Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-42vt5 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-02T07:02:58.336Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Associations between premenstrual symptoms and (traumatic) stress: a systematic review and three multilevel meta-analyses

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2025

Celine Bencker*
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria University Research Platform ‘Stress of Life (SOLE) – Processes and Mechanisms Underlying Everyday Life Stress’, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Ulrich S. Tran
Affiliation:
Department of Cognition, Emotion, and Methods in Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria University Research Platform ‘Stress of Life (SOLE) – Processes and Mechanisms Underlying Everyday Life Stress’, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Leopold H. O. Roth
Affiliation:
Department of Occupational, Economic, and Social Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
Sibel Nayman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Central Institute of Mental Health, Medical Faculty Mannheim, University of Heidelberg, Mannheim, Germany
Urs M. Nater
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria University Research Platform ‘Stress of Life (SOLE) – Processes and Mechanisms Underlying Everyday Life Stress’, University of Vienna, Vienna, Austria
*
Correspondence: Celine Bencker. Email: celine.bencker@univie.ac.at
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Core premenstrual disorders (PMDs), including premenstrual syndrome (PMS) and premenstrual dysphoric disorder, can cause significant impairment. Despite evidence linking stress and premenstrual symptoms, a systematic synthesis is lacking.

Aims

To systematically review the literature and meta-analyse evidence on the relationship between premenstrual symptoms and stress.

Method

Four databases (Web of Science, PubMed, PsycINFO, Scopus) and Google Scholar were searched for studies indexed before 27 August 2024 (no language/year restrictions) assessing the relationship between self-reported stress and premenstrual symptoms in regularly menstruating individuals (PROSPERO: CRD42021244503). Three multilevel meta-analyses estimated (a) the correlation between stress and premenstrual symptom severity, (b) stress differences between individuals with and without core PMD across the menstrual cycle and (c) the impact of traumatic experiences on the occurrence of premenstrual symptoms. Study quality and publication bias were assessed.

Results

We synthesised 188 effect sizes from 66 studies (N = 38 344), indicating (a) a positive correlation (r = 0.29, 95% CI 0.23–0.36); (b) higher stress levels in participants with core PMD (d = 0.79, 95% CI 0.32–1.26), particularly during the luteal phase (dlut = 1.01, 95% CI 0.46–1.57); and (c) over twofold higher odds (odds ratio 2.45, 95% CI 1.87–3.23) of PMS in individuals with a history of trauma. Heterogeneity was high (I2 = 84.64–91.38%); one meta-analysis (c) showed evidence of publication bias.

Conclusions

The results indicate an association between stress and premenstrual symptoms, an effect of cycle phase and trauma as a risk factor for PMS. Future research should explore underlying biopsychosocial mechanisms.

Information

Type
Review
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of Royal College of Psychiatrists

Normative fluctuations in progesterone and oestradiol during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle can be associated with affective, behavioural and somatic symptoms. Although mild premenstrual symptoms are common in females, with about 90% experiencing at least one symptom during their reproductive years, Reference Dennerstein, Lehert and Heinemann1 in individuals with core premenstrual disorders (PMDs), these symptoms are associated with considerable distress and impairment in everyday life. Core PMDs comprise premenstrual syndrome (PMS), which affects about 20–30% of females of reproductive age, Reference Dennerstein, Lehert and Heinemann1,Reference Steiner, Macdougall and Brown2 and premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD), which is at the most severe end of the spectrum and affects about 3% of females of reproductive age. Reference Reilly, Patel, Unachukwu, Knox, Wilson and Craig3 Core PMDs are characterised by the cyclic recurrence of symptoms that are confined to the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle and subside shortly after the onset of menses. PMDD is recognised as a depressive disorder in the DSM-5, 4 and has also recently been added to the ICD-11. 5 Based on the DSM-5, a diagnosis of PMDD requires the presence of at least five symptoms out of 11 symptom categories, including at least one affective symptom, that significantly interfere with work or school performance, usual activities or relationships with others. The criteria for PMS, according to the American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists (ACOG), are mostly equivalent to those for PMDD, with the exception that only one symptom from any domain must be present and must interfere with normal activities. 6 Given that the timing of symptoms is a key criterion, a diagnosis of PMS or PMDD requires confirmation of symptoms by prospective daily self-ratings over at least two consecutive menstrual cycles, although a provisional diagnosis can be made by a clinician. 4,6 In the present study, the terms PMS and PMDD are used in accordance with the ACOG and DSM-5 criteria, respectively, unless otherwise stated.

Regarding the aetiology of PMDs, current research assumes that hypersensitivity to normative cycle-related changes in progesterone and oestradiol, and their metabolites, in particular allopregnanolone, may underlie the symptoms. Reference Hantsoo and Epperson7Reference Schmidt, Nieman, Danaceau, Adams and Rubinow12 A possible pathway through which individuals develop this hypersensitivity may be an abnormal sensitisation in the stress response system and a dysregulation of the interaction between the hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG) axis, which is involved in controlling the release of gonadal hormones, and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, the major neuroendocrine stress response system. Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler13Reference Schweizer-Schubert, Gordon, Eisenlohr-Moul, Meltzer-Brody, Schmalenberger and Slopien15 Indeed, stress has repeatedly been implicated in the development, maintenance and exacerbation of PMS symptoms, Reference Beddig, Reinhard and Kuehner16Reference Lee and Im18 with evidence suggesting a dampened HPA axis function in PMS, reflected by lower cortisol levels, Reference Girdler, Pedersen, Straneva, Leserman, Stanwyck and Benjamin19Reference Nayman, Schricker, Reinhard, Dreer, Richter and Kuehner21 a delayed and attenuated cortisol awakening response, Reference Beddig, Reinhard and Kuehner16,Reference Hou, Huang and Zhou22 and a blunted cortisol response to stress. Reference Hamidovic, Davis and Soumare23,Reference Huang, Zhou, Wu, Wang and Zhao24

The association between stress and premenstrual symptoms is also evident on a subjective level: studies suggest that premenstrual symptoms are associated with higher levels of subjective stress, and symptoms appear to be exacerbated during times of stress. Reference Fontana and Badawy25Reference Yamamoto, Okazaki, Sakamoto and Funatsu31 This relationship might be particularly relevant during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, pointing to cycle-related effects on the relationship between subjective stress and premenstrual symptoms. Reference Gonda, Fountoulakis, Csukly, Telek, Pap and Rihmer32 In a literature review on stress and premenstrual symptoms published in 2016, the authors qualitatively analysed 48 studies based on their objectives, designs, methods and findings. Although the review highlighted a relevant relationship between stress and premenstrual symptoms, it also noted methodological differences between the studies and inconsistent results concerning potential moderating factors. Reference Lee and Im18

Exposure to trauma, as a particularly severe stressor, might further contribute to the assumed hypersensitivity to hormonal fluctuations and has been identified as a major risk factor for developing PMDs. Reference Islas-Preciado, Flores-Celis, González-Olvera and Estrada-Camarena33 Both longitudinal and cross-sectional studies have reported an association between PMS and multiple different types of trauma, including childhood abuse, Reference Azoulay, Reuveni, Dan, Goelman, Segman and Kalla34Reference Soydas, Albayrak and Sahin37 sexual assault Reference Golding and Taylor38,Reference McKinnon39 and combat-related trauma. Reference McKinnon39 This was supported by a meta-analysis from 2021, based on five case–control studies, which found that individuals who had experienced physical or sexual violence during childhood or adolescence were almost twice as likely to develop PMS as individuals without such experiences. Reference Islas-Preciado, Flores-Celis, González-Olvera and Estrada-Camarena33

Aims of the present systematic review and meta-analyses

Overall, the current state of research points to a relevant association between stress and premenstrual symptoms. However, no comprehensive synthesis of evidence has been conducted so far. The present study aims to consolidate the broad field of research and to improve understanding of the relationship between stress and premenstrual symptoms. Focusing on self-reported stress, we systematically reviewed and meta-analytically aggregated the current state of research, conducting three separate meta-analyses to address different research questions: The first meta-analysis (MA1) integrated evidence from studies assessing the relationship between premenstrual symptom severity and stress levels, aiming to estimate the magnitude of this association. A positive association was predicted. The second meta-analysis (MA2) sought to examine whether differences in subjective stress levels between individuals with and without core PMDs are cycle phase-dependent. We predicted higher stress levels in individuals with versus without PMDs and a larger group difference during the luteal phase compared with the follicular phase. The third meta-analysis (MA3) aimed to assess the impact of traumatic experiences on the occurence of premenstrual symptoms. We predicted that individuals with a history of trauma would show higher odds of experiencing PMS compared with individuals without a history of trauma. Finally, in all analyses, we assessed the influence of critical moderators, including publication year, mean age of the sample, study region and operationalisation of premenstrual symptoms and (traumatic) stress.

Method

Before conducting all searches, this systematic review and meta-analysis was preregistered with PROSPERO (identifier: CRD42021244503). Deviations from the protocol can be found in the Supplementary Material (see Supplementary File 1 available at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2025.10311). We followed the guidelines of the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA). Reference Page, Moher, Bossuyt, Boutron, Hoffmann and Mulrow40

Conceptualisation of stress and premenstrual symptoms

This review included studies that examined the relationship between premenstrual symptoms and self-reported stress, conceptualised as either a stressor or the appraisal of stress. Stressors include minor stressful events in everyday life, such as work overload or being stuck in traffic jam (known as daily hassles) Reference Almeida, Wethington and Kessler41 and major life events, such as the death of a loved one or dismissal from work. Reference Holmes and Rahe42 We further considered traumatic experiences as particularly severe stressors, and examined their impact on premenstrual symptoms in a separate analysis. Traumatic experiences are events that involve threat of death, physical harm or psychological harm that may lead to strong emotional responses. Reference Mueser, Rosenberg, Goodman and Trumbetta43

Search strategy and selection criteria

The literature search was based on a three-step search strategy and included studies from the first available date up to August 2024. A first search was conducted in June 2021, using the following electronic databases: Web of Science, PubMed, PsycINFO and Scopus. Additionally, the first 500 search results on Google Scholar were assessed for inclusion. The search syntax used in each database can be found in Supplementary File 2 (Table 1). The second search tier took place in November 2023, using the same databases and search strings as in the first stage, aiming to update the study sample with the most recent research published since 2021. In this step, we assessed the first 100 search results on Google Scholar – to identify any older relevant studies that may not have been indexed in 2021 – as well as the 100 most recent records. The final search was performed on 27 August 2024 to identify the most recent eligible studies before the completion of the analyses. In addition to the database search, reference lists of eligible papers were screened to retrieve further studies of interest. Both published and unpublished research reports, including dissertations and preprints, were eligible for inclusion. No year or language restrictions were applied.

The criteria for inclusion in one of the three meta-analyses were: (a) assessment of both self-reported stress and premenstrual symptom severity (either categorical or continuous); (b) provision of effect sizes for the relationship between stress and premenstrual symptoms, or, alternatively, sufficient information for effect sizes to be calculated (when such data were unavailable, corresponding authors were contacted to request the missing information); and (c) participants were menstruating individuals of reproductive age (age between 15 and 49 years 44 ). Studies with participants outside this age range were also included if the provided information indicated that the participants were postmenarcheal and premenopausal.

The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) studies investigating participants who were pregnant or breastfeeding at the time of the study, or (b) with irregular cycles (fluctuations ≥5 days or as stated by study authors) or cycle lengths of <21 or >35 days, Reference Munro, Critchley and Fraser45 (c) studies with animals as subjects and (d) qualitative studies and review articles. Effect sizes were extracted if at least one distinguishable subsample met the eligibility criteria. For instance, if a publication reported a group of female participants compared against a male group, only data from the first group were extracted. When studies reported separate data for subsamples with and without additional disorders (e.g. major depression), data from the subsample without comorbidities were extracted.

Additional specific eligibility criteria were defined for each of the three meta-analyses.

Meta-analysis 1: stress and premenstrual symptom severity

Studies were eligible for inclusion if (a) they investigated the relationship between self-reported stress and premenstrual symptom severity, and (b) Pearson’s r, and subsequently, Fisher’s z, could be extracted or calculated from the information provided.

Meta-analysis 2: stress and PMS – influence of cycle phase

Studies were eligible for inclusion if (a) they compared stress levels between individuals with core PMD and a control group (individuals without core PMD or only mild premenstrual symptoms), (b) data on this difference in stress levels were available from the same sample (within-participants design) for at least one measurement during the follicular phase and one measurement during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle, and (c) the provided data allowed for the extraction or calculation of Cohen’s d.

Meta-analysis 3: traumatic experiences as a risk factor for PMS

Studies were eligible for inclusion if they assessed (a) exposure to a traumatic event and (b) the severity of PMS symptoms, and (c) provided an odds ratio for the association between traumatic experience and PMS or sufficient data to calculate the odds ratio.

As the literature search yielded a large number of publications with major limitations, such as the lack of information on measures used, we defined the following further quality criteria for the main meta-analyses, to ensure the validity of the statistical results: (a) journal publications had to be published in a peer-reviewed journal that is indexed by Journal Citation Reports (2023) 46 and (b) publications had to report which assessment tools they used for both stress and premenstrual symptoms. Dissertations and preprints were included in the main analyses if criterion (b) was fulfilled. Although data were extracted from all studies that met the initial eligibility criteria, the main meta-analytic results reported were based on the studies that fulfilled these additional quality criteria.

Study selection and coding

Study selection and coding were carried out in two stages. Both involved equal steps, with the difference that in the second stage in 2024, the screening, data extraction and study quality assessment was carried out in parallel by C.B. and S.N., with good interrater agreement both for screening titles and abstracts (94.6%) and selecting full texts (97.7%). In 2021, those steps were completed by C.B. alone (first data extracted on 3 September 2021). Additionally, 20% of the eligible studies from 2021 were randomly selected for independent coding and study quality assessment by L.H.O.R. After screening titles and abstracts for inclusion, full texts of potentially relevant articles were reviewed for eligibility. For sources published in languages other than English or German, Google Translate was used for data extraction.

The primary outcome was statistical data on the relationship between PMS (symptoms) and stress. Secondary outcomes for moderator analyses included characteristics of publication (year of publication, publication type, country of study), study characteristics (operationalisation of stress, operationalisation of premenstrual symptoms, prospective versus retrospective assessment of premenstrual symptoms, consideration of hormonal contraceptive use in the samples), sample characteristics (population, sample size, mean age), symptom domain (MA1), information on the cycle phase (MA2) and time frame of the traumatic event (MA3). If data on the sample age were only available in the form of age groups, the mean age was approximated. Interrater reliability for data extraction was high. For categorical moderators, the lowest agreement was 91.3% (publication type in MA1). Similarly, intraclass correlation coefficients for effect size extraction and related data demonstrated high reliability: MA2 (1.00 in 2021, 0.98 in 2024), MA3 (1.00 in both years), MA1 (0.78 in 2021, 0.998 in 2024). Any disagreements regarding the eligibility of studies or coding of study characteristics were discussed until consensus was reached. The full coding scheme is openly available at https://osf.io/mcjsb/.

Quality assessment

All eligible studies were assessed for methodological quality by using customised tools for each meta-analysis (see https://osf.io/mcjsb/), primarily based on the Joanna Briggs Institute Critical Appraisal Checklist for Analytical Cross-Sectional Studies Reference Munn, Moola, Riitano and Lisy47 (MA1) and the Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies by the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (MA2 and MA3), 48 with adaptations made for each meta-analysis. Taking all aspects together, the studies were then rated as either poor, fair or good, based on the rating scheme.

Synthesis approach

Statistical analyses were performed in R for Windows (version 4.4.1 49 ), using the R package metafor (version 3.0.2 Reference Viechtbauer50 ). Three-level random-effects meta-analyses were performed to account for the multilevel structure of the data; that is, several studies reported multiple effect sizes. Variance was thus considered at three levels: sampling variance around the estimated population effect (level 1), variance between effect sizes within studies (level 2) and variance between studies (level 3). Reference Van den Noortgate, López-López, Marín-Martínez and Sánchez-Meca51 Model fit was tested with log-likelihood ratio tests, comparing the three-level model with models omitting either between- or within-study variance. We applied the restricted maximum likelihood estimation and used cluster-robust sandwich estimators for the standard errors. Additionally, prediction intervals were calculated to provide a range within which the true effect sizes of future studies are expected to fall. The R code used for all analyses can be accessed here: https://osf.io/mcjsb/.

Summary effect

Meta-analysis 1 (correlational)

Pearson’s r was extracted, or – if not directly reported – computed or transformed, to estimate the bivariate association between the severity of premenstrual symptoms and stress. All correlations were keyed in the same direction, with a positive correlation coefficient indicating an association of higher stress levels with more severe premenstrual symptoms. For synthesis, Pearson’s r was converted to Fisher’s z and later converted back to r for interpretation purposes.

Meta-analysis 2 (cycle differences)

Cohen’s d was used to estimate the mean difference in stress levels between individuals with and without PMS for each cycle phase, with higher values indicating higher stress scores in the PMS group (reference group) compared with the control group. In two studies where relevant data were only provided graphically, Reference Gallant, Popiel, Hoffman, Chakraborty and Hamilton52, Reference Petersen, London, Liang, Ghahremani, Gerards and Goldman53 group means and standard errors were extracted with the R package metaDigitise. Reference Pick, Nakagawa and Noble54

Meta-analysis 3 (traumatic stress)

To quantify the overall effect of traumatic experiences on the occurrence of PMS symptoms, the odds ratio (unadjusted, if available) was used as a summary effect size, with log-transformed values (logOdds) used for data synthesis. When frequency data were available in a 2 × 2 format, the escalc() function of the metafor package was used to directly compute the logOdds and the corresponding sampling variances. If no frequency data were available and no (unadjusted) odds ratios were reported, the available data were converted into logOdds by using formulas from Cooper et al. Reference Cooper, Hedges and Valentine55 To facilitate interpretation, logOdds were later transformed back to odds ratios. Two of the studies reported more than one comparison with the same control group. Reference Pilver, Levy, Libby and Desai56,Reference Takeda, Tadakawa, Koga, Nagase and Yaegashi57

Heterogeneity and moderator analyses

The distribution of effect sizes across studies was visualised by forest plots adapted to multilevel meta-analyses, plotting the study-specific effects and their precision, along with confidence intervals reflecting the sampling variance of individual effect sizes. Reference Fernández-Castilla, Declercq, Jamshidi, Beretvas, Onghena and Van den Noortgate58 Heterogeneity between and within studies was assessed with log-likelihood ratio tests and by calculating I 2 for each level of the model. Furthermore, prediction intervals were provided for each meta-analysis. Moderating variables were investigated by subgroup analyses and meta-regressions. Continuous moderator variables were centred around their means and categorical moderator variables were dummy-coded.

Robustness and assessment of reporting biases

We conducted leave-one-out sensitivity analyses to evaluate the robustness of the results. Reference Chambers59 Given that the performance of publication bias assessment methods varies under different conditions, Reference Carter, Schönbrodt, Gervais and Hilgard60 we employed a comprehensive sensitivity analysis approach to assess publication bias, applying and reporting results from multiple complementary methods. Publication bias was assessed both visually, using power-enhanced funnel plots, and statistically, by the skewness of the standardised deviates, precision effect test (PET) regressions and three-parameter selection models (3PSMs). The skewness of the standardised deviates (T S ) quantifies asymmetry in the distribution of effect sizes, capturing both magnitude and direction. T S can take any real value, where 0 indicates no skewness, positive values suggest missing studies on the left side of the funnel plot, and negative values indicate missing studies on the right. Reference Lin and Chu61 We classified skewness – and thus publication bias risk – as mild (<0.5), considerable (0.5–1) or substantial (>1). Reference Murad, Chu, Lin and Wang62 Additionally, we computed precision effect test/precision effect estimation with standard error (PET-PEESE) models, Reference Stanley and Doucouliagos63 regressing effect sizes on their standard errors (PET) or squared standard errors (PEESE), and applied 3PSM Reference Iyengar and Greenhouse64 to explore the likelihood of significant versus non-significant findings. 3PSM was chosen over other selection models as simulation studies have demonstrated its overall good performance. Reference Carter, Schönbrodt, Gervais and Hilgard60,Reference McShane, Böckenholt and Hansen65 We set the cut-off point for the 3PSM at P = 0.05. For the leave-one-out sensitivity analyses and assessment of publication bias, we used two-level models (i.e. the common random-effects model), aggregating the effect sizes within each study.

Results

Search results

Figure 1 provides a flow diagram of the screening and study selection process. The literature search yielded a total of 3876 records. After removing duplicates, the titles and abstracts of 2269 records were screened according to the inclusion criteria, ultimately leading to 314 studies that were reviewed in full text. Missing data from 27 studies were requested from corresponding authors with available contact information. Of these, seven requests were answered (response rate 26%), allowing additional information for three studies to be obtained. Following the primary eligibility criteria, data from n = 106 publications were extracted, which reported results from k = 104 studies: MA1, n = 71; MA2, n = 13; MA3, n = 26. An overview of main study characteristics can be found in Supplementary File 5 (Tables 1–3).

Fig. 1 Flow diagram following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) standards. PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Quality assessment and exclusion of data

Quality assessments of all studies are outlined in Fig. 2(a)–(c). Overall, study quality was low and only a small number of studies (nine out of 104) were rated as good. Particularly in MA1, there were considerable limitations in the study quality because of failures to report important information, such as the assessment tools used for premenstrual symptoms and stress (see Fig. 2(a)). Notably, only few studies assessed premenstrual symptoms prospectively over at least one menstrual cycle (MA1, 21%; MA2, 64%; MA3, 23%). For detailed quality ratings per study, please refer to Supplementary File 6 (Tables 1–3).

Fig. 2 Overview of quality ratings for all studies, including those not meeting stricter eligibility criteria. Design adapted from Reference Dubol, Epperson, Sacher, Pletzer, Derntl and Lanzenberger66 . Journal Citation Reports (2023). 46 The customised tools used for each meta-analysis are available here: https://osf.io/mcjsb/. MA1, meta-analysis 1; MA2, meta-analysis 2; MA3, meta-analysis 3; PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Based on the additional quality criteria, 38 publications were excluded from the main analyses. The following sections refer to the reduced study pool from 68 publications. Meta-analytic results considering all publications are provided in Supplementary File 4 (Table 1).

Meta-analytic results

Sixty-eight publications (k = 67) with j = 188 effect sizes were included in the three meta-analytic models, encompassing data from 38 494 participants (for a list of all included papers, see Supplementary File 3). All articles were published in English.

Operationalisations of key constructs

Across all three meta-analyses, assessment methods for (traumatic) stress varied considerably. Most studies measured stress appraisal, using validated questionnaires (e.g. Perceived Stress Scale Reference Cohen, Kamarck and Mermelstein67 ), subscales on perceived event stressfulness or single-item measures. The remaining studies included in MA1 and MA2 examined stressors – either as daily hassles or as life events – or operationalised stress, using the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales, Reference Lovibond and Lovibond68 reporting data on the stress subscale. The latter was categorised as assessing ‘unspecified tension’, as its items primarily capture a general state of arousal rather than explicitly assessing perceived stress, daily hassles or life events. Traumatic experiences were assessed by validated questionnaires, (diagnostic) interviews or single items. A comprehensive overview of the stress and trauma assessment methods and their classification for subgroup analyses is provided in Supplementary File 7 (Tables 1 and 2).

Regarding premenstrual symptom assessment, most studies – particularly in MA1 and MA3 – relied on retrospective questionnaires (e.g. Premenstrual Symptom Screening Tool Reference Steiner, Macdougall and Brown2 ) or structured interviews (e.g. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 for PMDD Reference Accortt, Bismark, Schneider and Allen69 ). Only a few employed daily prospective assessments across at least one menstrual cycle (e.g. Daily Record of Severity of Symptoms Reference Endicott, Nee and Harrison70 ). Studies investigating cycle effects (MA2) primarily used prospective assessments. In the primary studies, classification into the PMS group was based either on PMDD criteria, PMS criteria or on symptom severity scores. Detailed information on the instruments used in each primary study is available in Supplementary File 5 (Tables 1–3).

Summary effects

All three multilevel meta-analytic models yielded significant summary effects and showed superior model fits compared with the two-level models (Supplementary File 4, Table 2). The leave-one-out analyses revealed that one study Reference Antosz-Rekucka and Prochwicz71 had a disproportionate influence on the meta-analytic outcomes of two meta-analyses (MA1 and MA3), contributing with exceptionally large effect sizes. Consequently, this study was excluded from the subsequent primary analyses (for detailed influence diagnostics, refer to Supplementary File 4, Tables 7–9), reducing the data-set to 66 studies.

Figure 3 displays the distribution of effect sizes for each meta-analysis. The statistical results showed a significant relationship between stress and PMS symptoms in all meta-analyses: (a) MA1 (k = 35, j = 72, n = 6616) yielded a significant moderate correlation between stress and premenstrual symptom severity (r = 0.29, t(72) = 7.50, P < 0.001, 95% CI 0.23–0.36); (b) MA2 (k = 13, j = 44, n = 1249) yielded a significant large standardised mean difference between core PMDs and controls (d = 0.79, t(44) = 3.65, P = 0.003, 95% CI 0.32–1.26; results per cycle phase are reported in the next section); and (c) MA3 (k = 21, j = 69, n = 30 924) indicated that individuals with traumatic experiences had approximately 2.5 times higher odds of experiencing PMS compared with individuals without a traumatic history (odds ratio 2.45, t(69) = 6.93, P < 0.001, 95% CI 1.87–3.23). Significant heterogeneity was observed in all meta-analyses: I 2 MA1 = 84.64% (level 2: 15.5%, level 3: 69.15%; prediction interval −0.09 to 0.60), I 2 MA2 = 90.61 % (level 2: 12.97%, level 3: 77.65%; prediction interval −1.01 to 2.58), I 2 MA3 = 91.38 % (level 2: 33.5%, level 3: 57.88%; prediction interval 0.71–8.45).

Fig. 3 Forest plots for the three-level multilevel analyses. Black lines reflect the total precision of each study, considering the number of effect sizes and variability between them. Grey lines correspond to the study precision based on the sampling variance. J = number of effect sizes per study. The indicated Fisher’s z corresponds to r = 0.29, the logOdds corresponds to odds ratio 2.45. Depicted Cohen’s d values are derived from separate multilevel meta-analysis per cycle phase. Forest plots based on Fernández-Castilla et al. Reference Fernández-Castilla, Declercq, Jamshidi, Beretvas, Onghena and Van den Noortgate58 EDSP Study, Early Developmental Stages of Psychopathology Study; NC-ECA, North Carolina Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study; NSHLEW, National Study of Health and life Experiences of Women; PMDD, premenstrual dysphoric disorder; PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Moderator analyses

Meta-regressions testing the moderating effect of publication year and mean sample age yielded no significant results (for details refer to Supplementary File 4, Table 3 for statistical results and Figs. 13 for bubble plots).

MA1 revealed larger effect sizes in studies investigating students as participants (r = 0.38, 95% CI 0.30–0.46), compared with studies with samples from the community (r = 0.22, 95% CI 0.14–0.30, P = 0.006). Furthermore, when contrasting conceptualisations of stress/stressors to another, the association for daily hassles (r = 0.33, 95% CI 0.21–0.44) was significantly stronger compared with life events (r = 0.18, 95% CI 0.09–0.26). No significant differences were observed between the remaining stress categories, including perceived stress (r = 0.28, 95% CI 0.18–0.37) and unspecified tension (r = 0.41; 95% CI 0.00–0.70). In MA2, cycle phase (luteal versus follicular phase) and categorisation of premenstrual symptoms (PMS versus PMDD) emerged as significant moderators. Greater mean differences in stress levels between individuals with core PMDs versus controls were found in the luteal (d Lut = 1.01, 95% CI 0.46–1.57) compared with the follicular phase (d Fol = 0.58, 95% CI 0.15–1.01, P = 0.012). Furthermore, mean differences in stress levels were greater in studies comparing PMDD samples with controls (d PMDD = 1.17, 95% CI 0.49–1.85) versus studies comparing PMS samples with controls (d PMS = 0.33, 95% CI −0.30 to 0.95, P = 0.043). In MA3, no omnibus test of moderators was significant. Contrasting individual subgroups, largest effect sizes were found in studies linking premenstrual symptoms to PTSD (odds ratio 4.33, 95% CI 2.92–6.44), compared with unspecified type of trauma (odds ratio 1.96, 95% CI 1.43–2.68, P = 0.025) and in studies investigating PMDD as the outcome (odds ratio 3.47, 95% CI 2.09–5.76), compared with PMS symptoms (odds ratio 1.87, 95% CI 1.36–2.59, P = 0.019) or PMS (odds ratio 1.93, 95% CI 1.34–2.79, P = 0.036). Subgroup analyses examining study region, symptom domain (MA1), prospective versus retrospective assessment of premenstrual symptoms, publication type and consideration of hormonal contraception use revealed no significant effects in any of the meta-analyses. A detailed summary of all subgroup analyses is presented in Supplementary File 4 (Tables 4–6).

Publication bias

Sunset funnel plots (Fig. 4) show a slight rightward asymmetry for MA1 and MA3, suggesting potential publication bias. However, statistical analyses did not confirm this for MA1, except when the outlier Reference Antosz-Rekucka and Prochwicz71 was included in a sensitivity analysis. In contrast, MA3 showed considerable evidence of publication bias, with significant skewness (T S = 1.44, P = 0.007), significant PET (β 0 = 0.31, P < 0.001; β 1 = 1.72, P < 0.001) and PEESE (β 0 = 0.40, P < 0.001; β 1 = 2.33, P = 0.007) models and a lower likelihood of non-significant results (ω 2 = 0.25, P < 0.001). In MA2, no evidence of publication bias was found across all assessment approaches. For detailed results of publication bias assessments, refer to Supplementary File 4, Tables 10–12.

Fig. 4 Sunset funnel plots displaying effect sizes and standard errors, with colour gradients indicating power.

Discussion

This study systematically examined the association between self-reported stress and premenstrual symptoms by conducting three independent multilevel meta-analyses. Overall, our findings suggest that stress is significantly associated with more severe premenstrual symptoms, and that traumatic experiences may contribute to an elevated risk of premenstrual symptoms. In the following sections, we will discuss both the quantitative findings and the methodological limitations identified in the existing literature.

Summary of findings

Findings from the three meta-analyses indicated that (a) higher stress levels are associated with more severe premenstrual symptoms (r = 0.29); (b) individuals with core PMD experience higher stress levels than individuals without core PMD (d = 0.79), particularly during the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle (d Lut = 1.01); and (c) traumatic experiences are associated with about 2.5 times higher odds for developing PMS. The effects were particularly pronounced in individuals with PMDD compared with those with milder premenstrual symptoms.

The results highlight the role of stress in premenstrual symptoms and support the theory of heightened stress sensitisation as a possible pathway for the development and maintenance of premenstrual symptoms. Reference Girdler, Straneva, Light, Pedersen and Morrow20,Reference Huang, Zhou, Wu, Wang and Zhao24,Reference Klatzkin, Bunevicius, Forneris and Girdler72 In individuals with core PMDs, regulation of the stress response might be impaired because of reduced excitability of the GABAA receptor in response to allopregnanolone, Reference Hantsoo and Epperson11 leading to an increased sensitivity to stress during the luteal phase associated with allopregnanolone fluctuations. Reference Schweizer-Schubert, Gordon, Eisenlohr-Moul, Meltzer-Brody, Schmalenberger and Slopien15 Elevated stress levels in individuals with core PMDs, which are particularly pronounced during the luteal phase, support the assumption that hormonal fluctuations during this cycle phase, in conjunction with a dysregulation of the HPG and HPA axes, may give rise to the symptoms and associated heightened levels of stress. Since traumatic experiences, particularly during childhood, can lead to long-term alterations in stress response patterns, Reference Ehlert73,Reference Radley, Kabbaj, Jacobson, Heydendael, Yehuda and Herman74 trauma might further contribute to the pathway between stress sensitisation and core PMDs, and might thereby trigger hormonal sensitivity in susceptible females. Evidence of the link between trauma and hormone sensitivity was also provided by a study in individuals with menstrually related mood disorders, which found that in participants with a history of abuse, cyclical increases in progesterone and oestradiol predicted more severe premenstrual mood symptoms. Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Rubinow, Schiller, Johnson, Leserman and Girdler13 Furthermore, previous work suggests a luteal phase-specific effect of childhood adversity on stress and mood in individuals with core PMDs. Reference Nayman, Schricker, Reinhard and Kuehner75 The HPA profile of core PMDs coupled with the associated sensitivity to hormonal fluctuations during the luteal phase might therefore be unique compared with other mental disorders.

Limitations

The results of the present meta-analyses should be interpreted in light of some limitations, which may arise either from methodological decisions made during the review process or from limitations inherent in the primary studies themselves, including gaps in available data.

One important limitation is that our review exclusively relied on self-reported stress measures. As such, our data do not allow for conclusions about the biological stress reactivity or regulatory aspects of the biological stress systems in individuals with PMDs. To address this gap, a systematic review and meta-analysis focusing on the association between core PMDs and cortisol (re)activity is currently underway (PROSPERO: CRD420251052804). This work will allow for a more comprehensive understanding of stress in premenstrual symptoms.

In addition, the lack of longitudinal and experimental studies limits our ability to draw conclusions regarding the directionality of effects. Thus, it remains unclear whether stress exacerbates symptom severity or increases susceptibility to premenstrual symptoms, or whether the symptoms themselves contribute to elevated stress levels. Further adding to this uncertainty is the absence of data on the timing of stress levels and premenstrual symptom assessment, particularly in the case of MA1. As a result, it remains unclear whether stress levels were measured in direct proximity to symptom onset or at a more distant time point. This limitation is particularly relevant, as the temporal relationship between stress exposure or perception and symptom manifestation may affect both the strength and direction of observed associations. Future research should aim to clarify this aspect by incorporating precise longitudinal designs and time-lag analyses. Moreover, it remains unclear within which time frame following a trauma the odds of developing PMS are increased. Likewise, it is not yet known whether specific age ranges exist in which traumatic experiences are particularly associated with an increased risk of PMS, and to what extent the status of the reproductive system may play a role in this regard.

Beyond these factors, potential confounding influences also warrant consideration. We cannot draw conclusions regarding the potential role of comorbid disorders, as we did not specifically investigate the role of comorbidities such as depression, anxiety disorders or other psychiatric conditions, despite their possible influence on the relationship between stress and premenstrual symptoms.

Another factor affecting the robustness of our findings is the evidence of a risk of publication bias, suggesting selective reporting in MA3. Accordingly, the effect size for the relationship between trauma and premenstrual symptoms might be inflated.

Methodological heterogeneity between primary studies represents an additional limitation. The rather inclusive eligibility criteria applied in our meta-analyses allowed for substantial variation in study designs and operationalisation, contributing to the observed heterogeneity. Particularly, the multitude of assessment tools used across primary studies required the summarisation of instruments into broader categories for moderator analyses. Although this approach enabled synthesis across a diverse body of literature, it may have obscured more fine-grained or nuanced effects.

Finally, the potential impact of hormonal factors on the relationship remains unclear. Specifically, the roles of endogenous hormonal fluctuations during the menstrual cycle and exogenous hormonal modulation through contraceptive use cannot be determined based on our data. Only a few primary studies explicitly accounted for menstrual cycle phases, and there was inconsistent consideration of hormonal contraceptive use across participants. Although moderator analyses did not reveal significant effects, hormonal contraceptives influence oestradiol and progesterone levels, thereby altering menstrual cycle dynamics, and are associated with distinct HPA axis activity compared with naturally cycling individuals. Reference Bencker, Gschwandtner, Nayman, Grikšienė, Nguyen and Nater76 Additionally, hormonal contraceptives can have mood-stabilising effects and are commonly used as a treatment for PMDs by suppressing the hormonal fluctuations central to symptom manifestation. Reference Bencker, Gschwandtner, Nayman, Grikšienė, Nguyen and Nater76,Reference Hofmeister and Bodden77 Some primary studies failed to account for hormonal contraceptive use, whereas others included individuals using hormonal contraceptive without addressing its potential impact on results. This inconsistency may have introduced variability in the findings and affected the magnitude of the observed effects. The lack of methodological rigour in handling hormonal contraceptive use and assessing menstrual cycle dynamics highlights the need for future research to systematically assess and account for hormonal status as a critical variable.

Issues in current research practice

Our systematic review revealed significant quality issues in the available research. Several studies did not adhere to basic methodological standards, such as providing information on the assessment tools used, thereby impeding replicability. Further, some aspects specific to menstrual cycle research were neglected in multiple studies. This includes, for example, the consideration and valid assessment of menstrual cycle phases. Given that premenstrual symptoms in PMDs are restricted to the luteal phase of ovulatory cycles, Reference Hammarbäk, Ekholm and Bäckström78 it is important to validate the luteal phase and ovulatory cycles by using objective ovulation tests such as chromatographic ovulation tests. Furthermore, in the current research landscape, only a small proportion of studies prospectively assessed premenstrual symptoms by using daily symptom ratings across the menstrual cycle, which is a requirement for a valid diagnosis. This approach is crucial for confirming that symptoms are confined to the luteal phase rather than occurring throughout the entire cycle, ensuring a clear distinction from other mental disorders or the premenstrual exacerbation of other underlying disorders. However, the majority of primary studies have used retrospective assessments, which, as evidence suggests, may reduce specificity and lead to an overestimation of premenstrual symptoms. Reference Eisenlohr-Moul, Girdler, Schmalenberger, Dawson, Surana and Johnson79,Reference Henz, Ferreira, Oderich, Gallon, Castro and Conzatti80 In future research, it is therefore essential to use valid assessment tools for both premenstrual symptoms and stress, to clearly define concepts, and to take the menstrual cycle and confounding factors into account.

Future directions

Our findings point to gaps in the current research landscape, particularly concerning the temporal dynamics and the integration of psychological, biological and social factors. Future research should focus on longitudinal studies taking both within- and between-participant variations into consideration. This will allow for a deeper exploration of the temporal effects of experiencing symptoms versus the broader impact of having PMDs in general. Diary studies comparing the daily stress levels of individuals with and without core PMDs across the entire menstrual cycle could provide valuable insights into the temporal dynamics between premenstrual symptoms and stress. In addition, more research is needed to elucidate the psychobiological stress response in individuals with PMDs. A comprehensive framework of how, in the context of PMDs, alterations in the biological stress response relate to differences in subjective stress levels, and to what extent hormonal fluctuations across the menstrual cycle play a role in this regard, is still lacking.

Enhancing the understanding of the mechanisms and causes underlying premenstrual symptoms holds the promise of discovering effective treatments for PMDs. In addition to first-line psychopharmacological treatment of PMDD with antidepressants and hormonal contraceptives, Reference Hofmeister and Bodden77 stress management training might be a promising approach for the psychological treatment of core PMDs. Reference Han, Cha and Kim81 As in individuals with core PMDs, the stress level appears to be particularly pronounced in the luteal phase, those affected may particularly benefit from stress management methods during this cycle phase.

In conclusion, this study represents the first systematic review and statistical aggregation on the relationship between premenstrual symptoms and stress. Our findings indicate a relevant association between self-reported stress and premenstrual symptom severity. This association may be particularly strong during the luteal phase of the cycle, and trauma as particularly severe stressor was associated with increased odds of developing PMS or PMDD. Future studies should focus on the temporal dynamics of stress and premenstrual symptoms across the menstrual cycle, as well as their interactions with biological factors such as allopregnanolone and biomarkers of stress.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2025.10311

Data availability

The data and analysis code are openly available at https://osf.io/mcjsb/files/github.

Acknowledgements

The first author is a recipient of a DOC Fellowship of the Austrian Academy of Sciences at the Institute of Clinical and Health Psychology of the University of Vienna (26796). We thank Sarah Mannion for proofreading this manuscript and Erika Comasco for her valuable feedback on earlier versions of the manuscript.

Author contributions

C.B. was responsible for study conceptualisation, methodology, project administration, formal analysis, investigation, data curation and visualisation, and wrote the original draft of the manuscript. U.S.T. supervised the study, contributed to the methodology and reviewed and edited the manuscript. L.H.O.R. and S.N. contributed to the investigation and data curation, and reviewed and edited the manuscript. U.M.N. supervised the study, provided resources and reviewed and edited the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no specific grant from any funding agency, commercial or not-for-profit sectors.

Declaration of interest

None.

References

Dennerstein, L, Lehert, P, Heinemann, K. Epidemiology of premenstrual symptoms and disorders. Menopause Int 2012; 18: 4851.10.1258/mi.2012.012013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steiner, M, Macdougall, M, Brown, E. The premenstrual symptoms screening tool (PSST) for clinicians. Arch Women’s Ment Health 2003; 6: 203–9.10.1007/s00737-003-0018-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reilly, TJ, Patel, S, Unachukwu, IC, Knox, CL, Wilson, CA, Craig, MC, et al. The prevalence of premenstrual dysphoric disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord 2024; 349: 534–40.10.1016/j.jad.2024.01.066CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. APA, 2013.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO). International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, 11th ed. WHO, 2019.Google Scholar
American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. ACOG Practice Bulletin: Premenstrual syndrome. Int J Gynecol Obstet 2001; 73: 183–91.10.1016/S0020-7292(01)00400-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hantsoo, L, Epperson, CN. Premenstrual dysphoric disorder: epidemiology and treatment. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2015; 17: 87.10.1007/s11920-015-0628-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoyer, J, Burmann, I, Kieseler, ML, Vollrath, F, Hellrung, L, Arelin, K, et al. Menstrual cycle phase modulates emotional conflict processing in women with and without Premenstrual Syndrome (PMS) – a pilot study. PLoS ONE 2013; 8: e59780.10.1371/journal.pone.0059780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peters, JR, Schmalenberger, KM, Eng, AG, Stumper, A, Martel, MM, Eisenlohr-Moul, TA. Dimensional Affective Sensitivity to Hormones across the Menstrual Cycle (DASH-MC): a transdiagnostic framework for ovarian steroid influences on psychopathology. Mol Psychiatry 2025; 30: 251–62.10.1038/s41380-024-02693-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bäckström, T, Bixo, M, Johansson, M, Nyberg, S, Ossewaarde, L, Ragagnin, G, et al. Allopregnanolone and mood disorders. Prog Neurobiol 2014; 113: 8894.10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.07.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hantsoo, L, Epperson, CN. Allopregnanolone in premenstrual dysphoric disorder (PMDD): evidence for dysregulated sensitivity to GABA-A receptor modulating neuroactive steroids across the menstrual cycle. Neurobiol Stress 2020; 12: 100213.10.1016/j.ynstr.2020.100213CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schmidt, PJ, Nieman, LK, Danaceau, MA, Adams, LF, Rubinow, DR. Differential behavioral effects of gonadal steroids in women with and in those without premenstrual syndrome. N Engl J Med 1998; 338: 209–16.10.1056/NEJM199801223380401CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenlohr-Moul, T, Rubinow, DR, Schiller, CE, Johnson, JL, Leserman, J, Girdler, SS. Histories of abuse predict stronger within-person covariation of ovarian steroids and mood symptoms in women with menstrually related mood disorder. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 67: 142–52.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.01.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hantsoo, L, Jagodnik, KM, Novick, AM, Baweja, R, di Scalea, TL, Ozerdem, A, et al. The role of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis in depression across the female reproductive lifecycle: current knowledge and future directions. Front Endocrinol 2023; 14: 1295261.10.3389/fendo.2023.1295261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schweizer-Schubert, S, Gordon, JL, Eisenlohr-Moul, TA, Meltzer-Brody, S, Schmalenberger, KM, Slopien, R, et al. Steroid hormone sensitivity in reproductive mood disorders: On the role of the GABAA receptor complex and stress during hormonal transitions. Front Med 2021; 7: 479646.10.3389/fmed.2020.479646CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beddig, T, Reinhard, I, Kuehner, C. Stress, mood, and cortisol during daily life in women with Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder (PMDD). Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019; 109: 104372.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2019.104372CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jung, SJ, Roberts, AL, Chocano-Bedoya, P, Whitcomb, BW, Missmer, SA, Manson, JE, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder and development of premenstrual syndrome in a longitudinal cohort of women. Arch Womens Ment Health 2019; 22: 535–9.10.1007/s00737-018-0916-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Y, Im, EO. Stress and premenstrual symptoms in reproductive-aged women. Health Care Women Int 2016; 37: 646–70.10.1080/07399332.2015.1049352CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girdler, SS, Pedersen, CA, Straneva, PA, Leserman, J, Stanwyck, CL, Benjamin, S, et al. Dysregulation of cardiovascular and neuroendocrine responses to stress in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Psychiatry Res 1998; 81: 163–78.10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00074-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Girdler, SS, Straneva, PA, Light, KC, Pedersen, CA, Morrow, AL. Allopregnanolone levels and reactivity to mental stress in premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2001; 49: 788–97.10.1016/S0006-3223(00)01044-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nayman, S, Schricker, IF, Reinhard, I, Dreer, JK, Richter, AS, Kuehner, C. State and trait cognitions differentially affect cyclicity of mood and cortisol in individuals with and without premenstrual dysphoric disorder. J Psychopathol Clin Sci 2024; 133: 309–20.10.1037/abn0000894CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hou, L, Huang, Y, Zhou, R. Premenstrual syndrome is associated with altered cortisol awakening response. Stress 2019; 22: 640–6.10.1080/10253890.2019.1608943CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamidovic, A, Davis, J, Soumare, F. Blunted cortisol response to acute psychosocial stress in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2024; 27: pyae015.10.1093/ijnp/pyae015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huang, Y, Zhou, R, Wu, M, Wang, Q, Zhao, Y. Premenstrual syndrome is associated with blunted cortisol reactivity to the TSST. Stress 2015; 18: 160–8.10.3109/10253890.2014.999234CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fontana, AM, Badawy, S. Perceptual and coping processes across the menstrual cycle: an investigation in a premenstrual syndrome clinic and a community sample. Behav Med 1997; 22: 152–9.10.1080/08964289.1997.10543548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollenberg, AL, Hediger, ML, Mumford, SL, Whitcomb, BW, Hovey, KM, Wactawski-Wende, J, et al. Perceived stress and severity of perimenstrual symptoms: the BioCycle Study. J Womens Health 2010; 19: 959–67.10.1089/jwh.2009.1717CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jahromi, BN, Pakmehr, S, Hagh-Shenas, H. Work stress, premenstrual syndrome and dysphoric disorder: are there any associations? Iran Red Crescent Med J 2011; 13: 199202.Google Scholar
Potter, J, Bouyer, J, Trussell, J, Moreau, C. Premenstrual syndrome prevalence and fluctuation over time: results from a french population-based survey. J Womens Health 2009; 18: 31–9.10.1089/jwh.2008.0932CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramcharan, S, Love, EJ, Fick, GH, Goldfien, A. The epidemiology of premenstrual symptoms in a population-based sample of 2650 urban women: attributable risk and risk factors. J Clin Epidemiol 1992; 45: 377–92.10.1016/0895-4356(92)90039-PCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warner, P, Bancroft, J. Factors related to self-reporting of the pre-menstrual syndrome. Br J Psychiatry J Ment Sci 1990; 157: 249–60.10.1192/bjp.157.2.249CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yamamoto, K, Okazaki, A, Sakamoto, Y, Funatsu, M. The relationship between premenstrual symptoms, menstrual pain, irregular menstrual cycles, and psychosocial stress among Japanese college students. J Physiol Anthropol 2009; 28: 129–36.10.2114/jpa2.28.129CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gonda, X, Fountoulakis, KN, Csukly, G, Telek, T, Pap, D, Rihmer, Z, et al. Association of a trait-like bias towards the perception of negative subjective life events with risk of developing premenstrual symptoms. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2010; 34: 500–5.10.1016/j.pnpbp.2010.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Islas-Preciado, D, Flores-Celis, K, González-Olvera, J, Estrada-Camarena, E. Effect of physical and sexual violence during childhood and/or adolescence on the development of menstrual related mood disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Salud Ment 2021; 44: 145–53.10.17711/SM.0185-3325.2021.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Azoulay, M, Reuveni, I, Dan, R, Goelman, G, Segman, R, Kalla, C, et al. Childhood trauma and premenstrual symptoms: the role of emotion regulation. Child Abuse Negl 2020; 108: 104637.10.1016/j.chiabu.2020.104637CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertone-Johnson, ER, Whitcomb, BW, Missmer, SA, Manson, JE, Hankinson, SE, Rich-Edwards, JW. Early life emotional, physical, and sexual abuse and the development of premenstrual syndrome: a longitudinal study. J Womens Health 2014; 23: 729–39.10.1089/jwh.2013.4674CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koci, A, Strickland, O. Relationship of adolescent physical and sexual abuse to perimenstrual symptoms (PMS) in adulthood. Issues Ment Health Nurs 2007; 28: 7587.10.1080/01612840600996281CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Soydas, EA, Albayrak, Y, Sahin, B. Increased childhood abuse in patients with premenstrual dysphoric disorder in a Turkish sample: a cross-sectional study. Prim Care Companion CNS Disord 2014; 16. Available from https://doi.org/10.4088/PCC.14m01647.Google Scholar
Golding, JM, Taylor, DL. Sexual assault history and premenstrual distress in two general population samples. J Womens Health 1996; 5: 143–52.10.1089/jwh.1996.5.143CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McKinnon, BC. Relationship of trauma history and premenstrual syndrome among female veterans. Master’s thesis University of Iowa, 2009 (https://doi.org/10.17077/etd.o2t7c3yg).Google Scholar
Page, MJ, Moher, D, Bossuyt, PM, Boutron, I, Hoffmann, TC, Mulrow, CD, et al. PRISMA 2020 explanation and elaboration: updated guidance and exemplars for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021; 372: n160.Google ScholarPubMed
Almeida, DM, Wethington, E, Kessler, RC. The daily inventory of stressful events: an interview-based approach for measuring daily stressors. Assessment 2002; 9: 4155.Google ScholarPubMed
Holmes, TH, Rahe, RH. The social readjustment rating scale. J Psychosom Res 1967; 11: 213–8.10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mueser, KT, Rosenberg, SD, Goodman, LA, Trumbetta, SL. Trauma, PTSD, and the course of severe mental illness: an interactive model. Schizophr Res 2002; 53: 123–43.10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00173-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization (WHO). Reproductive Health Indicators: Guidelines for Their Generation, Interpretation and Analysis for Global Monitoring. WHO, 2006 (https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/43185).Google Scholar
Munro, MG, Critchley, HOD, Fraser, IS. The FIGO systems for nomenclature and classification of causes of abnormal uterine bleeding in the reproductive years: who needs them? Am J Obstet Gynecol 2012; 207: 259–65.10.1016/j.ajog.2012.01.046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clarivate. Journal Citation Reports TM. Clarivate, n.d. (https://jcr.clarivate.com/jcr/home).Google Scholar
Munn, Z, Moola, S, Riitano, D, Lisy, K. The development of a critical appraisal tool for use in systematic reviews addressing questions of prevalence. Int J Health Policy Manag 2014; 3: 123–8.10.15171/ijhpm.2014.71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute (NHLBI). Quality Assessment Tool for Observational Cohort and Cross-Sectional Studies. NHLBI, 2021 (https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools).Google Scholar
R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2024 (https://www.R-project.org/).Google Scholar
Viechtbauer, W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw 2010; 36: 148.10.18637/jss.v036.i03CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van den Noortgate, W, López-López, JA, Marín-Martínez, F, Sánchez-Meca, J. Three-level meta-analysis of dependent effect sizes. Behav Res Methods 2013; 45: 576–94.10.3758/s13428-012-0261-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallant, SJ, Popiel, DA, Hoffman, DM, Chakraborty, PK, Hamilton, JA. Using daily ratings to confirm premenstrual syndrome/late luteal phase dysphoric disorder. Part II. What makes a “real” difference? Psychosom Med 1992; 54: 167–81.10.1097/00006842-199203000-00004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petersen, N, London, ED, Liang, L, Ghahremani, DG, Gerards, R, Goldman, L, et al. Emotion regulation in women with premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Arch Womens Ment Health 2016; 19: 891–8.10.1007/s00737-016-0634-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pick, JL, Nakagawa, S, Noble, DWA. Reproducible, flexible and high-throughput data extraction from primary literature: The metaDigitise r package. Methods Ecol Evol 2019; 10: 426–31.10.1111/2041-210X.13118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cooper, H, Hedges, LV, Valentine, JC, eds. The Handbook of Research Synthesis and Meta-analysis, 3rd ed. Russell Sage Foundation, 2019.10.7758/9781610448864CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pilver, CE, Levy, BR, Libby, DJ, Desai, RA. Posttraumatic stress disorder and trauma characteristics are correlates of premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Arch Womens Ment Health 2011; 14: 383–93.10.1007/s00737-011-0232-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Takeda, T, Tadakawa, M, Koga, S, Nagase, S, Yaegashi, N. Premenstrual symptoms and posttraumatic stress disorder in Japanese high school students 9 months after the Great East-Japan Earthquake. Tohoku J Exp Med 2013; 230: 151–4.10.1620/tjem.230.151CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fernández-Castilla, B, Declercq, L, Jamshidi, L, Beretvas, SN, Onghena, P, Van den Noortgate, W. Visual representations of meta-analyses of multiple outcomes: extensions to forest plots, funnel plots, and caterpillar plots. Methodology 2020; 16: 299315.10.5964/meth.4013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, JM. Linear models. In Statistical Models in S (eds JM Chambers, TJ Hastie): Ch. 4. Routledge, 1992.Google Scholar
Carter, EC, Schönbrodt, FD, Gervais, WM, Hilgard, J. Correcting for bias in psychology: a comparison of meta-analytic methods. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci 2019; 2: 115–44.10.1177/2515245919847196CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, L, Chu, H. Quantifying publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics 2018; 74: 785–94.10.1111/biom.12817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murad, MH, Chu, H, Lin, L, Wang, Z. The effect of publication bias magnitude and direction on the certainty in evidence. BMJ Evid-Based Med 2018; 23: 84–6.10.1136/bmjebm-2018-110891CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanley, TD, Doucouliagos, H. Meta-regression approximations to reduce publication selection bias. Res Synth Methods 2014; 5: 6078.10.1002/jrsm.1095CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyengar, S, Greenhouse, JB. Selection models and the file drawer problem. Stat Sci 1988; 3: 109–17.Google Scholar
McShane, BB, Böckenholt, U, Hansen, KT. Adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis: an evaluation of selection methods and some cautionary notes. Perspect Psychol Sci 2016; 11: 730–49.10.1177/1745691616662243CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dubol, M, Epperson, CN, Sacher, J, Pletzer, B, Derntl, B, Lanzenberger, R, et al. Neuroimaging the menstrual cycle: a multimodal systematic review. Front Neuroendocrinol 2021; 60: 100878.10.1016/j.yfrne.2020.100878CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, S, Kamarck, T, Mermelstein, R. A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 1983; 24: 385396.10.2307/2136404CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lovibond, PF, Lovibond, SH. Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS–21, DASS–42) [database record]. APA PsycTests, 1995 (https://doi.org/10.1037/t01004-000).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Accortt, EE, Bismark, A, Schneider, T, Allen, J. Diagnosing premenstrual dysphoric disorder: the reliability of a structured clinical interview. Arch Womens Ment Health 2011; 14: 265–7.10.1007/s00737-011-0209-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Endicott, J, Nee, J, Harrison, W. Daily Record of Severity of Problems (DRSP): reliability and validity. Arch Womens Ment Health 2006; 9: 41–9.10.1007/s00737-005-0103-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antosz-Rekucka, R, Prochwicz, K. The relationship between trauma, stress, and premenstrual symptoms: the role of attributional style and trait anger. J Reprod Infant Psychol 2024; 10: 114.10.1080/02646838.2024.2377099CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klatzkin, RR, Bunevicius, A, Forneris, CA, Girdler, S. Menstrual mood disorders are associated with blunted sympathetic reactivity to stress. J Psychosom Res 2014; 76: 4655.10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.11.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ehlert, U. Enduring psychobiological effects of childhood adversity. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2013; 38: 1850–7.10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.06.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radley, JJ, Kabbaj, M, Jacobson, L, Heydendael, W, Yehuda, R, Herman, JP. Stress risk factors and stress-related pathology: neuroplasticity, epigeneticy, and endophenotypes. Stress 2011; 14: 481–97.10.3109/10253890.2011.604751CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nayman, S, Schricker, IF, Reinhard, I, Kuehner, C. Childhood adversity predicts stronger premenstrual mood worsening, stress appraisal and cortisol decrease in women with Premenstrual Dysphoric Disorder. Front Endocrinol 2023; 14: 1278531.10.3389/fendo.2023.1278531CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bencker, C, Gschwandtner, L, Nayman, S, Grikšienė, R, Nguyen, B, Nater, UM, et al. Progestagens and progesterone receptor modulation: effects on the brain, mood, stress, and cognition in females. Front Neuroendocrinol 2025; 76: 101160.10.1016/j.yfrne.2024.101160CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmeister, S, Bodden, S. Premenstrual syndrome and premenstrual dysphoric disorder. Am Fam Physician 2016; 94: 236–40.Google ScholarPubMed
Hammarbäk, S, Ekholm, UB, Bäckström, T. Spontaneous anovulation causing disappearance of cyclical symptoms in women with the premenstrual syndrome. Acta Endocrinol (Copenh) 1991; 125: 132–7.Google Scholar
Eisenlohr-Moul, TA, Girdler, SS, Schmalenberger, KM, Dawson, DN, Surana, P, Johnson, JL, et al. Toward the reliable diagnosis of DSM-5 premenstrual dysphoric disorder: the carolina premenstrual assessment scoring system (C-PASS). Am J Psychiatry 2017; 174: 51–9.10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.15121510CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henz, A, Ferreira, CF, Oderich, CL, Gallon, CW, Castro, JRS, Conzatti, M et al. Premenstrual Syndrome diagnosis: a comparative study between the Daily Record of Severity of Problems (DRSP) and the Premenstrual Symptoms Screening Tool (PSST). Rev Bras Ginecol E Obstetrícia 2018; 40: 20–5.Google ScholarPubMed
Han, J, Cha, Y, Kim, S. Effect of psychosocial interventions on the severity of premenstrual syndrome: a meta-analysis. J Psychosom Obstet Gynecol 2019; 40: 176–84.10.1080/0167482X.2018.1480606CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1 Flow diagram following Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) standards. PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Figure 1

Fig. 2 Overview of quality ratings for all studies, including those not meeting stricter eligibility criteria. Design adapted from66. Journal Citation Reports (2023).46 The customised tools used for each meta-analysis are available here: https://osf.io/mcjsb/. MA1, meta-analysis 1; MA2, meta-analysis 2; MA3, meta-analysis 3; PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Figure 2

Fig. 3 Forest plots for the three-level multilevel analyses. Black lines reflect the total precision of each study, considering the number of effect sizes and variability between them. Grey lines correspond to the study precision based on the sampling variance. J = number of effect sizes per study. The indicated Fisher’s z corresponds to r = 0.29, the logOdds corresponds to odds ratio 2.45. Depicted Cohen’s d values are derived from separate multilevel meta-analysis per cycle phase. Forest plots based on Fernández-Castilla et al.58 EDSP Study, Early Developmental Stages of Psychopathology Study; NC-ECA, North Carolina Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study; NSHLEW, National Study of Health and life Experiences of Women; PMDD, premenstrual dysphoric disorder; PMS, premenstrual syndrome.

Figure 3

Fig. 4 Sunset funnel plots displaying effect sizes and standard errors, with colour gradients indicating power.

Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 1

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 1(File)
File 30.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 2

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 2(File)
File 22.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 3

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 3(File)
File 77.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 4

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 4(File)
File 108.5 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 5

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 5(File)
File 121.1 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 6

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 6(File)
File 63.3 KB
Supplementary material: File

Bencker et al. supplementary material 7

Bencker et al. supplementary material
Download Bencker et al. supplementary material 7(File)
File 49.1 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.