgements are inherently involved when performing overall welfare assessments (Fraser Reference Fraser1995), although others claim that overall welfare assessment is not arbitrary and that a high level of accuracy can be achieved (Bracke et al. Reference Bracke, Spruijt and Metz1999). Thus, careful consideration must be given to the weighted scoring and aggregation system assigned to the proposed Welfare Quality® protocol for chimpanzees for meaningful overall welfare assessments to be produced.

Figure 2. The bottom-up approach of the Welfare Quality® scoring system, with aggregation of welfare indicators into 12 criteria scores, grouped further into four principle scores, and finally into an overall welfare categorisation.

Limitations and recommendations for future research

This study has four main limitations. The first limitation relates to the approach used to identify relevant literature. Literature reviews are current only at the point in time at which they are conducted, and work published post the search date is excluded. Additionally, this review focused only on peer-reviewed literature, although ‘grey literature’ would likely have offered valuable information from relevant within-zoo studies. Limiting the searches to the English language and articles available to the authors in full at the time of the search also potentially excluded relevant literature. The second limitation relates to time constraints. Given the vast amount of literature published on captive chimpanzee welfare, a decision was made to extract indicators from all included studies without a prior critical appraisal of each paper. While the validity of the indicators chosen was informally appraised prior to their inclusion in the adapted protocol, a critical appraisal of all the included studies using a specifically designed welfare indicator appraisal tool (similar to the one used by Williams et al. Reference Williams, Chadwick, Yon and Asher2018) would have further validated the chosen indicators. The authors acknowledge the limitations inherent in this retrospective, literature-based approach to indicator selection. Future efforts to implement this welfare assessment protocol should include a more systematic evaluation of the strength of evidence supporting each indicator to further strengthen the validity and reliability of the final tool. The third limitation of this study is that the lead author (NM) worked alone in reviewing the literature. Although the literature review followed a systematic methodology, it was not considered a ‘systematic review’ as reviews carried out by one person can inadvertently lead to increased bias in searching, screening, and data selection, as well as an increased chance of errors being made. The final limitation of this study is that the authors did not consult expert opinion before proposing indicators for this adapted Welfare Quality® protocol. Used in conjunction with one another, reviews of the existing literature alongside consultation with experts (keepers and specialists) would ensure that this adapted Welfare Quality® protocol offered a widely accepted and comprehensive welfare assessment tool.

In future studies, it would be useful to expand the research team and formally appraise the evidence from peer-reviewed literature on potential welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees. Subsequently, a workshop could be scheduled to refine the proposed indicators and design a scoring system that determines how indicators and criteria are scored, weighted, and aggregated into an overall welfare assessment. Finally, efforts could focus on piloting a functional welfare assessment tool, with subsequent re-evaluation and fine-tuning as needed.

Animal welfare implications

This study represents the first effort to adapt the Welfare Quality® protocol to assess captive chimpanzee welfare, and the methodology used for this study can be applied to other species in managed care. Developing species-specific welfare assessment protocols such as this contributes to the advancement of robust zoo animal welfare assessment methods at a time when both in situ and ex situ conservation efforts are increasingly crucial.

Additionally, the current manuscript builds upon the recent comprehensive review by Angley et al. (Reference Angley, Vale and Cronin2024), which examined the relationship between the care provided to captive chimpanzees (resource-based indicators) and the resulting impact on their welfare (animal-based indicators). The Angley et al. review served as the foundation for the development of the ChimpCARE Tool, an online resource that integrates the scoring, weighting, and aggregation of various welfare indicators to assess the well-being of captive chimpanzees.

While the ChimpCARE Tool was not specifically designed within the Welfare Quality® framework, the authors recognise substantial overlap between the indicators included in the current manuscript and those utilised in the ChimpCARE Tool. Both resources emphasise the importance of combining measures of the captive environment and management practices with direct assessments of the animals’ physical, behavioural, and mental states to comprehensively evaluate chimpanzee welfare.

The current work expands upon the literature synthesis provided in the Angley et al. review by specifically identifying and proposing a comprehensive set of welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees. Furthermore, by adapting the Welfare Quality® protocol for captive chimpanzees, the current manuscript offers an alternative, standardised framework for welfare assessment that can be compared to and potentially integrated with the ChimpCARE tool. The authors believe that the integration of these various perspectives and methodologies will provide caregivers and researchers with a suite of complementary resources to advance the assessment and improvement of welfare for this endangered primate species.

Conclusion

In conclusion, this work represents the first effort to adapt the EU Welfare Quality® protocol to assess captive chimpanzee welfare by proposing a set of valid, feasible, and reliable welfare indicators for this species. The proposed protocol was developed after substantial consultation with the published literature, and 14 (47%) animal-based and 16 (53%) resource-based welfare indicators were included to assess the 12 criteria and 4 principles of Welfare Quality®. This adapted protocol also includes indicators of positive affective state, reflecting the aim of modern welfare assessment tools to accommodate the shift in emphasis from avoiding negative affective states to promoting positive ones. Improving animal welfare standards has now become an institutional priority in the zoo industry, and thus the first effort to develop this chimpanzee-specific welfare assessment protocol will begin to enable zoos to make evidence-based assessments meaningful for this species.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2025.10021.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to acknowledge the assistance provided by Fiona Brown (MA, BSc Econ ILS, FHEA), an academic support librarian from Edinburgh University, who helped greatly in developing a robust search strategy for this work. Additionally, the authors wish to thank Helen Clarke (team leader at Dublin Zoo) for sharing their insights from years of experience working with captive chimpanzees.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contribution: Methodology: NM, NA; Supervision: NA, MB; Writing (original draft): NM; Writing (review & editing): NM, NA, MB.

References

Alford, P, Bloomsmith, M, Keeling, M and Beck, T 1995 Wounding aggression during the formation and maintenance of captive, multimale chimpanzee groupsZoo Biology 14: 347359https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430140406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, N, Amarasekaran, B and Riba, D 2021 An investigation into the influence of different types of nesting materials upon the welfare of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Animals 11: 1717. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11061835CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Angley, LP, Vale, GL and Cronin, KA 2024 The impact of care on chimpanzee welfare: A comprehensive review. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 275: 106272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2024.106272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aureli, F and FBM, De Waal 1997 Inhibition of social behavior in chimpanzees under high-density conditionsAmerican Journal of Primatology 41: 213228.10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1997)41:3<213::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-#3.0.CO;2-#>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aureli, F and Waal, FBMD 2000 Natural Conflict Resolution. University of California Press: Berkeley, CA, USA. https://doi.org/10.1525/9780520924932Google Scholar
Aureli, F and Yates, K 2010 Distress prevention by grooming others in crested black macaquesBiology Letters 6: 2729.10.1098/rsbl.2009.0513CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, KC 2000 Advanced age influences chimpanzee behavior in small social groupsZoo Biology 19: 111119.10.1002/1098-2361(2000)19:2<111::AID-ZOO2>3.0.CO;2-53.0.CO;2-5>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, KC 2004 Benefits of positive human interaction for socially housed chimpanzeesAnimal Welfare 13: 239245.10.1017/S0962728600026981CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, KC and Aureli, F 1997 Behavioural indicators of anxiety: An empirical test in chimpanzeesBehaviour 134: 10311050https://doi.org/10.1163/156853997X00386CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, KC and Easley, SP 1996 An analysis of regurgitation and reingestion in captive chimpanzeesApplied Animal Behaviour Science 49: 403415.10.1016/0168-1591(96)01061-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baldessari, A, Snyder, J, Ahrens, J and Murnane, R 2013 Fatal myocardial fibrosis in an aged chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)Pathobiology of Aging &Age Related Diseases 3: 21073. https://doi.org/10.3402/pba.v3i0.21073CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benn, AL, McLelland, DJ and Whittaker, AL 2019 A review of welfare assessment methods in reptiles, and preliminary application of the welfare quality® protocol to the pygmy blue-tongue skink, Tiliqua adelaidensis, using animal-based measuresAnimals (Basel) 9: 27https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9010027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernstein, IS 1962 Response to nesting materials of wild born and captive born chimpanzeesAnimal Behaviour 10: 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(62)90123-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bettinger, T, Wallis, J and Carter, T 1994 Spatial selection in captive adult female chimpanzeesZoo Biology 13: 167176.10.1002/zoo.1430130208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Birkett, LP and Newton-Fisher, NE 2011 How abnormal is the behaviour of captive, zoo-living chimpanzees? PLoS ONE 6: e20101, 17. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blokhuis, HJ 2008 International cooperation in animal welfare: the Welfare Quality® project. Acta Veterinaria Scandinavica 50(S1): S10. https://doi.org/10.1186/1751-0147-50-S1-S10CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blokhuis, HJ 2013 Improving Farm Animal Welfare: Science and Society Working Together: The Welfare Quality Approach. Wageningen Academic Publishers: Wageningen, The Netherlands. https://doi.org/10.3920/978-90-8686-770-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blokhuis, HJ, Veissier, I, Miele, M and Jones, B 2010 The Welfare Quality® project and beyond: Safeguarding farm animal well-being. Acta Agriculturae Scandinavica, Section A Animal Science 60: 129140https://doi.org/10.1080/09064702.2010.523480Google Scholar
Bloomsmith, M, Neu, K, Franklin, A, Griffis, C and McMillan, J 2015 Positive reinforcement methods to train chimpanzees to cooperate with urine collectionJournal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 54: 6669.Google ScholarPubMed
Bloomsmith, MA, Hasenau, J and Bohm, RP 2017Functionally appropriate nonhuman primate environments” as an alternative to the term “ethologically appropriate environments”. Journal of the American Association for Laboratory Animal Science 56: 102106.Google Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA and Lambeth, SP 1995 Effects of predictable versus unpredictable feeding schedules on chimpanzee behaviorApplied Animal Behaviour Science 44: 6574https://doi.org/10.1016/0168-1591(95)00570-ICrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Lambeth, SP and Alford, PL 1992 Female genital swellings have no effect on chimpanzee agonism in well-established groupsZoo Biology 11: 5360.10.1002/zoo.1430110107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Laule, GE, Alford, PL and Thurston, RH 1994 Using training to moderate chimpanzee aggression during feedingZoo Biology 13: 557566.10.1002/zoo.1430130605CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloomsmith, MA, Stone, AM and Laule, GE 1998 Positive reinforcement training to enhance the voluntary movement of group-housed chimpanzees within their enclosuresZoo Biology 17: 333341. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1998)17:4<333::AID-ZOO6>3.0.CO;2-A3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boissy, A, Manteuffel, G, Jensen, MB, Moe, RO, Spruijt, B, Keeling, LJ, Winckler, C, Forkman, B, Dimitrov, I, Langbein, J, Bakken, M, Veissier, I and Aubert, A 2007 Assessment of positive emotions in animals to improve their welfarePhysiology & Behavior 92: 375397https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Botreau, R, Bonde, M, Butterworth, A, Perny, P, Bracke, MB, Capdeville, J and Veissier, I 2007 Aggregation of measures to produce an overall assessment of animal welfare. Part 1: a review of existing methodsAnimal (Cambridge, England) 1: 11791187.Google ScholarPubMed
Botreau, R, Gaborit, M and Veissier, I 2012 Applying Welfare Quality® strategy to design a welfare assessment tool for foxes and mink farms. Proceedings of the Xth International Scientific Congress in Fur Animal Production, 433440. https://doi.org/10.3920/978-90-8686-760-8_63Google Scholar
Botreau, R, Veissier, I and Perny, P 2009 Overall assessment of animal welfare: strategy adopted in Welfare Quality®Animal Welfare 18: 363370. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600000762CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bracke, MB, Spruijt, BM and Metz, JH 1999 Overall animal welfare assessment reviewed. Is it possible? Netherlands Journal of Agricultural Science 47: 279291. https://doi.org/10.18174/njas.v47i3.466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brando, SI 2012 Animal learning and training. Implications for animal welfareThe Veterinary Clinics of North America: Exotic Animal Practice 15: 387398https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvex.2012.06.008Google ScholarPubMed
Brent, L 1992 Woodchip bedding as enrichment for captive chimpanzees in an outdoor enclosureAnimal Welfare 1: 161170. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600014986CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brownlow, AR, Plumptre, AJ, Reynolds, V and Ward, R 2001 Sources of variation in the nesting behavior of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Budongo forest, UgandaAmerican Journal of Primatology 55: 4955.10.1002/ajp.1038CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cabana, F, Jasmi, R and Maguire, R 2018 Great ape nutrition: low‐sugar and high‐fibre diets can lead to increased natural behaviours, decreased regurgitation and reingestion, and reversal of prediabetesInternational Zoo Yearbook 52: 4861https://doi.org/10.1111/izy.12172CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreras, R, Mainau, E, Arroyo, L, Moles, X, González, J, Bassols, A, Dalmau, A, Faucitano, L, Manteca, X and Velarde, A 2016 Housing conditions do not alter cognitive bias but affect serum cortisol, qualitative behaviour assessment and wounds on the carcass in pigsApplied Animal Behaviour Science 185: 3944.10.1016/j.applanim.2016.09.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caws, CE, Wehnelt, S and Aureli, F 2008 The effect of a new vertical structure in mitigating aggressive behaviour in a large group of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Animal Welfare 17: 149154. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600027652CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Celli, ML, Tomonaga, M, Udono, T, Teramoto, M and Nagano, K 2003 Tool use task as environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzeesApplied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 171182https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00257-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cerrone, M 2019 Keepers as social companions: Tactile communication and social enrichment for captive apesSign Systems Studies 47: 453479https://doi.org/10.12697/SSS.2019.47.3-4.06Google Scholar
Chelluri, GI, Ross, SR and Wagner, KE 2013 Behavioral correlates and welfare implications of informal interactions between caretakers and zoo-housed chimpanzees and gorillasApplied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 306315.10.1016/j.applanim.2012.06.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Claxton, AM 2011 The potential of the human–animal relationship as an environmental enrichment for the welfare of zoo-housed animalsApplied Animal Behaviour Science 133: 110https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2011.03.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clegg, ILK, Borger-Turner, JL and Eskelinen, HC 2015 C-Well: The development of a welfare assessment index for captive bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus)Animal Welfare 24: 267282https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.24.3.267CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conklin-Brittain, NL, Wrangham, RW and Hunt, KD 1998 Dietary response of chimpanzees and cercopithecines to seasonal variation in fruit abundance. II. Macronutrients. International Journal of Primatology 19: 971998. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1020370119096Google Scholar
Dawkins, MS 2006 A user’s guide to animal welfare scienceTrends in Ecology & Evolution 21: 7782https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2005.10.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Graaf, S, Ampe, B, Winckler, C, Radeski, M, Mounier, L, Kirchner, MK, Haskell, MJ, van Eerdenburg, FJCM, des Roches, ADB, Andreasen, SN, Bijttebier, J, Lauwers, L, Verbeke, W and Tuyttens, FAM 2017 Trained-user opinion about Welfare Quality® measures and integrated scoring of dairy cattle welfareJournal of Dairy Science 100: 63766388https://doi.org/10.3168/jds.2016-12255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Groot, B and Cheyne, SM 2016 Does mirror enrichment improve primate well-being? Animal Welfare 25: 163170. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.25.2.163CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vries, M, Engel, B, Uijl, I, van Schaik, G, Dijkstra, T, de Boer, IJM and Bokkers, EAM 2013 Assessment time of the Welfare Quality® protocol for dairy cattleAnimal Welfare 22: 8593. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.22.1.085CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Waal, FBM, Nishida, T, McGrew, WC, Marler, P, Pickford, M and de Waal, FB 1992 Appeasement, Celebration, and Food Sharing in the Two Pan Species. University of Tokyo Press: Tokyo, Japan.Google Scholar
Duncan, LM, Jones, MA, Lierop, MV and Pillay, N 2013 Chimpanzees use multiple strategies to limit aggression and stress during spatial density changesApplied Animal Behaviour Science 147: 159171.10.1016/j.applanim.2013.06.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duncan, LM and Pillay, N 2013 Shade as a thermoregulatory resource for captive chimpanzeesJournal of Thermal Biology 38: 169177.10.1016/j.jtherbio.2013.02.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fernandez, EJ 2022 Training as enrichment: A critical reviewAnimal Welfare 31. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.31.2.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, D 1995 Science, values and animal welfare: Exploring the ‘inextricable connection’. Animal Welfare 4: 103117. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600017516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraser, ON, Stahl, D and Aureli, F 2008 Stress reduction through consolation in chimpanzeesProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105: 85578562https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0804141105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fruth, B and Hohmann, G 1996 Nest building behavior in the great apes: the great leap forward? Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Fultz, A, Jackson-Jewett, R, Higginbotham, B and Lane, C 2015 Wounding rates in captive sanctuary chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)American Journal of Primatology 77: 8687.Google Scholar
Garner, JP 2005 Stereotypies and other abnormal repetitive behaviors: potential impact on validity, reliability, and replicability of scientific outcomesILAR Journal 46: 106117. https://doi.org/10.1093/ilar.46.2.106CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodall, J 1986 The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0266467400002029Google Scholar
Goodall, JM 1962 Nest building behaviour in the free-ranging chimpanzeeAnnals of the New York Academy of Sciences 102: 455467https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.1962.tb13652.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grunauer, PP and Walguarnery, JW 2018 Relative response to digital tablet devices and painting as sensory enrichment in captive chimpanzeesZoo Biology 37: 269273https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21431CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hassimoto, M, Harada, T and Harada, T 2004 Changes in hematology, biochemical values, and restraint ECG of rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) following 6-month laboratory acclimationJournal of Medical Primatology 33: 175186. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.2004.00069.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Held, SDE and Špinka, M 2011 Animal play and animal welfareAnimal Behaviour 81: 891899. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2011.01.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hemsworth, PH 2003 Human–animal interactions in livestock productionApplied Animal Behaviour Science 81: 185198.10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00280-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hermansen, JE, Strudsholm, K and Horsted, K 2004 Integration of organic animal production into land use with special reference to swine and poultryLivestock Production Science 90: 1126https://doi.org/10.1016/j.livprodsci.2004.07.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herrelko, ES, Buchanan-Smith, HM and Vick, SJ 2015 Perception of available space during chimpanzee introductions: Number of accessible areas is more important than enclosure sizeZoo Biology 34: 397405https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21234CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hill, SP and Broom, DM 2009 Measuring zoo animal welfare: theory and practiceZoo Biology 28: 531544https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockings, KJ, Anderson, JR and Matsuzawa, T 2009 Use of wild and cultivated foods by chimpanzees at Bossou, Republic of Guinea: Feeding dynamics in a human-influenced environmentAmerican Journal of Primatology 71: 636646https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20698CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, LM, Freeman, HD and Ross, SR 2016a Reconsidering coprophagy as an indicator of negative welfare for captive chimpanzeesApplied Animal Behaviour Science 176: 112119https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2016.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, LM, Shender, MA and Ross, SR 2016b Behavioral research as physical enrichment for captive chimpanzeesZoo Biology 35: 293297https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21297CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G 2008 A preliminary model of human-animal relationships in the zooApplied Animal Behaviour Science 109: 105127https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2007.04.013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, G and Melfi, V 2015 Are we ignoring neutral and negative human-animal relationships in zoos? Zoo Biology 34: 18https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21182CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosey, G, Melfi, V, Formella, I, Ward, S J, Tokarski, M, Brunger, D, Brice, S and Hill, SP 2016 Is wounding aggression in zoo-housed chimpanzees and ring-tailed lemurs related to zoo visitor numbers? Zoo Biology 35: 205209. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosey, GR 2005 How does the zoo environment affect the behaviour of captive primates? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 90: 107129https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.08.015CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hosey, GR and Skyner, LJ 2007 Self-injurious behavior in zoo primatesInternational Journal of Primatology 28: 14311437https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-007-9203-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howell, CP and Cheyne, SM 2019 Complexities of using wild versus captive activity budget comparisons for assessing captive primate welfareJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 22: 7896https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2018.1500286CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hubbard, GB, Lee, DR and Eichberg, JW 1991 Diseases and pathology of chimpanzees at the southwest foundation for biomedical researchAmerican Journal of Primatology 24: 273282. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.1350240313CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humle, T and Hill, C M 2016 People–primate interactions: implications for primate conservation. In: Wich, SA and Marshall, AJ (eds) An Introduction to Primate Conservation pp 219240. Oxford University Press, Oxford, UK. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198703389.003.0014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, SL, Ross, SR and Bloomsmith, MA 2016a Characterizing abnormal behavior in a large population of zoo-housed chimpanzees: Prevalence and potential influencing factorsPeerJ 2016https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2225Google Scholar
Jacobson, SL, Ross, SR and Bloomsmith, MA 2016b Characterizing abnormal behavior in a large population of zoo-housed chimpanzees: prevalence and potential influencing factorsPeerJ 4: e2225https://doi.org/10.7717/peerj.2225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensvold, MLA 2008 Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) responses to caregiver use of chimpanzee behaviorsZoo Biology 27: 345359https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20189CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jensvold, MLA, Buckner, JC and Stadtner, GB 2010 Caregiver-chimpanzee interactions with species-specific behaviorsInteraction Studies 11: 396409https://doi.org/10.1075/is.11.3.03jenCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensvold, MLA, Sanz, CM, Fouts, RS and Fouts, DH 2001 Effect of enclosure size and complexity on the behaviors of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 4: 5369. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0401_5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, N, Sherwen, SL, Robbins, R, McLelland, DJ and Whittaker, AL 2022 Welfare assessment tools in zoos: From theory to practiceVeterinary Sciences 9: 170. https://doi.org/10.3390/vetsci9040170CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khan, BN 2013 Impact of captivity on social behavior of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)Journal of Animal and Plant Sciences 23: 779785.Google Scholar
Knierim, U and Winckler, C 2009 On-farm welfare assessment in cattle: validity, reliability and feasibility issues and future perspectives with special regard to the Welfare Quality® approachAnimal Welfare 18: 451458.10.1017/S0962728600000865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowles, TG, Warriss, PD, Brown, SN, Edwards, JE and Mitchell, MA 1995 Response of broilers to deprivation of food and water for 24 hoursBritish Veterinary Journal 151: 197202https://doi.org/10.1016/S0007-1935(95)80210-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koops, K, McGrew, WC, Matsuzawa, T and Knapp, LA 2012 Terrestrial nest-building by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): Implications for the tree-to-ground sleep transition in early homininsAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology 148: 351361https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.22056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosheleff, VP and Anderson, CNK 2009 Temperature’s influence on the activity budget, terrestriality, and sun exposure of chimpanzees in the Budongo Forest, UgandaAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology 139: 172181. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20970CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koski, SE and Sterck, EHM 2007 Triadic post-conflict affiliation in captive chimpanzees: does consolation console? Animal Behaviour 73: 133142https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2006.04.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kumar, S, Laurence, H, Owston, MA, Sharp, RM, Williams, P, Lanford, RE, Hubbard, GB and Dick, EJ Jr 2017 Natural pathology of the captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): A 35-year reviewJournal of Medical Primatology 46: 271290https://doi.org/10.1111/jmp.12291CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lambeth, SP, Bloomsmith, MA and Alford, PL 1997 Effects of human activity on chimpanzee woundingZoo Biology 16: 327333.10.1002/(SICI)1098-2361(1997)16:4<327::AID-ZOO4>3.0.CO;2-C3.0.CO;2-C>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lambeth, SP, Hau, J, Perlman, JE, Martino, M and Schapiro, SJ 2006 Positive reinforcement training affects hematologic and serum chemistry values in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)American Journal of Primatology 68: 245256https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20148CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leavens, DA, Hopkins, WD and Thomas, RK 2004 Referential communication by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Journal of Comparative Psychology 118: 4857https://doi.org/10.1037/0735-7036.118.1.48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lock, LC and Anderson, JR 2013 Kin, daytime associations, or preferred sleeping sites? Factors influencing sleep site selection in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Folia Primatologica 84: 158169https://doi.org/10.1159/000351553CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mason, GJ and Latham, NR 2004 Can’t stop, won’t stop: is stereotypy a reliable animal welfare indicator? Animal Welfare 13: 5769. https://doi.org/10.1017/S096272860001438XCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCulloch, SP 2013 A critique of FAWC’s Five Freedoms as a framework for the analysis of animal welfare. Journal of Agricultural and Environmental Ethics 26(5): 959975. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-012-9434-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ 2012 Animal emotions, behaviour and the promotion of positive welfare statesNew Zealand Veterinary Journal 60: 18. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926799CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mellor, DJ 2015a Enhancing animal welfare by creating opportunities for positive affective engagementNew Zealand Veterinary Journal 63: 38. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926799CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ 2015b Positive animal welfare states and encouraging environment-focused and animal-to-animal interactive behavioursNew Zealand Veterinary Journal 63: 916https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ 2016 Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the “Five Freedoms” towards “A Life Worth Living”Animals 6https://doi.org/10.3390/ani6030021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ and Beausoleil, NJ 2015 Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Animal Welfare 24(3): 241253. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.24.3.241CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mellor, DJ and Reid, CSW 1994 Concepts of animal well-being and predicting the impact of procedures on experimental animals. In: Baker, R, Jenkin, G and Mellor, DJ (eds) Improving the Well-being of Animals in the Research Environment pp. 318. Australian and New Zealand Council for the Care of Animals in Research and Teaching: Glen Osmond, SA, Australia.Google Scholar
Mellor, DJ, Patterson-Kane, E and Stafford, KJ 2009 The Sciences of Animal Welfare. Wiley-Blackwell: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Mendl, M, Burman, OHP and Paul, ES 2010 An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and moodProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 277: 28952904https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.0303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miao, ZH, Glatz, PC and Ru, YJ 2004 Review of production, husbandry and sustainability of free-range pig production systemsAsian-Australasian Journal of Animal Sciences 17: 16151634https://doi.org/10.5713/ajas.2004.1615CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mononen, J, Møller, SH, Hansen, SW, Hovland, AL, Koistinen, T, Lidfors, L, Malmkvist, J, Vinke, CM and Ahola, L 2012 The development of on-farm welfare assessment protocols for foxes and mink: the WelFur projectAnimal Welfare 21: 363371https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.21.3.363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mulleder, C, Troxler, J, Laaha, G and Waiblinger, S 2007 Can environmental variables replace some animal-based parameters in welfare assessment of dairy cows? Animal Welfare 16: 153156. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600031213CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nash, LT, Fritz, J, Alford, PA and Brent, L 1999 Variables influencing the origins of diverse abnormal behaviors in a large sample of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)American Journal of Primatology 48: 1529.10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1999)48:1<15::AID-AJP2>3.0.CO;2-R3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
National Institutes of Health (NIH) 2013 A Review of Literature and Animal Welfare/Regulatory Requirements and Guidance Pertaining to the Space Density Needs of Captive Research Chimpanzees. NIH Office of Research Infrastructure Programs, Bethesda, Maryland, USA.Google Scholar
Neal Webb, SJ, Hau, J and Schapiro, SJ 2018 Captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) behavior as a function of space per animal and enclosure typeAmerican Journal of Primatology 80: e22749https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neal Webb, SJ, Hau, J and Schapiro, SJ 2019 Does group size matter? Captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) behavior as a function of group size and compositionAmerican Journal of Primatology 81: e22947https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22947CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunamaker, EA, Lee, DR and Lammey, ML 2012 Chronic diseases in captive geriatric female chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Comparative Medicine (Memphis) 62: 131136. https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2014.926799Google ScholarPubMed
Obanda, V, Omondi, GP and Chiyo, PI 2014 The influence of body mass index, age and sex on inflammatory disease risk in semi-captive chimpanzeesPLoS ONE 9https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0104602CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obsen, MT, Madsen, SB, Sloth, S, Gintberg, MM, Jensen, TH, Pertoldi, C, Pagh, S and Alstrup, AKO 2021 Sleep meditation as auditory enrichment for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Genetics and Biodiversity Journal 5: 8096.10.46325/gabj.v5i3.148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ogden, JJ, Finlay, TW and Maple, TL 1990 Gorilla adaptations to naturalistic environmentsZoo Biology 9: 107121https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.1430090203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Padrell, M, Amici, F, Cordoba, MP, Giberga, A, Broekman, A, Almagro, S and Llorente, M 2021 Artificial termite-fishing tasks as enrichment for sanctuary-housed chimpanzees: behavioral effects and impact on welfareAnimals 11. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11030603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Phillips, CJC, Izmirli, S, Aldavood, SJ, Alonso, M, Choe, BI, Hanlon, A, Handziska, A, Illmann, G, Keeling, L, Kennedy, M, Lee, GH, Lund, V, Mejdell, C, Pelagic, VR and Rehn, T 2012 Students’ attitudes to animal welfare and rights in Europe and Asia. Animal Welfare 21(1): 87100. https://doi.org/10.7120/096272812799129466CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pomerantz, O and Terkel, J 2009 Effects of positive reinforcement training techniques on the psychological welfare of zoo-housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)American Journal of Primatology 71: 687695https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pritchard, JC, Barr, AR and Whay, HR 2006 Validity of a behavioural measure of heat stress and a skin tent test for dehydration in working horses and donkeysEquine Veterinary Journal 38: 433438https://doi.org/10.2746/042516406778400634CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pruetz, JD and Bertolani, P 2007 Savanna chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus, hunt with toolsCurrent Biology 17: 412417https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.12.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pruetz, JD, Fulton, SJ, Marchant, LF, McGrew, WC, Schiel, M and Waller, M 2008 Arboreal nesting as anti-predator adaptation by savanna chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) in southeastern SenegalAmerican Journal of Primatology 70: 393401https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20508CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Raindi, D, Rees, J, Hirschfeld, J, Wright, H, Dobbs, P, Moitti, S, White, K, Stahl, W, Martin, M, Redrobe, S, Hughes, F, Liptovszky, M, Baiker, K and Grant, M 2022 Periodontal health, neutrophil activity and cardiovascular health in captive chimpanzeesArchives of Oral Biology 134https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archoralbio.2021.105342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reamer, L, Talbot, CF, Hopper, LM, Mareno, MC, Hall, K, Brosnan, SF, Lambeth, SP and Schapiro, SJ 2015 Assessing quantity of space for captive chimpanzee welfareAmerican Journal of Primatology 77: 8485.Google Scholar
Reamer, LA, Haller, RL, Thiele, E, Freeman, HD, Lambeth, SP and Schapiro, SJ 2014 Factors affecting initial training success of blood glucose testing in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Zoo Biology 33: 212220https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.21123CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reamer, LA, Webb, SJN, Jones, R, Thiele, E, Haller, RL, Schapiro, SJ, Lambeth, SP and Hanley, PW 2020 Validation and utility of a body condition scoring system for chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)American Journal of Primatology 82:e23101https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.23188CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reinhardt, V 2003 Working with rather than against macaques during blood collectionJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6: 189197.10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_04CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reynolds, V and Reynolds, F 1965 Chimpanzees of the Budongo Forest, Primate Behaviour . Field Studies of Monkeys and Apes pp 368424. Holt, Rinehart & Winston: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Rose, PE and Riley, LM 2022 Expanding the role of the future zoo: Wellbeing should become the fifth aim for modern zoos. Frontiers in Psychology 13: 1018722. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1018722CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, SR, Bloomsmith, MA, Bettinger, TL and Wagner, KE 2009 The influence of captive adolescent male chimpanzees on wounding: management and welfare implicationsZoo Biology 28: 623634https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, SR, Calcutt, S, Schapiro, SJ and Hau, J 2011a Space use selectivity by chimpanzees and gorillas in an indoor-outdoor enclosureAmerican Journal of Primatology 73: 197208. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.20891CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, SR, Joshi, PB, Terio, KA and Gamble, KC 2022 A 25-year retrospective review of mortality in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) in accredited US zoos from a management and welfare perspectiveAnimals (Basel) 12: 1878https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12151878CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, SR and Lukas, KE 2006 Use of space in a non-naturalistic environment by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla)Applied Animal Behaviour Science 96: 143152https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, SR, Wagner, KE, Schapiro, SJ, Hau, J and Lukas, KE 2011b Transfer and acclimatization effects on the behavior of two species of African great ape (Pan troglodytes and Gorilla gorilla gorilla) moved to a novel and naturalistic zoo environmentInternational Journal of Primatology 32: 99117https://doi.org/10.1007/s10764-010-9441-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rutherford, KMD, Donald, RD, Lawrence, AB and Wemelsfelder, F 2012 Qualitative Behavioural Assessment of emotionality in pigs. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 139(3-4): 218224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.04.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salas, M, Manteca, X, Abáigar, T, Delclaux, M, Enseñat, C, Martínez-Nevado, E, Quevedo, and Fernández-Bellon, H 2018 Using farm animal welfare protocols as a base to assess the welfare of wild animals in captivity—Case study: Dorcas gazelles (Gazella dorcas)Animals (Basel) 8: 111. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070111CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samson, DR and Hunt, KD 2014 Chimpanzees preferentially select sleeping platform construction tree species with biomechanical properties that yield stable, firm, but compliant nestsPLoS ONE 9: e95361. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095361CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schapiro, SJ, Bloomsmith, MA and Laule, GE 2003 Positive reinforcement training as a technique to alter nonhuman primate behavior: quantitative assessments of effectivenessJournal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 6: 175187. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327604JAWS0603_03CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seiler, BM, Dick, EJ Jr, Guardado-Mendoza, R, Vandeberg, JL, Williams, JT, Mubiru, JN and Hubbard, GB 2009 Spontaneous heart disease in the adult chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes)Journal of Medical Primatology 38: 5158. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0684.2008.00307.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sherwen, SL, Hemsworth, LM, Beausoleil, NJ, Embury, A and Mellor, DJ 2018 Rethinking the utility of the Five Domains model. Animal Welfare 27(3): 269282.Google Scholar
Skovlund, CR, Kirchner, MK, Moos, LW, Alsted, N, Manteca, X, Tallo-Parra, O, Stelvig, M and Forkman, B 2021 A critical review of animal-based welfare indicators for polar bears (Ursus maritimus) in zoos: Identification and evidence of validity. Animal Welfare 30(1): 118. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.30.1.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spijkerman, RP, Dienske, H, van Hooff, JARAM and Jens, W 1994 Causes of body rocking in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Animal Welfare 3: 193211. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600016845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spoolder, H, de Rosa, G, Hörning, B, Waiblinger, S and Wemelsfelder, F 2003 Integrating parameters to assess on-farm welfareAnimal Welfare 12: 529534. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600026130CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stewart, FA and Pruetz, JD 2013 Do chimpanzee nests serve an anti-predatory function? American Journal of Primatology 75: 593604https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stoinski, TS, Hoff, MP and Maple, TL 2001 Habitat use and structural preferences of captive western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla): Effects of environmental and social variablesInternational Journal of Primatology 22: 431447https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010707712728CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Strong, V, Moittie, S, Sheppard, MN, Liptovszky, M, White, K, Redrobe, S, Cobb, M and Baiker, K 2020 Idiopathic myocardial fibrosis in captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Veterinary Pathology 57: Unpaginated. https://doi.org/10.1177/0300985819879442CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Swaisgood, RR and Shepherdson, DJ 2005 Scientific approaches to enrichment and stereotypies in zoo animals: What’s been done and where should we go next? Zoo Biology 24: 499518. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takemoto, H 2004 Seasonal change in terrestriality of chimpanzees in relation to microclimate in the tropical forestAmerican Journal of Physical Anthropology 124: 8192https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.10342CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarou, LR and Bashaw, MJ 2007 Maximizing the effectiveness of environmental enrichment: Suggestions from the experimental analysis of behaviorApplied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 189204https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.05.026CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Temple, D, Manteca, X, Dalmau, A and Velarde, A 2013 Assessment of test–retest reliability of animal-based measures on growing pig farmsLivestock Science 151: 3545https://doi.org/10.1016/j.livsci.2012.10.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Truelove, MA, Martin, JE, Langford, FM and Leach, MC 2020 The identification of effective welfare indicators for laboratory-housed macaques using a Delphi consultation processScientific Reports 10: 20402. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-77437-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Varki, N, Anderson, D, Herndon, JG, Pham, T, Gregg, CJ, Cheriyan, M, Murphy, J, Strobert, E, Fritz, J, Else, J G and Varki, A 2009 Heart disease is common in humans and chimpanzees, but is caused by different pathological processesEvolutionary Applications 2: 101112. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-4571.2008.00064.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Videan, EN 2006 Sleep in captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes): The effects of individual and environmental factors on sleep duration and qualityBehavioural Brain Research 169: 187192https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2005.12.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Videan, EN and Fritz, J 2007 Effects of short- and long-term changes in spatial density on the social behavior of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Applied Animal Behaviour Science 102: 95105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.03.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Videan, EN, Fritz, J, Murphy, J, Borman, R, Smith, HF and Howell, S 2005a Training captive chimpanzees to cooperate for an anesthetic injectionLab Animal 34: 4348https://doi.org/10.1038/laban0505-43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Videan, EN, Fritz, J, Schwandt, ML, Smith, HF and Howell, S 2005b Controllability in environmental enrichment for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Journal of Applied Animal Welfare Science 8: 117130https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0802_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Waiblinger, S, Boivin, X, Pedersen, V, Tosi, M-V, Janczak, AM, Visser, EK and Jones, RB 2006 Assessing the human–animal relationship in farmed species: A critical reviewApplied Animal Behaviour Science 101: 185242https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2006.02.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallace, EK, Altschul, D, Korfer, K, Benti, B, Kaeser, A, Lambeth, S, Waller, B M and Slocombe, KE 2017 Is music enriching for group-housed captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)? PLoS One 12https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172672CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Webb, SJN, Hau, J and Schapiro, SJ 2018a Captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) behavior as a function of space per animal and enclosure typeAmerican Journal of Primatology 80: e22749https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22749CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, SJN, Hau, J and Schapiro, SJ 2018b Refinements to captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) care: a self-medication paradigmAnimal Welfare 27: 327341https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.27.4.327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webb, SJN, Hau, J and Schapiro, SJ 2019 Does group size matter? Captive chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) behavior as a function of group size and compositionAmerican Journal of Primatology 81: e22947https://doi.org/10.1002/ajp.22947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Welfare Quality® 2009 Welfare Quality® assessment protocol for dairy cows. Welfare Quality Network. Available at: https://www.welfarequalitynetwork.net/media/1319/dairy-cattle-protocol.pdfGoogle Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F and Farish, M 2004 Qualitative categories for the interpretation of sheep welfare: a reviewAnimal Welfare 13: 261268. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600028372CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wemelsfelder, F, Hunter, EA, Mendl, MT and Lawrence, AB 2000 The spontaneous qualitative assessment of behavioural expressions in pigs: first explorations of a novel methodology for integrative animal welfare measurementApplied Animal Behaviour Science 67: 193215https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(99)00093-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wemelsfelder, F, Hunter, TEA, Mendl, MT and Lawrence, AB 2001 Assessing the ‘whole animal’: a free choice profiling approachAnimal Behaviour 62: 209220https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.2001.1741CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, E, Chadwick, C, Yon, L and Asher, L 2018 A review of current indicators of welfare in captive elephants (Loxodonta africana and Elephas maximus)Animal Welfare 27: 235249https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.27.3.235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, RC, Nash, LT, Scarry, CJ, Videan, EN and Fritz, J 2010 Factors affecting wounding aggression in a colony of captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)Zoo Biology 29: 351364. https://doi.org/10.1002/zoo.20243CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolfensohn, S, Shotton, J, Bowley, H, Davies, S, Thompson, S and Justice, WSM 2018 Assessment of welfare in zoo animals: Towards optimum quality of lifeAnimals (Basel) 8: 110https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8070110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamanashi, Y, Matsunaga, M, Shimada, K, Kado, R and Tanaka, M 2016 Introducing tool-based feeders to zoo-housed chimpanzees as a cognitive challenge: spontaneous acquisition of new types of tool use and effects on behaviours and use of spaceJournal of Zoo and Aquarium Research 4: 147155. https://doi.org/10.19227/jzar.v4i3.235Google Scholar
Yamanashi, Y, Nogami, E, Teramoto, M, Morimura, N and Hirata, S 2018 Adult-adult social play in captive chimpanzees: Is it indicative of positive animal welfare? Applied Animal Behaviour Science 199: 7583https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2017.10.006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, J 2010 Breeding for pleasure: the value of pleasure and pain in evolution and animal welfareAnimal Welfare 19: 2938. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0962728600002219CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yeates, J 2018 Naturalness and animal welfareAnimals (Basel) 8: 53https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8040053CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yeates, JW and Main, DCJ 2008 Assessment of positive welfare: A reviewThe Veterinary Journal 175: 293300https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2007.05.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Table 1. Keywords used in searches of six reference databases to identify relevant articles to construct a comprehensive list of welfare indicators for captive chimpanzees

Figure 1

Table 2. The Welfare Quality® protocols for five species in managed care were reviewed, and the indicators chosen to assess each criterion were tabulated below to identify common indicators used across all species

Figure 2

Figure 1. PRISMA 2020 flow diagram outlining the results of the sourcing of literature for the review of welfare assessment for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes).

Figure 3

Table 3. Resource-based welfare indicators extracted from full texts of included studies were categorised as relating to the environment, enrichment or management practices

Figure 4

Table 4. Animal -based welfare indicators extracted from full texts of included studies were categorised as relating to health, behaviour or physiology

Figure 5

Table 5. The 12 criteria and 30 indicators chosen to create the adapted Welfare Quality® protocol for captive chimpanzees

Figure 6

Figure 2. The bottom-up approach of the Welfare Quality® scoring system, with aggregation of welfare indicators into 12 criteria scores, grouped further into four principle scores, and finally into an overall welfare categorisation.

Supplementary material: File

Mc Gill et al. supplementary material

Mc Gill et al. supplementary material
Download Mc Gill et al. supplementary material(File)
File 332.7 KB