Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-dbm8p Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-10T05:03:14.780Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

African harps as units of cultural evolution: a cladistic analysis on their morphology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  29 July 2025

Salomé Strauch*
Affiliation:
UMR 7206 Eco-anthropologie, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France
Guillaume Lecointre
Affiliation:
UMR 7205 Institut de Systématique, Evolution, Biodiversité, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France
Pierre Darlu
Affiliation:
UMR 7206 Eco-anthropologie, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France
Sylvie Le Bomin
Affiliation:
UMR 7206 Eco-anthropologie, Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle, Paris, France UMR 8223 Institut de Recherche en Musicologie, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France
*
Corresponding author: Salomé Strauch; Email: salome.strauch@edu.mnhn.fr

Abstract

In Africa, harps exhibit significant morphological diversity, yet their historical trajectory remains largely underexplored. Phylogenetic reconstruction methods offer valuable tools for understanding this diversity and the relationships between groups of harps. This study is among the first to apply one of these methods, cladistics, to the morphology of a musical instrument, analysing 318 harps and 83 characters. We present a well-resolved phylogenetic tree, which shows several clades corresponding to geocultural regions, in alignment with ethnomusicological classifications. We show that this tree robustly represents the patterns of vertical transmission in the cultural evolution of harp morphology across Africa, despite the limited contribution of several tested characters. Additionally, a comparison with previous research reveals that characters coding decorations exert a minimal influence on the vertical evolution of these musical instruments. These findings provide valuable insights into the cultural evolution of harps on a continental scale, offering a clearer understanding of their diversity and revealing major evolutionary mechanisms.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press.

1. Introduction

The harp is a musical instrument characterized by its strings stretched between the soundbox and the neck, aligned perpendicular to the body of the instrument (Hornbostel and Sachs, Reference Hornbostel (Von) and Sachs1961). Today, it is played on several continents, including Africa, from Mauritania to Uganda and in most Central African countries (DeVale, Reference DeVale and Libin2014; Leclair, Reference Leclair2007). The use of the harp on this continent for several millennia is evidenced by specimens found in Egypt, dated around 1500 BCE (Emerit et al., Reference Emerit, Guichard, Jeammet, Perrot, Thomas, Vendries, Vincent and Ziegler2017), as well as by painted depictions discovered in Chad (Bailloud, Reference Bailloud1997; Blench, Reference Blench2013; Menardi Noguera, Reference Menardi Noguera2023), Algeria (Kubik, Reference Kubik2010), and Egypt (Emerit et al., Reference Emerit, Guichard, Jeammet, Perrot, Thomas, Vendries, Vincent and Ziegler2017), dating from 1500 to 500 BCE. Africa is particularly notable for its rich diversity of harps, in terms of morphology (size, materials, shapes, decorations; Speranza, Reference Speranza, Bruguière and Speranza1999), their contexts of use (entertainment, ritual ceremonies, court music; Arom, Reference Arom2019; Bruguière, Reference Bruguière, Bruguière and Speranza1999), and the music they produce. Despite this, the history of the harp in Africa remains poorly understood, largely due to the preservation challenges of these instruments over time (Lawergren, Reference Lawergren1981) and the lack of written records due to oral transmission in local populations (Arom, Reference Arom1988; Shelemay, Reference Shelemay and Stone2008; Vansina, Reference Vansina and Ki-Zerbo1980). This implies that in Central Africa, the earliest mentions of the instrument are linked to European travellers’ accounts (Fürniss, Reference Fürniss, Bruguière and Speranza1999; Praetorius, Reference Praetorius1619). Furthermore, in the literature, African harps are more frequently studied as music-producing objects with cultural significance in specific contexts (Herndon, Reference Herndon1974; Johnson, Reference Johnson1995), rather than as material and cultural objects, resulting in fragmented descriptions (Strauch & Le Bomin, Reference Strauch, Le Bomin, Lawlor and Joyce2024).

Phylogenetic reconstruction methods, derived from life sciences, can help distinguish different groups of African harps and explain their diversity, potentially providing more insights into their history. These methods are increasingly applied to cultural materials, such as folktales (Tehrani, Reference Tehrani2013), languages (Ben Hamed et al., Reference Ben Hamed, Darlu and Vallée2005; Gaillard-Corvaglia et al., Reference Gaillard-Corvaglia, Léonard and Darlu2008; Gray & Jordan, Reference Gray and Jordan2000; Koile et al., Reference Koile, Greenhill, Blasi, Bouckaert and Gray2022), lithic assemblages (Rineau et al., Reference Rineau, Moncel and Zeitoun2023), initiation rites (Bentley et al., Reference Bentley, Moritz, Ruck and O’Brien2021), myths (d’Huy, Reference d’Huy2012, Reference d’Huy2013), concepts (Charbonnat et al., Reference Charbonnat, Ben Hamed and Lecointre2014), manuscripts (Platnick & Cameron, Reference Platnick and Cameron1977; Robinson, Reference Robinson2016; Schmidt et al., Reference Schmidt, Macé and Weiler2003), figures of the tree of life (Fisler et al., Reference Fisler, Crémière, Darlu and Lecointre2020), woven textiles (Buckley, Reference Buckley2012), and Buddha statues (Marwick, Reference Marwick, Tjoa-Bonatz, Reinecke and Bonatz2012). Although music is a shared cultural heritage among all past and present human populations, only a few phylogenetic analyses have been conducted in this field (Aguirre-Fernández et al., Reference Aguirre-Fernández, Barbieri, Graff, Pérez de Arce, Moreno and Sánchez-Villagra2021; Le Bomin et al, Reference Le Bomin, Lecointre and Heyer2016; Tëmkin & Eldredge, Reference Tëmkin and Eldredge2007; Windram et al., Reference Windram, Charlston, Tomita and Howe2022).

Applying phylogenetic reconstruction methods to African harps is therefore particularly relevant, first because their history and diversity are underexplored, and second because, as musical instruments, they hold significant cultural value across various environments, lifestyles, and sociocultural practices of the populations that use them. Furthermore, the deep evolutionary history of musical instruments (including harps) in hunter-gatherer populations in Central Africa (Padilla-Iglesias et al., Reference Padilla-Iglesias, Blanco-Portillo, Pricop, Ioannidis, Bickel, Manica, Vinicius and Migliano2024) shows the interest for such phylogenetic analyses. One such method, cladistics, has previously been used to study the evolution of the morphology and decorations of 318 African harps (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023). The resulting phylogenetic tree showed three main groups of harps. These groups align with the geographical distributions documented in the literature and are predominantly represented by harps from Gabon, the Democratic Republic of Congo, and Uganda. The results also showed that the evolution of the morphology and decorations of African is not solely driven by vertical transmission processes (from one generation to the next), as there is a significant proportion of innovations that are transmitted horizontally (between peers).

Here, we distinguish between morphological characteristics, which deal with the organological and functional features of harps, and decorative characteristics, which have no impact on the music produced and are intended to decorate the harp (i.e. carvings, paintings, and engravings). In this article, we present a cladistic analysis focused solely on the morphology of these harps. It is therefore identical to previous analysis (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023), except for the exclusion of decorative characters. This enables a direct comparison between the two analyses and allows us to assess the impact of decorative features on the topology of the phylogenetic tree (i.e. by determining whether its structure changes and how these changes affect the relationships between harps). This approach also narrows the focus to the morphological evolution of the musical instruments. However, it is important to note that the reverse is not feasible, as not all harps are decorated, which would prevent a cladistic analysis focusing solely on decorative features for the same 318 harps. Our goals are therefore to (1) identify groups of harps and the morphological features that define them; (2) discuss the role of different transmission processes (vertical, horizontal) of innovations in the evolution of African harp morphology, and their sharing as a result of selective pressures or retroactive reappearances; and (3) assess the impact of removing decorative features on the resulting phylogenetic tree by comparing it to the previously obtained one.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Cladistics: theoretical principles and parsimony method

Phylogenetic systematics, or cladistics, is an approach to systematics based on the kinship relationships between evolutionary units and on inherited modifications (Hennig, Reference Hennig1966). In this study, we employ the terms evolutionary unit (EU) (Meacham, Reference Meacham1984) and individual (Tëmkin & Eldredge, Reference Tëmkin, Eldredge, Serrelli and Gontier2015), as they are applicable to both biological and cultural entities. An EU is a group of individuals (as a species), whereas an individual is a tangible representative of that EU (as a living organism).

Among cladistic methods, parsimony is the one employed in this study. Its goal is to produce phylogenetic trees and select the tree or trees that minimize the number of evolutionary changes. A cladogram (a phylogenetic tree produced by cladistic methods) is generated through the parsimony analysis of a EUs–characters matrix. This matrix is a two-dimensional table, with the EUs under study listed in rows and the characters listed in columns. A character has at least two states and corresponds to an attribute observed on all individuals of an EU that exhibits at least two observable forms (Darlu et al., Reference Darlu, Tassy, d’Haese and Zaragüeta I Bagils2019). For instance, if the character ‘Number of strings’ has the states ‘5 strings’, ‘6 strings’, and ‘8 strings’, it reflects a specific attribute of harps (they have strings), and the states represent observable forms (some harps have 5 strings, others have 6, and some have 8). However, defining a character and its states is not merely a description of an attribute and its forms; it formalizes the hypothesis that two forms observed in two EUs are similar enough to be derived from a single ancestral form (Barriel, Reference Barriel, Heams, Huneman, Lecointre and Silberstein2011; Tassy, Reference Tassy2022). When defining a character and assigning different states to different EUs, the hypothesis is that individuals (and thus the EUs to which they belong) sharing a particular form of an attribute (e.g. harps with 5 strings) are more closely related to each other than to individuals (and the EUs they belong to) with a different form of that attribute (e.g. harps with 6 and 8 strings). The way a character is defined reflects the hypothesis being tested in the analysis. For instance, if it is hypothesized that 8-string harps evolved separately from 5-string and 6-string harps, coding the character ‘Number of Strings’ with three states to represent each observable form (‘5 strings’, ‘6 strings’, and ‘8 strings’) does not formalize this evolutionary hypothesis. In contrast, defining the character with the two states ‘fewer than 8 strings’ and ‘8 strings’ does. Thus, the definition of character states represents a primary homology (de Pinna, Reference de Pinna1991): it is a hypothesis of homology, that is, of identity, which serves as an argument for determining the evolutionary relationships between EUs. The EUs–characters matrix therefore records all the states (in the cells) for each character (in the columns) exhibited by the EUs (in the rows) (Pleijel, Reference Pleijel1995).

A cladogram is the result of a parsimony analysis conducted on the EUs–characters matrix. It groups together the studied EUs by maximizing the contiguity of identical character states (a principle of coherence; Rieppel, Reference Rieppel2004, Reference Rieppel, Deleporte and Lecointre2005). There are three consequences: it maximizes hierarchical congruence between the groupings of EUs supported by different character states; it minimizes the number of steps, that is, the number of character state changes (parsimony); and if the graph is read through the theory of evolution, attributes shared by EUs are inherited from a common ancestor.

On the cladogram, the EUs can belong to monophyletic groups, or clades, defined by one or more synapomorphies. A synapomorphy is a derived character state shared by all members of a clade; it represents the form of an attribute that originated in a single ancestral EU and was inherited by all its descendants (Hennig, Reference Hennig1966; Tassy & Fischer, Reference Tassy and Fischer2021). It is a character state for which the hypothesis of homology has not been refuted, representing a secondary homology (de Pinna, Reference de Pinna1991). Secondary homology refers to a relationship between two structures that are similar enough to be considered inherited from the same ancestral structure, that is, homologous structures (Bock, Reference Bock1973; Brower & Schawaroch, Reference Brower and Schawaroch1996). The hypothesis of homology (i.e. potential homology or primary homology) is tested through cladistic analysis and is thus central to it (Cracraft, Reference Cracraft1981). As Patterson (Reference Patterson, Joysey and Friday1982: 34) notes, ‘The idea that every worthwhile hypothesis of homology specifies a hierarchy of groups is all I wish to emphasize here. The force of a hypothesis of homology is that the inclusive group is monophyletic, by virtue of the homology.’

Homoplastic characters states are forms of attributes that are similar but not shared by an ancestral EU of all its descendants. The analysis has revealed these states to be different among these EUs, that is, that the hypothesis of homology is refuted.

According to cladistic theory (conceptualized within the life sciences), it is assumed that nature can be ‘ordered in a single specifiable pattern which can be represented by a branching diagram or hierarchical classification’ (Platnick, Reference Platnick1979: 538). Therefore, in cladistic methods, transmission can only be vertical, meaning that a given (non-homoplastic) character state is inherited from an ancestral EU by all its descendants.

However, it is recognized that transmission processes are as diverse for cultural EUs as they are for some biological units: innovations can be transmitted vertically (from an ancestor to its descendants), horizontally (between descendants of different ancestors), or obliquely (from an ancestor to the descendants of another ancestor) (Creanza et al., Reference Creanza, Kolodny and Feldman2017; L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, Reference Cavalli-Sforza1986). For simplicity, both horizontal and oblique transmission processes will be referred to as horizontal transmission (Borgerhoff Mulder et al., Reference Borgerhoff Mulder, Nunn and Towner2006). This type of transmission cannot be unambiguously identified by cladistic methods, as they only consider vertical transmission and homoplasy can have different causes: horizontal transmission, but also reversion, similar selection pressure (e.g. availability of materials, musical styles), independent appearances, or character coding errors.

This implies that all character states transmitted horizontally will be considered homoplastic by the analysis, that is, as similar and not shared by an ancestral EU and all of its descendants. Conversely, one could also assume that homoplastic character states may be due to horizontal transfer. Nevertheless, applying these methods to cultural material allows for testing numerous hypotheses and determining hierarchical relationships between the considered EUs. These methods account for the diversity resulting from evolution, whether biological or cultural.

2.2. The 318 harps of the matrix

In Africa, harps consistently feature a soundbox, a neck and strings, with the vast majority also including a soundboard, a string holder, and tuning pegs. Additionally, some harps have a soundbox extension, a shelf, and/or a base (Fig. 1). Across the continent, each of these structural components exhibits variation in size, shape, material, and ornamentation, which may include engraving, carving, and/or painting.

Figure 1. Illustration of two harps and their constituent parts. (A) Orungu harp described in Gabon by Sylvie Le Bomin, in the field. (B) Ngbaka harp from Democratic Republic of Congo described by Noé Coussot at the AfricaMuseum (Belgium).

The cladistic analysis is conducted on the same 318 harps as those used by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023), including 314 African harps for the ingroup and four non-African harps for the outgroup (Supplementary Table S1).

The country and population of attribution are known for all harps on the ingroup: they come from 15 different countries and are attributed to 71 different populations. They were all described between 2016 and 2023. Among them, 68.5% were described in European and African museums, 30.9% in the field, and 0.6% in private collections (Fig. 2). Each harp is complete or is missing only a few pegs and/or strings. Those missing are considered identical to those present on the harp in the analysis.

Figure 2. Geographic distribution of the 314 African harps in the matrix by country of attribution, showing the number of harps per country and the proportion described in museums, in the field, or in private collections.

The four non-African harps added to the matrix serve as the outgroup, that is, they will be used to root the produced cladogram (Barriel & Tassy, Reference Barriel and Tassy1998; Maddison et al., Reference Maddison, Donoghue and Maddison1984). Two are attributed to Afghanistan, one to Burma, and one to Russia. They were described in 2023 at the Pitt Rivers Museum (England) and the Scenkonstmuseet (Sweden).

2.3. The 83 characters of the matrix

The 83 characters used in this analysis (Supplementary Table S2) are derived from the matrix used by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023). Characters coding for decorations (engravings, paintings, and carvings) were either removed or modified, so that the matrix now contains only morphological characters (Table 1). They were defined on the basis of a data set compiled between 2016 and 2023, which describes 700 harps using, in particular, 223 morphological and decorative parameters. All descriptive parameters were reviewed to produce this matrix, considering their relevance from an evolutionary point of view (i.e. do they convey, as they stand, a homology hypothesis?) to determine whether they could be directly converted into characters, whether they needed to be modified, or whether they were considered irrelevant (see Strauch, Reference Strauch2023: 107–115 for more details). In this study, 21.7% of characters are equivalent to the corresponding parameters, 39.8% are modified from the associated parameters, and 38.6% are created for the analysis.

Table 1. Number of characters associated with the different parts of the harp in both analyses

The three modified characters originally coded for the presence and engraving of the soundbox extension, the shelf, and the seal reinforcement, with three states: ‘absent’, ‘present without engraving’, and ‘present with engraving’. These characters were renamed ‘Presence of a soundbox extension’, ‘Presence of a shelf’, and ‘Presence of a seal reinforcement’ and now have the two states ‘present’ and ‘absent’.

Among the 38 excluded characters, three coded for the colours of the paintings, five for the presence of engraving on various harp parts, one for the presence of paint on the soundboard, and 29 for the carvings on the harp. As mentioned previously, removing decorative characters from the matrix allows for an evaluation of their impact on the topology of the resulting cladogram. It also significantly reduces the number of missing data in the matrix (coded as ‘?’), with the one used by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023) containing 19% missing data compared to 9.60% in this analysis. This is because not all harps are carved and when one is, not all its constituent parts are carved. Such characters cannot be coded for a non-carved harp and are therefore inapplicable. They can lead to impossible state reconstructions for nodes in the cladogram (Platnick et al., Reference Platnick, Griswold and Coddington1991), even though some authors believe that it is irrelevant to exclude characters simply because of their missing data (Poe & Wiens, Reference Poe, Wiens and Wiens2000).

Of the 83 characters in the matrix, seven characters code for continuous measurements (1–3: maximum overall length, height, and width of the harp; 12, 13: soundbox length and maximum width in the middle of the soundbox; 66, 67: neck straight length and neck curved length to straight length ratio). They were discretized by considering four classes of the same amplitude for each character, and therefore four states. Additional harp measurements were not taken into account in the analyses, as their discretization poses several problems in cladistics. For example, this procedure relies on many arbitrary choices, and the addition or removal of harps can potentially alter intervals and therefore character states (Stevens, Reference Stevens1991; Thiele, Reference Thiele1993).

Fifty-three characters are binary and 30 are multistate. One character is uninformative (9: Presence of metallic plates next to the string holder), that is, only one harp has the state ‘1: Presence’ and the other 317 have the state ‘0: Absence’. For each character, the attribution of a given state to a harp has been made based on its photographs and its description. With the exception of modified characters, no character states have been changed between the matrix used by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023) and that of this analysis.

2.4. The parsimony analysis

The matrix includes 318 harps and 83 characters (Supplementary Table S3). The parsimony analysis was performed with PAUP* 4.0a169 (Swofford, Reference Swofford2003), with a heuristic search, using random stepwise addition with the TBR (Tree Bisection and Reconnection) branch-swapping algorithm. Both convergences and reversals were allowed, following Wagner parsimony. ACCTRAN optimization (‘accelerated transformation’), which favours reversals, was applied here (Darlu et al., Reference Darlu, Tassy, d’Haese and Zaragüeta I Bagils2019; Swofford & Maddison, Reference Swofford, Maddison and Mayden1992) because it maximizes the number of secondary homologies in the tree (de Pinna, Reference de Pinna1991). The ‘max-length = 0’ rule was enforced, meaning that any unsupported branch in the tree, under all possible character optimizations, was collapsed (i.e. when its maximum length is 0). All characters were treated as unordered, meaning that any transformation from one state to another for each character ‘costs’ one step (Fitch, Reference Fitch1971). All traits were supposed to evolve independently and neither characters nor state transformations were weighted. The resulting trees were rooted using the four outgroups.

3. Results

The parsimony analysis provided 500,000 most parsimonious trees of 1581 steps. They are summarized via majority-rule (50%) consensus tree of 1581 steps (Fig. 3). This type of consensus only retains clades present on more than 50% of the trees (Margush & McMorris, Reference Margush and McMorris1981). Consistency and retention indexes (CI and RI) are used as measures of homoplasy in the matrix related to the most parsimonious solutions and vary between 0 and 1. Considering ${L_m}$ the minimum number of transformations if the characters are binary, ${L_o}$ the length of the most parsimonious tree, and $G$ the number of steps if all changes occur along the terminal branches, they are defined as $CI = \frac{{{L_m}}}{{{L_o}}}$ (Kluge & Farris, Reference Kluge and Farris1969) and $RI = \frac{{G - {L_o}}}{{G - {L_m}}}$ (Archie, Reference Archie1989; Farris, Reference Farris1989).

Figure 3. Cladogram of the majority-rule (50%) consensus tree (1581 steps), retained from 500,000 trees (1581 steps). Branch lengths are not informative. The cladogram is rooted with the four outgroup harps, circled in grey. The identifiers of the harps represented by at least four harps in the matrix are colour-coded according to their country of origin: Democratic Republic of the Congo (dark blue), Gabon (orange), Cameroon (light blue), Central African Republic (red), Uganda (green), Nigeria (dark yellow), and South Sudan (plum). The ingroup is marked by a red dot at its node. The six clades that are numbered in the figure (1–6) correspond to those that are examined in detail in this paper, with the first three colour-coded in blue, green, and orange, respectively, based on the main geographical attribution of the harps composing each clade. The numbers in the yellow circles correspond to the proportion of trees where the node is retained, while white boxes indicate the character state transformations at each node, with the state noted in superscript.

Although these indices are calculated differently, they can be interpreted in a similar manner: ‘if the tree fits the data perfectly, with no extra steps, they take on the value of 1’ (Goloboff, Reference Goloboff1991: 218), and their values decrease as homoplasy increases. For this consensus tree, CI = 0.092 and RI = 0.706.

The ingroup is monophyletic, comprising the 314 African harps analysed. This clade is characterized by a soundbox length between 12 and 36.625 cm (character 12, state 0: 120), the absence of soundholes in the upper right (490) and lower left (520) parts of the soundboard, the presence of an internal string holder (561), a neck plugged in the soundbox (650), and twisted strings (821). The transformations for characters 56 and 82 are ambiguous, meaning they could have occurred elsewhere in the tree without additional steps under a different optimization (Agnarsson & Miller, Reference Agnarsson and Miller2008).

Only two clades include all the harps from a single country, and only those harps: the two harps from Afghanistan are grouped together within the outgroup and the two harps from Liberia are more closely related to each other than to the harp from Angola in clade 4 (Fig. 3). No other clade meets both conditions of containing only harps from a given country and all the harps from that country. However, six clades can be distinguished, with the first three containing 97.45% of the harps in the ingroup. The vast majority (or at least half, depending on the country) of the harps from a given country can be assigned to one of these main clades (Table 2).

Table 2. Number of harps of the ingroup by country of attribution in each clade

The term ‘main clade’ is used here to refer to the clade containing the majority of harps attributed to a given country. Clade 1 is the main clade for the harps from the Democratic Republic of the Congo (DRC), the Central African Republic (CAR), Nigeria, Sudan, and Tanzania. Clade 2 is the main clade for the harps from Uganda, and clade 3 is the main clade for the harps from Gabon, Cameroon, and Equatorial Guinea (EQG). Clade 4 is the main clade for the harps from Liberia and Angola. The harps from the Republic of the Congo (ROC) are equally divided between clades 1 and 3, and the harps from Kenya are 50% in clade 3 and 50% in clade 5.

Clade 1 includes 154 harps (Fig. 3), characterized by a cylindrical soundbox (140) with a domed bottom (151) and tight lacing that attaches the soundboard to the soundbox (371). The transformations for characters 14 and 37 are ambiguous.

Clade 2 includes 40 harps, characterized by the presence of nails on the front of the harp to attach the soundboard to the soundbox (321), by the fact that the lacing that attaches the soundboard is loose (381) and absent from the back of the harp (360), and by the absence of a soundhole in the lower right of the soundboard (540). Only the transformation for character 36 is unambiguous.

Clade 3 includes 112 harps, characterized by the presence of a shelf (51), a neck resting on the soundbox (653), pegs with a choked knob (701), and the presence of a gap between the body and the head of the pegs (751). None of the transformations are ambiguous.

Clade 4 includes three harps, characterized by a maximum overall width between 26.9 and 37.95 cm (32), an ellipsoidal morphology of the soundbox (143) and the soundboard (193), wooden and metal nails (282), absence of soundholes (460), an external string holder (562), strings tied to or around the string holder (600), and the absence of pegs (680). The transformations for characters 3, 14, 19, and 28 are ambiguous.

Clade 5 includes three harps, characterized by the presence of hairs on all the soundboard (401), the presence of only one soundhole (461), and pegs that do not increase in diameter between the body and the head (780). The transformation for character 40 is ambiguous.

Clade 6 includes two harps, characterized by the presence of a shelf (51), a bucket-shaped soundbox (145), an ellipsoidal soundboard (193) made of reptile skin (211), notched pegs (761), and the presence of five strings (832). The transformations for characters 14 and 19 are ambiguous.

CI and RI can also be computed for individual characters (Supplementary Table S4). Only six characters have a CI of 1 (7.23% of all characters): they code for the presence of a sound-modifying element on the neck (7), presence of metallic plates next to the string holder (9), soundbox material (11), presence of mirlitons (44), presence of holes in the string holder (59), and presence of pegs (68). Six additional characters have an CI of 0.5 (7.23%): they code for the presence of a bone collar around the string holder (8), presence of a musical instrument attached to the harp (10), presence of a soundboard (18), dorsal position of the nails (33), soundholes location (45), and peg mounting angle (73). All other characters have a CI below 0.5 (85.54%): 41 of them have a CI less than or equal to 0.1 (49.40%) and four have a CI below 0.03. They code for the presence of a suspension cord (43), presence of a gap between the body and the head of the pegs (75), increased peg diameter (78), and presence of twists on the strings (82).

Characters with a CI of 1 also have an RI of 1 (6.02% of all characters), except for character 9 as it is uninformative. Fifty-three characters have an RI greater than or equal to 0.5 (63.85%) and 25 have an RI below 0.5 (30.12%), including eight characters with an RI equal to 0. They code for the presence of a bone collar around the string holder (8), presence of metallic plates next to the string holder (9), presence of a musical instrument attached to the harp (10), presence of seams to attach the soundboard (26), presence of ligatures to attach the soundboard (27), soundholes location (45), presence of a fork on the pegs (77), and presence of a ridge on the pegs (80).

4. Discussion

4.1. Main clades and geographic distribution of harps

Apart from the harps from Liberia, harps attributed to the same country within the ingroup do not form monophyletic groups on the cladogram (Fig. 3). However, they are predominantly grouped within the same main clade (Table 2). For instance, whereas 95.9% of harps from the DRC are found in the main clade 1, three harps attributed to this country are located in clade 2. Two of these are manufactured by the Alur people, who inhabit both the DRC and Uganda (Tamisier, Reference Tamisier1998): this may explain the placement of these two DRC harps within clade 2, a clade primarily represented by Ugandan harps. The third harp is attributed to the Nyamwezi people, a population based in Tanzania (Tamisier, Reference Tamisier1998) but not in the DRC. The geographical and ethnological information associated with the harp is therefore inconsistent, and at least one of them can be assumed to have been incorrectly recorded in museum collections (Gansemans, Reference Gansemans1996). A similar situation is thought to have occurred with the only harp attributed to Gabon that falls outside clade 3. It is in clade 1 and exhibits morphological characteristics that are significantly different from the other Gabonese harps studied. Such attribution errors may explain the unexpected position in another clade of the affected harps.

It can also be explained by the geographical position of the attributed country and by where the attributed population live. For example, South Sudan is represented by four harps in the matrix: half are placed in clade 1 and are attributed to the Zande population, which is present in South Sudan, the DRC, and CAR. As shown by our results (Table 2), clade 1 is the main clade for the DRC and CAR harps. The other half of the South Sudanese harps are in clade 2 and are attributed to the Acholi population, which is found in South Sudan and Uganda (clade 2 being the main clade of Ugandan harps). Given South Sudan’s geographical position between the DRC, CAR (clade 1), and Uganda (clade 2), and the distribution of the Zande and Acholi populations across these countries, the division of South Sudanese harps between clades 1 and 2 is consistent. A similar pattern is observed for harps from the ROC, which are split between clades 1 and 3. Geographically, ROC is located between Gabon and the DRC. The ROC harp in clade 1 is attributed to the Ngbaka people, who also reside in the DRC and CAR, whereas the ROC harp in clade 3 is associated with the Kele people, a population also found in Gabon (clade 3 being the main clade for Gabonese harps).

Regarding the harps attributed to Cameroon, 82.35% are placed in clade 3 and 17.65% in clade 1. All Cameroonian harps from clade 3 are attributed to the Fang Ntumu population and come from the South region, near the border with Gabon and Equatorial Guinea (main clade 3). In contrast, the three Cameroonian harps from clade 1 are attributed to the North and Far North regions, bordering Nigeria and Chad (main clade 1). These harps are linked to three different populations: the two Fali and Uldeme harps are grouped together, whereas the Tupuri harp is positioned much further apart within clade 1 (Fig. 3). Thus, the results clearly demonstrate a division of Cameroonian harps between clades 1 and 3 based on their region of attribution, which aligns with the well-documented differences in harp morphology and usage between the northern and southern regions of the country (Fernando, Reference Fernando, Bruguière and Speranza1999; Fürniss, Reference Fürniss, Bruguière and Speranza1999; Rivière, Reference Rivière1999).

Although the main clade of Ugandan harps is clade 2, four harps from this country are found in clade 1, likely due to the geographic proximity between Uganda and the DRC. Clade 6 consists of two Ugandan harps, which form a sister group to all other harps within the ingroup. These two harps are attributed to the Twa population, a collective term encompassing many Pygmy groups across the DRC, ROC, and Uganda (Bahuchet, Reference Bahuchet2012). Notably, these are the only harps in the matrix specifically made for tourists, which likely accounts for their markedly different morphological characteristics compared to other Ugandan harps and the rest of the ingroup.

Thus, although harps from the same country do not form a monophyletic group within the ingroup, except for those from Liberia, a clear geographical coherence is still observed in the cladogram (Fig. 4). Indeed, harps from the same geographic area are primarily grouped together, although there are exceptions that can be explained by, in particular, their manufacture (e.g. tourist harps), misidentification in museums, or their attribution data (countries, regions, and populations). Therefore, the use of their country of attribution as a distinguishing criterion has its limits, as the use of harps in Africa is largely determined by the populations who play them, and the distribution of these populations does not align with African administrative borders, which are recent and the result of colonialism, with little relevance at the local scale (Coquery-Vidrovitch, Reference Coquery-Vidrovitch2012). However, country attribution is more frequently known in museums, and above all more reliable, than ethnological designation (population, linguistic group) or more precise geographical identifications (region, town, etc.). Additionally, for some countries that are equally represented in two clades (South Sudan, ROC, Kenya, Chad), these results should be interpreted cautiously due to the small number of harps attributed to those countries (Table 2). It would be valuable to include more harps from each country to confirm their placement in one clade or another.

Figure 4. Geographical distribution of harps in the main clades defined on the cladogram of the majority-rule (50%) consensus. For countries with harps attributed equally to two main clades, their assignment to these clades is represented by two arbitrarily distributed colours.

The first three main clades identified in this analysis confirm numerous geographic descriptions found in the literature, where three groups of harps are typically distinguished. Jadinon (Reference Jadinon and Omasombo2014) describes a continuous change in harp morphology along the axis connecting southern Cameroon, Gabon, EQG, DRC, and Uganda. Similarly, Wachsmann (Reference Wachsmann1964) defined and mapped three groups of harps based on how the neck is attached to the soundbox, without, however, reference to countries or borders. Yet, these groups align closely with the composition of the first three main clades identified here.

As with country attributions, harps assigned to the same population do not form monophyletic groups and are more widely dispersed within a given main clade. This indicates that it is not possible to determine a harp’s population of origin based solely on its morphological features. Nevertheless, such attribution is often made in ethnomusicology (de Dampierre, Reference de Dampierre1991; Speranza, Reference Speranza and de Dampierre1995), suggesting that ‘the information available in museums is incomplete and, unfortunately, often incorrect’ (Speranza, Reference Speranza, Bruguière and Speranza1999: 74).

4.2. Characters, phylogenetic signal, and transmission of innovations

The application of cladistics to cultural data encounters a major issue, frequently discussed in the literature (Boyd et al., Reference Boyd, Bogerhoff-Mulder, Durham, Richerson, Weignart, Mitchel, Richerson and Maasen1997; Cavalli-Sforza & Feldman, Reference Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman1981; Greenhill et al., Reference Greenhill, Currie and Gray2009; Guglielmino et al., Reference Guglielmino, Viganotti, Hewlett and Cavalli-Sforza1995; Lukas et al., Reference Lukas, Towner and Borgerhoff Mulder2021; Steele et al., Reference Steele, Jordan and Cochrane2010; Tëmkin, Reference Tëmkin, Panebianco and Serrelli2016): cultural innovations can be transmitted either vertically (from an ancestor to all its descendants) or horizontally (between descendants of different ancestors or from one ancestor to the descendants of others). Vertical transmission of innovations produces homologies and implies a hierarchical pattern (nested sets of EUs from the most general attributes to the most particular attributes), which is represented by cladograms. Conversely, horizontal transmission leads to similarities that are not inherited, resulting in a non-hierarchical evolutionary pattern. Thus, horizontal transmission cannot be captured by cladistic methods, which were developed to investigate biological evolution (Hennig, Reference Hennig1966). Although cladistics can be useful when applied to cultural data, these methods may not be entirely suited to them. Specifically, cladistic methods are well adapted to biological evolution and to the vertical transmission of cultural traits (L. Cavalli-Sforza, Reference Cavalli-Sforza2005), whereas horizontal transmission of cultural traits can significantly impact the results obtained (Currie et al., Reference Currie, Greenhill and Mace2010). Therefore, the application of cladistics in cultural evolution is only relevant when the innovations being studied are transmitted primarily or exclusively through vertical transmission. Consistency and retention indices help estimate the tree-likeness of the data (Bryant et al., Reference Bryant, Filimon, Gray, Mace, Holden and Shennan2005) and the reliability of the results – that is, to what extent the characters and their states, as encoded in the matrix, reflect a hierarchical structure and, thus, are homologous (and vertically transmitted). These indices are frequently used in cultural evolution studies to detect horizontal transmission (Collard et al., Reference Collard, Shennan and Tehrani2006; Fisler et al., Reference Fisler, Crémière, Darlu and Lecointre2020; Nunn et al., Reference Nunn, Arnold, Matthews and Mulder2010).

The analysis showed that 7.23% of the characters have a CI of 1, whereas 85.54% have a CI below 0.5. Additionally, 36.14% of characters have an RI above 0.7, and 30.12% have an RI below 0.5. Therefore, while some characters are fully homologous (and thus vertically transmitted), the majority are not. Nevertheless, the cladogram is well resolved, exhibiting relatively few polytomies, and its nodes are recovered in nearly all 500,000 trees retrieved by the analysis. Moreover, our results show that the geographical distribution of the main clades on the cladogram is both consistent per se and aligned with descriptions from the literature (Fig. 4). Such a structured tree could not have been produced if the characters were random. This suggests that the hierarchical structure observed in the consensus tree, although weakly supported by the characters (as indicated by the low CI and RI values), lacks a viable alternative hypothesis based on the data used. On another argument line, one must keep in mind that there is no contradiction between low CIs and RIs and a well-resolved cladogram. This conjunction can be explained by the fact that slightly homoplastic characters (e.g. binary characters with two changes in the tree, then with a CI of 0.5) can provide structure in a large cladogram (Philippe et al., Reference Philippe, Lecointre, Lê and Le Guyader1996) if changes of the homoplastic character state are well separated by several nodes. Moreover, it is noteworthy that these indices yield contradictory results in this study, both for numerous characters and the cladogram (CI = 0.092, RI = 0.706): the former indicates a high degree of homoplasy, whereas the latter suggests the opposite. Several biases are associated with the use of these indices (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023), and, as Archie highlights, ‘it is not apparent from the literature how CI and RI values should be used’ (Archie, Reference Archie, Sanderson and Hufford1996: 181).

Despite this, there is still a significant portion of our data set that does not contribute to the vertical evolution of harp morphology, which could be due to several factors: (1) the homology hypothesis is still assumed to be correct, but the way the character is coded does not adequately describe it; (2) the character may be transmitted horizontally, thus not following a hierarchical pattern; or (3) the character may not be evolutionarily relevant (e.g. if its transformations are random) and should be excluded from the cladistic analysis. It would therefore be beneficial to re-examine the entire set of characters and their transformations across the cladogram, identifying those that should be recoded or removed to reduce ‘noise’ in the data set and maximize the vertical fraction of the phylogenetic signal within the matrix.

For example, we can examine the 12 characters with a CI of 1 (indicating that the number of transformations from one state to another is minimal on the tree) or 0.5 (indicating that these characters require twice as many steps as theoretically necessary). Among them, five relate to features that alter the sound produced by the harp: buzzers (characters 7, 8, 9), mirlitons (character 44), and instruments attached to the harp (character 10). These characters, therefore, exhibit a strong phylogenetic signal (compared to other characters) both individually and collectively, as they all concern the modification of the harp’s natural sound. They can be retained for future analyses.

Similarly, the four characters with a CI below 0.03 are particularly homoplastic (and thus not vertically transmitted). Two of these characters involve tuning pegs (characters 75 and 78), and while they theoretically require only one transformation (as they have only two states), the cladogram shows 38 transformations for character 75 and 35 transformations for character 78. Therefore, these characters do not contribute to the vertical evolution of harp morphology. Moreover, the use of the harp and the music it produces are not constrained by the morphology of the pegs, as long as they are long enough to attach the strings and wide enough to fit into the neck. This suggests that the morphology of the pegs is relatively free, allowing for a high degree of shape variability, where selection may be more arbitrary and individual rather than codified. Therefore, we assume that these characters are not evolutionarily relevant and should be excluded from future analyses.

The seven characters coding for discretized continuous measurements (characters 1, 2, 3, 12, 13, 66, and 67) are also homoplastic. Their CI is consistently below 0.1, with each character showing 38–60 transformations on the cladogram. However, these characters provide information on the general dimensions of the harp, its soundbox, and its neck. These dimensions indirectly reflect the instrument’s potential use and context of application (e.g. can it be played while walking, or must it be placed on the ground?). Therefore, we suggest that the reason these characters do not contribute to the vertical evolution of harp morphology is that their coding does not capture potential homologies. In future analyses, it may be worthwhile to order the states of these characters (Farris, Reference Farris1970) or to test their relevance with an alternative coding approach, by transforming them into qualitative variables or into quantitative continuous characters (Goloboff et al., Reference Goloboff, Mattoni and Quinteros2006).

4.3. Morphology and decorations of African harps

In cladistics, a classical method for testing the ‘noise’ introduced by certain characters is to exclude them from the analysis. This has been done in the present study, which focuses solely on morphological characters, as the 41 characters coding for harp decorations included in Strauch’s analysis (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023) were removed or reformulated (Table 1). Strauch’s study produced a 50% majority-rule consensus of one million most parsimonious trees, with a length of 2400 steps. It was well-resolved, with the vast majority of its nodes consistently recovered in the one million trees. Its CI was 0.092, and its RI was 0.650. The CIs of the two analyses are therefore identical, but the RI for the morphology-only analysis is slightly higher (RI = 0.706).

The cladogram obtained in our study shows the same geographical consistency as the one presented by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023: 120), with the main clades including harps from the same countries as in that study. Figure 4, inspired by the figure produced by Strauch (Reference Strauch2023: 134), illustrates similar distributions. The only difference concerns the two harps from Liberia and the one from Angola: in this study, they are grouped together in clade 4 (Fig. 4), whereas they are included within the main clade of Gabonese harps in the analysis that incorporated decorative elements (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023).

In both studies, harps from the same countries are generally distributed similarly across the main clades. However, some differences can be observed. The proportion of harps from three countries (Cameroon, Gabon, Uganda) found in their respective main clades slightly decreases in this analysis compared to Strauch’s (Reference Strauch2023) by 0.55%, 1.05%, and 2.50%, respectively. Conversely, this proportion increases notably for the CAR, the DRC, and Nigeria (5.24%, 13.10%, and 25%, respectively) in this analysis.

The results obtained by considering only the morphology of the harps are overall similar to those obtained when also taking their decorations (sculptures, paintings, engravings) into account, in terms of topology, indices (CI and RI), and geographical congruence of the cladogram. This indicates that the characters coding for decorations do not have a major impact on the vertical evolution of harps; in other words, they do not contribute significantly enough to induce noticeable changes in the cladogram. The only observed changes after removing these characters are generally positive, as the RI is higher, and the proportion of harps from the DRC, CAR, and Nigeria found in their main clade 1 is increased in this analysis. It is therefore interesting to note that decorations do not exert a significant impact on the evolution of harps compared to their morphology, despite the fact that they are often prioritized, particularly anthropomorphic sculptures, in various categorizations of harps (Strauch & Le Bomin, Reference Strauch, Le Bomin, Lawlor and Joyce2024). This study highlights the importance of examining the morphology of harps as a whole in their description and study, rather than focusing solely on their decorations.

5. Conclusion

This study is among the first to apply cladistic methods to the morphology of a musical instrument (Strauch, Reference Strauch2023; Tëmkin & Eldredge, Reference Tëmkin and Eldredge2007) and to examine such an extensive data set (318 harps, 83 characters). The consensus cladogram reveals several major clades, whose geographical distribution aligns with established ethnomusicological descriptions. These groups are primarily represented by harps from the DRC, Gabon, Uganda, and Liberia. However, the hierarchical structure of the cladogram is weakly supported by the characters, indicating that the majority of them, as coded in the matrix, do not significantly contribute to the vertical evolution of harp morphology. Additionally, the results were compared to those of Strauch (Reference Strauch2023), showing that harp decorations have only a minor influence on vertical evolutionary patterns compared to morphological features.

This study highlights the relevance of cladistic methods when applied to cultural artefacts and has successfully characterized the evolution of harps on a continent-wide scale in Africa. In relation to the field of ethnomusicology, this research invalidates certain practices, such as attributing a harp to a specific population based solely on its morphological similarity to others, and prioritizing sculptures as key elements in the morphological descriptions of harps.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/ehs.2025.10009.

Acknowledgements

We thank Noé Coussot, Géraldine Robic, Marie-France Mifune, Susanne Fürniss, and Rémy Jadinon, who provided the initial descriptions of the harps studied here. We also thank all the people who gave us the opportunity to describe their harps in the field (Gabon, Cameroon, Uganda) and in their private collections, as well as the various institutions that gave us access to their collections: the Africa Museum and the Musical Instrument Museum (Belgium), the Musée du Quai Branly-Jacques Chirac, the Musée de l’Homme, the Cité de la Musique, and the Musée d’Angoulême (France), the Ethnological Museum of Berlin, the Völkerkundesammlung, and the Overseas Museum (Germany), the Pitt Rivers Museum (England), the Swedish Museum of Performing Arts (Sweden), the Uganda National Museum (Uganda), and the National Museum of Arts, Rites and Traditions (Gabon). We thank Paul Aubier for helpful discussions and insightful feedback.

Author contributions

SS and SLB designed the study and collected the original data. SLB coordinated the collaboration, writing, and analysis. All authors conceived the data matrix. SS conducted the analysis. SS wrote the article with input from GL, PD, and SLB.

Funding statement

This research was supported by Agence Nationale de la Recherche (grant no. ANR-19-CE27-0013-01).

Competing interests

All authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Data availability statement

The data used in this study can be found in the supplementary material and in a public repository on GitLab: https://gitlab.com/cultural-evolution/african-harps-as-units-of-cultural-evolution/-/tree/main.

References

Agnarsson, I., & Miller, J. A. (2008). Is ACCTRAN better than DELTRAN? Cladistics, 24(6), 10321038. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.2008.00229.x.Google Scholar
Aguirre-Fernández, G., Barbieri, C., Graff, A., Pérez de Arce, J., Moreno, H., & Sánchez-Villagra, M. R. (2021). Cultural macroevolution of musical instruments in South America. Humanities and Social Sciences Communications, 8(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-021-00881-z.Google Scholar
Archie, J. W. (1989). Homoplasy excess ratios: New indices for measuring levels of homoplasy in phylogenetic systematics and a critique of the consistency index. Systematic Zoology, 38(3), 253269. https://doi.org/10.2307/2992286.Google Scholar
Archie, J. W. (1996). Measures of homoplasy. In Sanderson, M. J., and Hufford, L. (Eds.), Homoplasy: The recurrence of similarity in evolution (pp. 153188). Cambridge: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Arom, S. (1988). Systèmes Musicaux en Afrique Subsaharienne. Canadian University Music Review / Revue de musique des universités canadiennes, 9(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.7202/1014922ar.Google Scholar
Arom, S. (2019). Polyphonies et Polyrythmies instrumentales d’Afrique Centrale. In D. France (Ed.), Structure et méthodologie – Volume 1 (pp. 928). Sampzon: Delatour France.Google Scholar
Bahuchet, S. (2012). Changing language, remaining pygmy. Human Biology, 84(1), 1143. https://doi.org/10.1353/hub.2012.a470785.Google Scholar
Bailloud, G. (1997). Art rupestre en Ennedi. Sépia.Google Scholar
Barriel, V. (2011). Caractère. In Heams, T., Huneman, P., Lecointre, G., & Silberstein, M. (Eds.), Les Mondes darwiniens. L’évolution de l’évolution. Volume 1 (2nd ed., pp. 163196, Sciences & philosophie). Editions Matériologiques.Google Scholar
Barriel, V., & Tassy, P. (1998). Rooting with multiple outgroups: Consensus versus parsimony. Cladistics, 14(2), 193200. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.1998.tb00332.x.Google Scholar
Ben Hamed, M., Darlu, P., & Vallée, N. (2005). On cladistic reconstruction of linguistic trees through vowel data. Journal of Quantitative Linguistics, 12(1), 79109. https://doi.org/10.1080/09296170500055467.Google Scholar
Bentley, R. A., Moritz, W. R., Ruck, D. J., & O’Brien, M. J. (2021). Evolution of initiation rites during the Austronesian dispersal. Science Progress, 104(3), 00368504211031364. https://doi.org/10.1177/00368504211031364.Google Scholar
Blench, R. (2013). Methods and results in the reconstruction of music history in Africa and a case study of instrumental polyphony. Azania: Archaeological Research in Africa, 48(1), 3164. https://doi.org/10.1080/0067270X.2013.771016.Google Scholar
Bock, W. J. (1973). Philosophical foundations of classical evolutionary classification. Systematic Zoology, 22(4), 375392. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412945.Google Scholar
Borgerhoff Mulder, M., Nunn, C. L., & Towner, M. C. (2006). Cultural macroevolution and the transmission of traits. Evolutionary Anthropology, 15(2), 5264. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20088.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Bogerhoff-Mulder, M., Durham, W. H., & Richerson, P. J. (1997). Are cultural phyogenies possible? In Weignart, P., Mitchel, S. D., Richerson, P. J., & Maasen, S. (Eds.), Human by Nature: Between Biology and the Social Sciences (pp. 355386). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Brower, A. V. Z., & Schawaroch, V. (1996). Three steps of homology assessment. Cladistics, 12(3), 265272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.1996.tb00014.x.Google Scholar
Bruguière, P. (1999). Le chant de la harpe. In Bruguière, P., and Speranza, G. (Eds.), La Parole du fleuve, Harpes d’Afrique centrale (pp. 107114). Cité de la musique, Musée de la musique.Google Scholar
Bryant, D., Filimon, F., & Gray, R. D. (2005). Untangling our past: languages, trees, splits and networks. In: Mace, R., Holden, C. J., & Shennan, S. (eds.), The evolution of cultural diversity: a phylogenetic approach. (pp. 6784). London: University College London Press.Google Scholar
Buckley, C. D. (2012). Investigating cultural evolution using phylogenetic analysis: The origins and descent of the Southeast Asian tradition of warp Ikat weaving. PLoS One, 7(12), e52064. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0052064.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. (2005). Evolution biologique, Evolution culturelle. Odile Jacob.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1986). Cultural evolution. American Zoologist, 26(3), 845856. https://doi.org/10.1093/icb/26.3.845.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Charbonnat, P., Ben Hamed, M., & Lecointre, G. (2014). Apparenter la pensée? Vers une phylogénie des concepts savants. Editions Matériologiques.Google Scholar
Collard, M., Shennan, S. J., & Tehrani, J. J. (2006). Branching, blending, and the evolution of cultural similarities and differences among human populations. Evolution and Human Behavior, 27(3), 169184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.07.003.Google Scholar
Coquery-Vidrovitch, C. (2012). Frontières africaines et mondialisation. Histoire@Politique, 17(2), 149164. https://doi.org/10.3917/hp.017.0149.Google Scholar
Cracraft, J. (1981). Pattern and process in paleobiology: The role of cladistic analysis in systematic paleontology. Paleobiology, 7(4), 456468.Google Scholar
Creanza, N., Kolodny, O., & Feldman, M. W. (2017). Cultural evolutionary theory: How culture evolves and why it matters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(30), 77827789. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620732114.Google Scholar
Currie, T. E., Greenhill, S. J., & Mace, R. (2010). Is horizontal transmission really a problem for phylogenetic comparative methods? A simulation study using continuous cultural traits. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1559), 39033912. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0014.Google Scholar
Darlu, P., Tassy, P., d’Haese, C., & Zaragüeta I Bagils, R. (2019). La reconstruction phylogénétique. Concepts et méthodes (Nouvelle édition revue et augmentée). Editions Matériologiques.Google Scholar
de Dampierre, E. (1991). Harpes Zandé. Klincksiek.Google Scholar
de Pinna, M. C. (1991). Concepts and tests of homology in the cladistic paradigm. Cladistics, 7(4), 367394.Google Scholar
DeVale, S. C. (2014). Harp, III: Africa. In Libin, L. (Ed.), The Grove Dictionary of Musical Instruments (2nd ed., pp. 557561). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
d’Huy, J. (2012). Un ours dans les étoiles, recherche phylogénétique sur un mythe préhistorique. Préhistoire du Sud-ouest, 20(1), 91106.Google Scholar
d’Huy, J. (2013). A phylogenetic approach of mythology and its archaeological consequences. Rock Art Research, 30(1), 115118.Google Scholar
Emerit, S., Guichard, H., Jeammet, V., Perrot, S., Thomas, A., Vendries, C., Vincent, A., & Ziegler, N. (2017). Musiques! Échos de l’Antiquité. Musée du Louvre-Lens, Snoeck Publishers.Google Scholar
Farris, J. S. (1970). Methods for computing Wagner trees. Systematic Biology, 19(1), 8392. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412028.Google Scholar
Farris, J. S. (1989). The retention index and the rescaled consistency index. Cladistics, 5(4), 417419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.1989.tb00573.x.Google Scholar
Fernando, N. (1999). Les harpes du Nord-Cameroun. In Bruguière, P., and Speranza, G. (Eds.), La Parole du fleuve, Harpes d’Afrique centrale (pp. 145150). Cité de la musique, Musée de la musique.Google Scholar
Fisler, M., Crémière, C., Darlu, P., & Lecointre, G. (2020). The treeness of the tree of historical trees of life. PLoS One, 15(1), e0226567. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0226567.Google Scholar
Fitch, W. M. (1971). Distinguishing homologous from analogous proteins. Systematic Zoology, 20, 406416. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412448.Google Scholar
Fürniss, S. (1999). Les premiers voyageurs européens, collecteurs de harpes en Afrique centrale. In Bruguière, P., and Speranza, G. (Eds.), La Parole du fleuve, Harpes d’Afrique centrale (pp. 4955). Cité de la musique, Musée de la musique.Google Scholar
Fürniss, S. (2023). Innovation crushing cultural memory: Harps in South Cameroon. In Lawlor, H., and Joyce, S. (Eds.), Harp Studies II. World Harp Traditions (pp. 259282). Four Courts Press.Google Scholar
Gaillard-Corvaglia, A., Léonard, L. L., & Darlu, P. (2008). Approche cladistique des vocalismes d’Oïl à partir des données de l’ALF. Bollettino dell’Atlante Linguistico Italiano, III Serie, 32, 5596.Google Scholar
Gansemans, J. (1996). Creating a context for African musical instruments. Museum International, 48(1), 2125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0033.1996.tb01282.x.Google Scholar
Goloboff, P. A. (1991). Homoplasy and the choice among cladograms. Cladistics, 7(3), 215232. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.1991.tb00035.x.Google Scholar
Goloboff, P. A., Mattoni, C. I., & Quinteros, A. S. (2006). Continuous characters analyzed as such. Cladistics, 22(6), 589601. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.2006.00122.x.Google Scholar
Gray, R. D., & Jordan, F. M. (2000). Language trees support the express-train sequence of Austronesian expansion. Nature, 405, 10521055. https://doi.org/10.1038/35016575.Google Scholar
Greenhill, S. J., Currie, T. E., & Gray, R. D. (2009). Does horizontal transmission invalidate cultural phylogenies? Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1665), 22992306. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1944.Google Scholar
Guglielmino, C. R., Viganotti, C., Hewlett, B., & Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1995). Cultural variation in Africa: Role of mechanisms of transmission and adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 92(16), 75857589. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.92.16.7585.Google Scholar
Hennig, W. (1966). Phylogenetic Systematics. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Herndon, M. (1974). Analysis: The herding of sacred cows? Ethnomusicology, 18(2), 219262. https://doi.org/10.2307/850580.Google Scholar
Hornbostel (Von), E. M., & Sachs, C. (1961). Classification of musical instruments. Translated from the original German by Anthony Baines and Klaus P. Waschmann. Galpin Society Journal, 14, 329. https://doi.org/10.2307/842168.Google Scholar
Jadinon, R. (2014). Les traditions musicales de l’Ubangi. Omasombo, J. (Ed.), Sud-Ubangi. Bassins d’eau et espace agricole. (pp. 107125). MRAC, Le Cri, Samsa.Google Scholar
Johnson, H. M. (1995). An ethnomusicology of musical instruments: Form, function, and meaning. Journal of the Anthropological Society of Oxford, 26(3), 257269.Google Scholar
Kluge, A. G., & Farris, J. S. (1969). Quantitative phyletics and the evolution of anurans. Systematic Biology, 18(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412407.Google Scholar
Koile, E., Greenhill, S. J., Blasi, D. E., Bouckaert, R., & Gray, R. D. (2022). Phylogeographic analysis of the Bantu language expansion supports a rainforest route. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 119(32), e2112853119. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2112853119.Google Scholar
Kubik, G. (2010). Theory of African Music, Volume I. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lawergren, B. (1981). Acoustics and evolution of arched harps. The Galpin Society Journal, 34, 110129. https://doi.org/10.2307/841475.Google Scholar
Le Bomin, S., Lecointre, G., & Heyer, E. (2016). The evolution of musical diversity: The key role of vertical transmission. PLoS One, 11(3), e0151570. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0151570.Google Scholar
Leclair, M. (2007). La musique et ses instruments au musée du quai Branly. La Lettre de l’OCIM. Musées, Patrimoine Et Culture Scientifiques Et Techniques, 112, 3039. https://doi.org/10.4000/ocim.739.Google Scholar
Lukas, D., Towner, M., & Borgerhoff Mulder, M. (2021). The potential to infer the historical pattern of cultural macroevolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 376(1828), 20200057. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/tjvgy.Google Scholar
Maddison, W. P., Donoghue, M. J., & Maddison, D. R. (1984). Outgroup analysis and parsimony. Systematic Biology, 33(1), 83103. https://doi.org/10.2307/2413134.Google Scholar
Margush, T., & McMorris, F. (1981). Consensus-trees. Bulletin of Mathematical Biology, 43(2), 239244. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0092-8240(81)90019-7Google Scholar
Marwick, B. (2012). A cladistic evaluation of ancient Thai bronze Buddha images: Six tests for a phylogenetic signal in the Griswold collection. In: Tjoa-Bonatz, M., Reinecke, A., & Bonatz, D. (Eds.), Connecting Empires and States: Selected Papers from the 13th International Conference of the European Association of Southeast Asian Archaeologists, (Vol. 2, pp. 161178). NUS Press.Google Scholar
Meacham, C. A. (1984). The role of hypothesized direction of characters in the estimation of evolutionary history. Taxon, 33(1), 2638. https://doi.org/10.2307/1222026.Google Scholar
Menardi Noguera, A. (2023). The harpist from Bichagara and his drummer (Ennedi, Chad). Les Cahiers de l’AARS, 24, 173186.Google Scholar
Nunn, C. L., Arnold, C., Matthews, L., & Mulder, M. B. (2010). Simulating trait evolution for cross-cultural comparison. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1559), 38073819. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0009.Google Scholar
Padilla-Iglesias, C., Blanco-Portillo, J., Pricop, B., Ioannidis, A. G., Bickel, B., Manica, A., Vinicius, L., & Migliano, A. B. (2024). Deep history of cultural and linguistic evolution among Central African hunter-gatherers. Nature Human Behaviour, 8, 12631275. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-024-01891-y.Google Scholar
Patterson, C. (1982). Morphological characters and homology. In Joysey, K. A., and Friday, A. E. (Eds.), Problems of Phylogenetic Reconstruction, Systematics Association Special Volume no. 21 (pp. 2174). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Philippe, H., Lecointre, G., , H. L. V., & Le Guyader, H. (1996). A critical study of homoplasy in molecular data using morphologically based cladograms and its consequences for character weighting. Molecular Biology and Evolution, 13(9), 11741186. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordjournals.molbev.a025682.Google Scholar
Platnick, N. I. (1979). Philosophy and the transformation of cladistics. Systematic Zoology, 28(4), 537546. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412566.Google Scholar
Platnick, N. I., & Cameron, H. D. (1977). Cladistic methods in textual, linguistic, and phylogenetic analysis. Systematic Biology, 26(4), 380385. https://doi.org/10.2307/2412794.Google Scholar
Platnick, N. I., Griswold, C. E., & Coddington, J. A. (1991). On missing entries in cladistic analysis. Cladistics, 7(4), 337343. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1096-0031.1991.tb00042.x.Google Scholar
Pleijel, F. (1995). On character coding for phylogeny reconstruction. Cladistics, 11, 309315. https://doi.org/10.1016/0748-3007(95)90018-7.Google Scholar
Poe, S., & Wiens, J. J. (2000). Character selection and the methodology of morphological phylogenetics. In Wiens, J. J. (Ed.), Phylogenetic Analysis of Morphological Data (pp. 2036). Smithsonian Institution Press.Google Scholar
Praetorius, M. (1619). Syntagma Musicum II: De organographia; Theatrum instrumentum.Google Scholar
Rieppel, O. (2004). The language of systematics, and the philosophy of ‘total evidence’. Systematics and Biodiversity, 2(1), 919. https://doi.org/10.1017/s147720000400132x.Google Scholar
Rieppel, O. (2005). Le cohérentisme en systématique. In Deleporte, P. & Lecointre, G. (coord.), Philosophie de la systématique, Biosystema 24 (pp. 115126). Société française de systématique.Google Scholar
Rineau, V., Moncel, M. H., & Zeitoun, V. (2023). Revealing evolutionary patterns behind homogeneity: The case of the Palaeolithic assemblages from Notarchirico (Southern Italy). Journal of Archaeological Method and Theory, 30(1), 203238. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10816-022-09558-6.Google Scholar
Rivière, H. (1999). Notes sur l’instrumentarium musical des Ntumu du Cameroun. Journal Des Africanistes, 69(2), 121146. https://doi.org/10.3406/jafr.1999.1212.Google Scholar
Robinson, P. (2016). Four rules for the application of phylogenetics in the analysis of textual traditions. Digital Scholarship in the Humanities, 31(3), 637651. https://doi.org/10.1093/llc/fqv065.Google Scholar
Schmidt, T., Macé, C., & Weiler, J. F. (2003). Le classement des manuscrits par la statistique et la phylogénétique: Les cas de Grégoire de Nazianze et de Basile le Minime. Revue D’histoire Des Textes, 35, 241273. https://doi.org/10.3406/rht.2003.1513.Google Scholar
Shelemay, K. K. (2008). Notation and oral tradition. In Stone, R. M. (Ed.), The Garland Handbook of African Music (2nd ed., pp. 2444). Routledge.Google Scholar
Speranza, G. (1995). Sculpter au singulier. In de Dampierre, E. (Ed.), Une esthétique perdue. Harpes et harpistes du Haut-Oubangui (pp. 2576). Presses de l’Ecole normale supérieure. Société d’ethnologieGoogle Scholar
Speranza, G. (1999). Les harpes. In Bruguière, P., and Speranza, G. (Eds.), La Parole du fleuve, Harpes d’Afrique centrale (pp. 5994). Cité de la musique, Musée de la musique.Google Scholar
Steele, J., Jordan, P., & Cochrane, E. (2010). Evolutionary approaches to cultural and linguistic diversity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 365(1559), 37813785. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0202.Google Scholar
Stevens, P. F. (1991). Character states, morphological variation and phylogenetic analyses: A review. Systematic Botany, 16(3), 553583. https://doi.org/10.2307/2419343.Google Scholar
Strauch, S. (2023). Systématique, évolution culturelle et ethnomusicologie: Étude interdisciplinaire des harpes africaines (PhD thesis). Muséum National d’Histoire Naturelle. http://dx.doi.org/10.13140/RG.2.2.26670.01608Google Scholar
Strauch, S., & Le Bomin, S. (2024). Studying the diversity of Central African harps. In Lawlor, H., and Joyce, S. (Eds.), Harp Studies II. World Harp Traditions (pp. 236258). Four Court Press.Google Scholar
Swofford, D. L. (2003). PAUP*. Phylogenetic Analysis Using Parsimony (*and Other Methods). Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Swofford, D. L., & Maddison, W. P. (1992). Parsimony, character–state reconstructions, and evolutionary inferences. In Mayden, R. L. (Ed.), Systematics, Historical Ecology, and North American Freshwater Fishes (pp. 186223). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Tamisier, J. C. (1998). Dictionnaire des peuples: Sociétés d’Afrique, d’Amérique, d’Asie et d’Océanie. Larousse.Google Scholar
Tassy, P. (2022). Evolution and cladistics: The fate of Darwin’s ‘community of descent’. Bulletin de la Société D’histoire Naturelle de Toulouse, 158, 4357.Google Scholar
Tassy, P., & Fischer, M. S. (2021). ‘Cladus’ and clade: A taxonomic odyssey. Theory in Biosciences, 140(1), 7785. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12064-020-00326-2.Google Scholar
Tehrani, J. J. (2013). The phylogeny of Little Red Riding Hood. PLoS One, 8(11), e78871. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0078871.Google Scholar
Tëmkin, I. (2016). Homology and phylogenetic inference in biological and material cultural evolution. In Panebianco, F. & Serrelli, E. (Eds.), Understanding Cultural Traits: A Multidisciplinary Perspective on Cultural Diversity (pp. 287313). Springer.Google Scholar
Tëmkin, I., & Eldredge, N. (2007). Phylogenetics and material cultural evolution. Current Anthropology, 48(1), 146154. https://doi.org/10.1086/510463.Google Scholar
Tëmkin, I., & Eldredge, N. (2015). Networks and hierarchies: Approaching complexity in evolutionary theory. In Serrelli, E. & Gontier, N. (Eds.), Macroevolution: Explanation, Interpretation and Evidence. Interdisciplinary Evolution Research Volume 2 (pp. 183226). Springer.Google Scholar
Thiele, K. (1993). The holy grail of the perfect character: the cladistic treatment of morphometric data. Cladistics, 9, 275304. https://doi.org/10.1006/clad.1993.1020.Google Scholar
Vansina, J. (1980). La tradition orale et sa méthodologie. In Ki-Zerbo, J. (Ed.), Histoire générale de l’Afrique, I: Méthodologie et préhistoire africaine (pp. 167190). UNESCO. Jeune Afrique, Stock.Google Scholar
Wachsmann, K. (1964). Human migration and African harps. Journal of the International Folk Music Council, 16, 8488. https://doi.org/10.2307/835087.Google Scholar
Windram, H. F., Charlston, T., Tomita, Y., & Howe, C. J. (2022). A phylogenetic analysis of two preludes from J.S. Bach’s well-tempered clavier II. Early Music, 50(3), 373393. https://doi.org/10.1093/em/caac027.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Illustration of two harps and their constituent parts. (A) Orungu harp described in Gabon by Sylvie Le Bomin, in the field. (B) Ngbaka harp from Democratic Republic of Congo described by Noé Coussot at the AfricaMuseum (Belgium).

Figure 1

Figure 2. Geographic distribution of the 314 African harps in the matrix by country of attribution, showing the number of harps per country and the proportion described in museums, in the field, or in private collections.

Figure 2

Table 1. Number of characters associated with the different parts of the harp in both analyses

Figure 3

Figure 3. Cladogram of the majority-rule (50%) consensus tree (1581 steps), retained from 500,000 trees (1581 steps). Branch lengths are not informative. The cladogram is rooted with the four outgroup harps, circled in grey. The identifiers of the harps represented by at least four harps in the matrix are colour-coded according to their country of origin: Democratic Republic of the Congo (dark blue), Gabon (orange), Cameroon (light blue), Central African Republic (red), Uganda (green), Nigeria (dark yellow), and South Sudan (plum). The ingroup is marked by a red dot at its node. The six clades that are numbered in the figure (1–6) correspond to those that are examined in detail in this paper, with the first three colour-coded in blue, green, and orange, respectively, based on the main geographical attribution of the harps composing each clade. The numbers in the yellow circles correspond to the proportion of trees where the node is retained, while white boxes indicate the character state transformations at each node, with the state noted in superscript.

Figure 4

Table 2. Number of harps of the ingroup by country of attribution in each clade

Figure 5

Figure 4. Geographical distribution of harps in the main clades defined on the cladogram of the majority-rule (50%) consensus. For countries with harps attributed equally to two main clades, their assignment to these clades is represented by two arbitrarily distributed colours.

Supplementary material: File

Strauch et al. supplementary material

Strauch et al. supplementary material
Download Strauch et al. supplementary material(File)
File 1.2 MB