Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-2bdfx Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-13T09:46:29.637Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Early predictors of late childhood behavioural outcomes following very preterm birth

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 July 2025

Zeyuan Sun*
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
Andrew J Lawrence
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, London, UK
Laila Hadaya
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
Alexandria O’Reilly Mescall
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
Lu Zhang
Affiliation:
Department of Maternal and Child Health, West China School of Public Health and West China Fourth Hospital, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
Qiaoyue Ge
Affiliation:
Department of Maternal and Child Health, West China School of Public Health and West China Fourth Hospital, Sichuan University, Chengdu, China
Emily Simonoff
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK
Serena J Counsell
Affiliation:
Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
A David Edwards
Affiliation:
Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
Paola Dazzan
Affiliation:
Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology, and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Mental Health Biomedical Research Centre at South London and Maudsley NHS Foundation Trust and King’s College London, London, UK
Chiara Nosarti
Affiliation:
Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, London, UK Research Department of Early Life Imaging, School of Biomedical Engineering and Imaging Sciences, King’s College London, London, UK
*
Corresponding author: Zeyuan Sun; Email: zeyuan.sun@kcl.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Children born very preterm (VPT; ≤32 weeks’ gestation) are at higher risk of developing behavioural problems, encompassing socio-emotional processing and attention, compared to term-born children. This study aimed to examine multi-dimensional predictors of late childhood behavioural and psychiatric outcomes in very preterm children, using longitudinal clinical, environmental, and cognitive measures.

Methods

Participants were 153 VPT children previously enrolled in the Evaluation of Preterm Imaging study who underwent neuropsychological assessments at 18–24 months, 4–7 years and 8–11 years as part of the Brain Immunity and Psychopathology following very Preterm birth (BIPP) study. Predictors of late childhood behavioural and psychiatric outcomes were investigated, including clinical, environmental, cognitive, and behavioural measures in toddlerhood and early childhood. Parallel analysis and exploratory factor analysis were conducted to define outcome variables. A prediction model using elastic-net regularisation and repeated nested cross-validation was applied to evaluate the predictive strength of these variables.

Results

Factor analysis revealed two key outcome factors in late childhood: externalising and internalising-socio-emotional problems. The strongest predictors of externalising problems were response inhibition, effortful control and internalising symptoms in early childhood (cross-validated R2=.256). The strongest predictors of internalising problems were autism traits and poor cognitive flexibility in early childhood (cross-validated R2=.123). Cross-validation demonstrated robust prediction models, with higher accuracy for externalising symptoms.

Conclusions

Early childhood cognitive and behavioural outcomes predicted late childhood behavioural and psychiatric outcomes in very preterm children. These findings underscore the importance of early interventions targeting cognitive development and behavioural regulation to mitigate long-term psychiatric risks in very preterm children.

Information

Type
Original Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Individuals born very preterm (VPT; ≤32 weeks’ gestation) are at higher risk of developing behavioural problems, encompassing socio-emotional functioning and attention, compared to term-born children (Delobel-Ayoub et al., Reference Delobel-Ayoub, Kaminski, Marret, Burguet, Marchand, N’Guyen, Matis, Thiriez, Fresson, Arnaud, Poher, Larroque and Group2006; Johnson & Marlow, Reference Johnson and Marlow2011; Samuelsson et al., Reference Samuelsson, Holsti, Adamsson, Serenius, Hagglof and Farooqi2017). If these problems persist over time and are not addressed, they could lead to clinically significant psychiatric outcomes (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, O’Reilly, Ni, Wolke and Marlow2019). Recent work has shown that children born very preterm are more likely to receive a psychiatric diagnosis compared to their term-born peers: a ten-fold risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a five-fold risk for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), a two-fold risk for anxiety disorder (AD), as well as a 50% higher risk of receiving a diagnosis of mood disorder (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, de Miranda, Albuquerque, Indredavik, Evensen, Van Lieshout, Saigal, Taylor, Raikkonen, Kajantie, Marlow, Johnson, Woodward, Austin, Nosarti, Jaekel, Wolke, Cheong, Burnett and Doyle2021). Both clinical and subthreshold mental health problems have significant emotional and financial costs for families, and implications for public services, such as health insurance, education, and wider social support systems (Behrman & Butler, Reference Behrman and Butler2007). Therefore, early identification of behavioural problems in very preterm children and the provision of targeted interventions to support their development is vital for their well-being over the life course.

A major challenge for early identification of psychopathology is that often overt psychiatric symptoms do not manifest until childhood or even later, when individuals enter complex social environments, which may be overwhelming in the context of suboptimal socio-emotional development (Wolke, Johnson, & Mendonça, Reference Wolke, Johnson and Mendonça2019). These findings point to the need to find the most significant predictors of behavioural and psychiatric outcomes early in life, to prevent behavioural problems from escalating and becoming clinically significant.

Previous studies have attempted to identify predictors of behavioural and psychiatric outcomes associated with very preterm birth with varying degrees of success, including clinical dimensions (e.g., gestational age and birth weight), environmental factors (e.g., family income, maternal education) and cognitive variables (e.g., intelligence and executive function) (Delobel-Ayoub et al., Reference Delobel-Ayoub, Arnaud, White-Koning, Casper, Pierrat, Garel, Burguet, Roze, Matis, Picaud, Kaminski, Larroque and Grp2009; Treyvaud et al., Reference Treyvaud, Ure, Doyle, Lee, Rogers, Kidokoro, Inder and Anderson2013). However, these prediction models are either predominantly constrained by sample size or are not used in clinical practice (Linsell et al., Reference Linsell, Malouf, Morris, Kurinczuk and Marlow2017; Yaari et al., Reference Yaari, Treyvaud, Lee, Doyle and Anderson2019). Models lacking measurements at different developmental stages and adequate age adjustment may also be unreliable, given that child development is a complex and dynamic process wherein environmental factors may gradually supersede the impact of biological factors as children grow (Cicchetti, Reference Cicchetti2016; Maggi, Irwin, Siddiqi, & Hertzman, Reference Maggi, Irwin, Siddiqi and Hertzman2010).

This study aimed to explore the most significant longitudinal predictors of late childhood behavioural and psychiatric outcomes in very preterm children, thereby providing insights for early identification and intervention. We hypothesised that environmental (e.g., maternal anxiety), cognitive (e.g., executive function, intelligence) and behavioural outcomes (e.g., temperament, internalising and externalising symptoms, autism traits) in toddlers (18–24 months) and young children (4–7 years) would predict later behavioural and psychiatric outcomes during late childhood (8–11 years).

Methods

Participants were very preterm children and their parents who took part in the Brain, Immunity and Psychopathology following very Preterm birth (BIPP) study. The BIPP study invited consenting participants who previously took part in the Evaluation of Preterm Imaging study (ePrime; EudraCT 2009-011602-42) (n=511) (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Redshaw, Kennea, Rivero-Arias, Gonzales-Cinca, Nongena, Ederies, Falconer, Chew, Omar, Hardy, Harvey, Eddama, Hayward, Wurie, Azzopardi, Rutherford, Counsell and ePrime2018) to complete a follow-up assessment between the ages of 8 and 11 years. 485 participants also completed neurodevelopmental assessments at 18–24 months (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Redshaw, Kennea, Rivero-Arias, Gonzales-Cinca, Nongena, Ederies, Falconer, Chew, Omar, Hardy, Harvey, Eddama, Hayward, Wurie, Azzopardi, Rutherford, Counsell and ePrime2018) and 251 participants at 4–7 years (Kanel et al., Reference Kanel, Vanes, Pecheva, Hadaya, Falconer, Counsell, Edwards and Nosarti2021; Vanes et al., Reference Vanes, Hadaya, Kanel, Falconer, Ball, Batalle, Counsell, Edwards and Nosarti2021). Infants recruited into ePrime had the following inclusion criteria: birth before 33 weeks of gestation, maternal age above 16 years, and mothers not being hospital inpatients. Exclusion criteria were major congenital malformations, contraindications to magnetic resonance imaging, parents not being able to speak English, or being subject to child protection proceedings. Here we report data from 153 BIPP participants who completed the three assessments by July 2023 (Figure 1).

Figure 1. Recruitment flow chart.

This study was conducted in line with the ethical standards of the 1964 Helsinki Declaration. The ePrime study was approved by the Hammersmith and Queen Charlotte’s Research Ethics Committee (09/H0707/98); BIPP by the South East Research Ethics Committee (19/LO/1940) and the Stanmore Research Ethics Committee (18/LO/0048).

Behavioural outcome measures

Behavioural outcome measures were collected at three timepoints.

18–24 months assessment (T1)

Toddlers’ cognitive, motor and language development was indexed by five subscale scores of the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development–Third Edition (BSID-III) (Bayley, Reference Bayley2006) and two subscale scores of the Parent Report of Children’s Abilities-Revised (PARCA-R) (Johnson et al., Reference Johnson, Marlow, Wolke, Davidson, Marston, O’Hare, Peacock and Schulte2004). Autism traits were evaluated with the Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (M-CHAT) (Robins, Fein, & Barton, Reference Robins, Fein and Barton1999), using the total number of failed items. Maternal anxiety was indexed by the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) trait score (Spielberger, Reference Spielberger1970).

4–7 years assessment (T2)

Children’s temperament was indexed by three subscale scores of the Children’s Behaviour Questionnaire – Very Short Form (CBQ-VSF) (Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006). Children’s mental health was indexed by the internalising and externalising problem scores of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaires (SDQ) (Goodman, Reference Goodman1997). Internalising difficulties are derived from the emotional and peer relationship problems subscales, while externalising difficulties are derived from the conduct and hyperactivity/inattention problems subscales (Goodman, Lamping, & Ploubidis, Reference Goodman, Lamping and Ploubidis2010). Autism traits were measured with the Social Responsiveness Scale Second Edition (SRS-2) (Constantino et al., Reference Constantino, Davis, Todd, Schindler, Gross, Brophy, Metzger, Shoushtari, Splinter and Reich2003).

Cognitive variables were Intelligence Quotient (IQ), indexed by 12 item scores of the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-fourth edition (WPPSI-IV) (Wechsler, Reference Wechsler2012), everyday executive functions, indexed by three subscale scores of the Behaviour Rating Inventory of Executive Function, Preschool Version (BRIEF-P) (Gioia, Isquith, Guy, & Kenworthy, Reference Gioia, Isquith, Guy and Kenworthy2000), emotion regulation, indexed by the Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC) (Shields & Cicchetti, Reference Shields and Cicchetti1997), attentional control, indexed by the Attention Network Task (ANT) (Rueda et al., Reference Rueda, Fan, McCandliss, Halparin, Gruber, Lercari and Posner2004), cognitive flexibility, indexed by the Dimensional Change Card Sorting (DCCS) (Zelazo, Reference Zelazo2006), working memory, indexed by Digit Span (using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [WISC] age-adapted normative scores), sustained attention, indexed by the Track-it! task (Fisher et al., Reference Fisher, Thiessen, Godwin, Kloos and Dickerson2013) and empathy, indexed by Empathy Questionnaire (Rieffe, Ketelaar, & Wiefferink, Reference Rieffe, Ketelaar and Wiefferink2010). The cognitively stimulating home environment was indexed by our adapted version of the Cognitively Stimulating Parenting Scale (CSPS) (Vanes et al., Reference Vanes, Hadaya, Kanel, Falconer, Ball, Batalle, Counsell, Edwards and Nosarti2021).

8–11 years assessment (T3)

Child’s temperament was indexed by three subscale scores of the Temperament in Middle Childhood Questionnaire, (TMCQ) (Simonds & Rothbart, Reference Simonds and Rothbart2004). Children’s mental health was indexed by SDQ internalising and externalising scores. Autism-related symptoms were indexed by the SRS-2. Anxiety symptoms were indexed by the Spence Children’s Anxiety Scale (SCAS) (Spence, Reference Spence1998).

Socio-demographic and clinical measures

The following socio-demographic and clinical measures were collected. Sex, ethnicity (defined by self-report based on classifications by the UK Office for National Statistics) (ONS, 2019), age at assessment, maternal education (categorized as “low” if she left full-time education before the age of 19, and “high” if she left full-time education after the age of 19) (Kleine et al., Reference Kleine, Falconer, Roth, Counsell, Redshaw, Kennea, Edwards and Nosarti2020) and relative social deprivation, indexed by the 2019 Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD) (Ministry of Housing, 2019), calculated based on maternal postcode at recruitment. Gestational age (in weeks) and birth weight (in grams) were also collected.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analysis was performed using R-4.3.1 (R Project for Statistical Computing). Descriptive results for categorical variables were presented as n (%) and continuous variables as means (standard deviations). Parallel analysis (R package psych) (Horn, Reference Horn1965) and exploratory factors analysis (R package lavaan) (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012) were conducted to reduce the dimensionality of the outcomes. The code used can be accessed here: (https://github.com/RobinZSun/dCVnet_BIPP). Further details on variable characteristics can also be found in Supplementary Table 1. The study variables exhibited missingness ranging from 0% to 23.53% across predictors, while the missingness for outcome variables ranged from 6.54% to 45.75% (complete distributions provided in Supplementary Table 1). Formal testing using Jamshidian and Jalal’s (Reference Jamshidian and Jalal2010) multivariate Missing Completely At Random procedure rejected the missing completely at random assumption (median χ 2=25.29, p=0.01), supporting our missing-at-random working assumption. All missing data were jointly imputed using missForest’s random forest approach (Stekhoven & Buhlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Buhlmann2012). To confirm the representativeness of the study sample, demographic and clinical factors were compared between participants included in the current analysis (N = 153) and all who completed the T2 assessments (N = 251) (See Supplementary Table 2). There were no significant differences in terms of sex distribution (χ = 0.01, p = 0.93), IMD (t = −0.71, p = 0.48) and GA (t = −0.89, p = 0.37) between the two samples.

Prediction modelling

The seven separate childhood outcome variables (three TMCQ subscale scores, SCAS total score, two SDQ broad categories and SRS-2 total score) were included in an exploratory factors analysis. The derived factors were used as outcomes for the prediction models, and separate prediction models were built for each outcome factor. All cognitive and behavioural measures collected at T1 and T2 were included as predictors. We excluded any predictors with pre-imputation missing values greater than 20% (two variables). To ensure our models prediction performance did not reflect trivial effects of assessment timing and demographic factors, both predictors and outcome factors were adjusted for age at assessment, while predictors were further adjusted for sex, ethnicity, mother’s education, IMD and gestational age. To retain the most significant longitudinal predictors of outcomes, we used an approach that accommodates a wide range of predictors and their combinations, while eliminating redundant predictors from the model. This approach is called double cross-validation for the elastic-net (dCVnet); R package dCVnet (https://www.github.com/AndrewLawrence/dCVnet) developed by coauthor AL (Lawrence et al., Reference Lawrence, Stahl, Duan, Fennema, Jaeckle, Young, Dazzan, Moll and Zahn2022). The dCVnet software fits regression models with elastic net-regularization and provides internal validity of the prediction performance using double (also known as “nested”) cross-validation (repeated k-fold type). This combines an inner cross-validation, which tunes elastic net hyperparameters, with an outer cross-validation of model performance measures. This ensures the validity of the cross-validation measures (Cawley & Talbot, Reference Cawley and Talbot2010), while the regularisation performs data-driven variable selection, reduces overfitting, and allows models with correlated predictors (Zou & Hastie, Reference Zou and Hastie2005). The model utilises simple linear functions of predictors and is more directly interpretable than complex machine-learning techniques (Christodoulou et al., Reference Christodoulou, Ma, Collins, Steyerberg, Verbakel and Van Calster2019). For each outcome factor, the prediction models used a gaussian family elastic-net with alpha = 0.2 and a data-derived sequence of 100 lambdas on a log scale. Internal cross-validation was 30×10-fold, while external cross-validation was 100×10-fold. The cross-validated prediction performance was evaluated by three indicators: the Root-Mean-Square Error (RMSE), the Mean-Absolute Error (MAE) and R square (r 2).

Results

Sample characteristics

Sample characteristics are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Clinical, socio-demographic and parental characteristics of the sample (n = 153)

a “Low” indicates exiting full-time education by 19 years, “High” indicates exiting full-time education later than 19 years or still in full-time education.

Factor analysis of behavioural outcomes in late childhood

Parallel analysis and exploratory factor analysis on outcome variable identified two factors, jointly explaining a cumulative 54.9% of the total variance. Individual loadings of outcome variables for these two factors are depicted in Figure 2.

Figure 2. Heatmap of loadings of each variable on Factor 1 (externalising problems) and Factor 2 (internalising-socio-emotional problems). Positive loadings (>0) are indicated by red and negative loadings (<0) by blue.

Factor 1 was driven by positive loadings for measures capturing externalising symptoms (SDQ externalising symptoms), Surgency and Negative Affectivity (TMCQ), as well as negative loadings for measures capturing impulse control (TMCQ Effortful control). We labelled this factor as “Externalising”.

Factor 2 was driven by positive loadings for measures capturing internalising symptoms (SDQ internalising symptoms), autism traits (SRS-2), anxiety symptoms (SCAS) and Negative Affectivity (TMCQ), as well as negative loadings for Surgency (TMCQ). We labelled this factor as “Internalising-socio-emotional”.

Prediction of behavioural and mental health outcomes in late childhood

Cross-validation revealed robust, better than chance, prediction of both “externalising” and “internalising-social-emotional” outcomes (Table 2). Model coefficients (Table 3) suggest that increased externalising problems in late childhood were associated with increased maternal anxiety in toddlerhood (maternal STAI), poorer verbal concept formation and reasoning (WPPSI similarities), working memory (WPPSI picture memory and digit span), emotion regulation, increased autistic traits (SRS-2) and more behavioural problems in early childhood (CBQ surgency and SDQ internalising problems), with the most important predictors being poorer inhibitory self-control, emergent metacognition, more externalising problems and reduced effortful control. They also show that internalising-socio-emotional problems in late childhood were associated with more autistic traits in toddlerhood (M-Chat), poorer general cognitive ability (WPPSI information) and more internalising problems, with the most important predictors being poorer cognitive flexibility, increased autistic traits (SRS-2) and lower surgency (Table 3).

Table 2. Model performance measures of cross-validations (k = 10, lambda = 100)

Note: RMSE, Root-Mean-Square Error; MAE, Mean-Absolute Error; r 2, R square.

Table 3. Prospective predictors of behavioural and mental health outcomes in late childhood

Note: hyphen (“-”) indicates variables assigned a coefficient of exactly zero and so not selected in the model. The largest magnitude coefficients (greater than |0.06|) are highlighted in bold. In addition, the following predictors were included, but not selected by either model: BSID-III cognitive subscale, BSID-III expressive language subscale, BSID-III receptive language subscale, BSID-III fine motor subscale, BSID-III gross motor subscale, PARCA_R cognition subscale, PARCA_R language subscale, WPPSI block design, WPPSI matrix reasoning, WPPSI bug search, WPPSI picture concepts, WPPSI cancellation, WPPSI zoo location, WPPSI object assembly, WPPSI receptive vocabulary, WPPSI picture naming, CBQ negative affectivity, Track it! memory, Track it! attention, Card sort total, ANT correct percentage, Emque total, CSPS total. For variable characteristics see Supplementary Table 1.

Discussion

In this study, we explored the most important longitudinal predictors of late childhood behavioural and psychiatric outcomes in very preterm children. We identified two behavioural and psychiatric phenotypes in late childhood, the first characterised by externalising problems and the second by internalising-social-emotional problems. Analyses investigated previously collected variables (cognitive, behavioural, parental measures in toddlerhood and early childhood) as predictors of behavioural and psychiatric outcomes in later childhood. Our findings indicated that greater maternal anxiety, poorer executive function, verbal concept formation and reasoning, working memory, emotion regulation, effortful control, more autistic traits and behavioural problems during early childhood were associated with externalising problems in later childhood. They also showed that more autistic traits in toddlerhood and early childhood, poorer general cognitive ability and flexibility, lower surgency and more internalising problems, were associated with internalising-social-emotional problems in later childhood.

Our findings are in line with previous studies investigating associations between mental health outcomes and cognitive, behavioural and parental measures separately (Bayer et al., Reference Bayer, Ukoumunne, Lucas, Wake, Scalzo and Nicholson2011; Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson, & Lichtenstein, Reference Pettersson, Lahey, Larsson and Lichtenstein2018). Behavioural and mental health outcomes were summarised into “externalising” and “internalising-social-emotional” problems, which incorporated multi-dimensional mental health features consistent with common psychiatric outcomes during late childhood (Johnson & Marlow, Reference Johnson and Marlow2011; Treyvaud et al., Reference Treyvaud, Ure, Doyle, Lee, Rogers, Kidokoro, Inder and Anderson2013). The “externalising” factor used in our analyses comprised externalising symptoms (conduct problems and hyperactivity), surgency, referring to low shyness, high-intensity pleasure and impulsiveness (Oldehinkel et al., Reference Oldehinkel, Hartman, De Winter, Veenstra and Ormel2004), negative affectivity, referring to the tendency to attribute negative feelings to contexts, and overexpression of negative affect (Putnam et al., Reference Putnam, Ellis, Rothbart, Eliasz and Angleitner2001) and lack of effortful control, referring to individuals’ ability to direct their attention and regulate their emotions and behaviours (Damon, Lerner, & Eisenberg, Reference Damon, Lerner and Eisenberg2006). In contrast, the “internalising-social-emotional” factor comprised autistic traits, internalising symptoms (emotional and peer problems), negative affectivity, anxiety symptoms and low surgency. Notably, negative affectivity loaded on both factors, suggesting that susceptibility to negative emotions may be a common underlying feature (Engle & McElwain, Reference Engle and McElwain2011). On the other hand, surgency loaded on both factors yet in opposite directions, which is in line with previous findings (Secinti & Gürbüz, Reference Secinti and Gürbüz2021). The performance of the prediction model for externalising problems was slightly better than that for internalising-social-emotional problems. This could be because externalising symptoms tend to have a higher intensity than the internalising ones in late childhood (Papachristou & Flouri, Reference Papachristou and Flouri2020), and were therefore easier to observe and measure in this study.

Our findings showed that the most important predictor of late childhood internalising-socio-emotional problems among very preterm children was a higher level of autism traits in early childhood, which is consistent with prior research (Mandy et al., Reference Mandy, Midouhas, Hosozawa, Cable, Sacker and Flouri2022). The association between early autism traits and later internalising problems has been the focus of previous studies. From a biological perspective, shared genetic influences between autistic-like traits and emotional and behavioural problems have been reported (Hollo, Wehby, & Oliver, Reference Hollo, Wehby and Oliver2014; Ronald, Edelson, Asherson, & Saudino, Reference Ronald, Edelson, Asherson and Saudino2010), indicating potential shared biological pathways. However, environmental factors have also been put forward to understand the association. For example, social opportunities provided by parents may influence children’s social and communication skills. Children with poorer social communication skills demonstrated more autistic symptoms compared to those with better social communication abilities (Craig et al., Reference Craig, Crippa, Ruggiero, Rizzato, Russo, Fanizza and Trabacca2021), which could lead to a higher level of emotional problems (Salomone et al., Reference Salomone, Settanni, Ferrara, Salandin and Team2019). Another study also suggested that earlier communication and social interaction difficulties may mediate the associations between autism traits and subsequent internalising problems (Hallett, Ronald, Rijsdijk, & Happe, Reference Hallett, Ronald, Rijsdijk and Happe2010; Stice & Lavner, Reference Stice and Lavner2019). Communicative impairments can confuse a child’s understanding of others, limit self-expression, and reduce coping strategies for negative emotions, such as anxiety and sadness (Donoso et al., Reference Donoso, Rattray, de Bildt, Tillmann, Williams, Absoud and Totsika2023).

We found associations between low effortful control and later externalising problems; and low surgency and later internalising-socio-emotional problems. These findings indicate consistency in temperamental traits from early to late childhood, as we used the same scales for both predictor (CBQ) and outcome (TMCQ) at different ages (Putnam, Gartstein, & Rothbart, Reference Putnam, Gartstein and Rothbart2006). Research findings suggest that children’s temperaments are relatively stable across developmental stages (Komsi et al., Reference Komsi, Raikkonen, Pesonen, Heinonen, Keskivaara, Jarvenpaa and Strandberg2006), mirroring our findings – where the effect of three temperamental types in early childhood (captured by the CBQ) aligns with those observed in later childhood (captured by the TMCQ). Our findings support previous studies linking temperament and behavioural problems. For instance, effortful control plays an important role in shaping psychological and social adjustment (Meehan, De Panfilis, Cain, & Clarkin, Reference Meehan, De Panfilis, Cain and Clarkin2013). High effortful control has been linked to a reduced risk of externalising problems (Oldehinkel et al., Reference Oldehinkel, Hartman, Ferdinand, Verhulst and Ormel2007) and low effortful control to increased behavioural problems (Atherton, Zheng, Bleidorn, & Robins, Reference Atherton, Zheng, Bleidorn and Robins2019). Low surgency has been associated with a greater likelihood of exhibiting behavioural wariness, with emotion regulation playing a moderating role (Dollar & Stifter, Reference Dollar and Stifter2012). It has also been suggested that there may be transactional effects between maternal anxiety and child effortful control (Behrendt et al., Reference Behrendt, Wade, Bayet, Nelson and Bosquet Enlow2020): maternal anxiety could lead to low control in children and vice versa, and this interaction further contributes to subsequent behavioural problems in offspring.

In terms of executive function, we found that poorer inhibitory self-control in early childhood was associated with more externalising problems in later childhood, while poorer cognitive flexibility in early childhood was linked to internalising social-emotional problems in later childhood. Genetic studies have shown that the relationship between response inhibition and externalising problems could be explained by both genetic and shared environmental influences (Rhee et al., Reference Rhee, Friedman, Smith Watts, Corley, Hewitt, Robinson and Zahn-Waxler2018). Another study suggested that the impact of poor response inhibition on behavioural problems was related to its association with disruptive social competence in primary school children (Wang & Liu, Reference Wang and Liu2021). At a biological level, research indicates that enhanced parasympathetic activity may serve as a common pathway underlying both poor self-control and externalising problems in children (Kahle, Utendale, Widaman, & Hastings, Reference Kahle, Utendale, Widaman and Hastings2018). On the other hand, children showing good cognitive flexibility skills may be less likely to develop internalising behaviour problems (Patwardhan, Nelson, McClelland, & Mason, Reference Patwardhan, Nelson, McClelland and Mason2021). Cognitive flexibility may enable children to shift between activities, allowing them to disengage from negative stimuli and aversive situations, and instead focus selectively on more positive thoughts (Garon, Bryson, & Smith, Reference Garon, Bryson and Smith2008). More broadly, associations between executive functions and behavioural problems are frequently reported; however, the directionality of these associations remains uncertain (Donati, Meaburn, & Dumontheil, Reference Donati, Meaburn and Dumontheil2021; Yang et al., Reference Yang, Shields, Zhang, Wu, Chen and Romer2022). Inconsistent findings may be due to variability in the measurements used to assess executive functions and behavioural problems, participant characteristics (e.g., age groups, clinical vs. general populations), and the specific behavioural problems studied (e.g., ADHD, ASD, externalising behaviour) (Yang et al., Reference Yang, Shields, Zhang, Wu, Chen and Romer2022). A recent study indicated that executive function is both a risk marker and a consequence of youth transdiagnostic psychopathology (Romer & Pizzagalli, Reference Romer and Pizzagalli2021). There is evidence that general psychopathology and executive dysfunction may share neural circuits, specifically within the frontoparietal network, visual association cortex, and cerebello-thalamo-cerebro-cortical pathways (Alnaes et al., Reference Alnaes, Kaufmann, Doan, Cordova-Palomera, Wang, Bettella, Moberget, Andreassen and Westlye2018; Karcher, Michelini, Kotov, & Barch, Reference Karcher, Michelini, Kotov and Barch2021; Moberget et al., Reference Moberget, Alnaes, Kaufmann, Doan, Cordova-Palomera, Norbom, Rokicki, van der Meer, Andreassen and Westlye2019).

Noticeably, both internalising and externalising symptoms in early childhood were associated with externalising problems in later childhood, while only internalising symptoms in early childhood were associated with internalising-socio-emotional problems in later childhood. This may be due, at least in part, to the way our outcomes of interest were defined based on the results of the factor analysis. The externalising factor captures conduct problems, hyperactivity, low impulse control but also negative affectivity, which has been associated with both internalising and externalising disorders in children (Mikolajewski et al., Reference Mikolajewski, Allan, Hart, Lonigan and Taylor2013). On the other hand, the internalising-socio-emotional problems factor consists of a broad set of difficulties in social engagement and emotional regulations (e.g., autistic traits, emotional and peer problems, anxiety symptoms and susceptibility to negative emotions), hence it is plausible that early conduct problems and hyperactivity (i.e., SDQ externalising symptoms) may not contribute to the development of internalising-socio-emotional problems in later childhood. Our findings can also be explained by the stability of internalising and externalising symptoms, as externalising symptoms in childhood have been reported to be more transient and worsen over time, whereas internalising symptoms tend to show more consistency (Blok et al., Reference Blok, de Mol, van der Ende, Hillegers, Althoff, Shaw and White2022). Furthermore, internalising symptoms have been found to predict later internalising outcomes, whereas externalising symptoms have not been shown to predict externalising outcomes (Sallis et al., Reference Sallis, Szekely, Neumann, Jolicoeur-Martineau, van, Hillegers, Greenwood, Meaney, Steiner, Tiemeier, Wazana, Pearson and Evans2019).

We found that poorer verbal concept formation, reasoning, working memory and emotion regulation in early childhood were associated with more externalising problems in later childhood, while poorer general cognitive ability was prospectively associated with internalising-socio-emotional problems. Despite limited evidence, it has been suggested that reduced performance in reading, writing, and spelling may be associated with emotional and behavioural problems in children (Khanam & Nghiem, Reference Khanam and Nghiem2018). Among the various mediating factors, parental education and school performance appear to play a significant role in this association (Tamayo Martinez et al., Reference Tamayo Martinez, Tiemeier, Luijk, Law, van der Ende, Verhulst and Jansen2021). The relationship between externalising problems and cognitive abilities, such as working memory (Augusti, Torheim, & Melinder, Reference Augusti, Torheim and Melinder2014; Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long, & Weigard, Reference Huang-Pollock, Shapiro, Galloway-Long and Weigard2017) and emotional regulation (Augusti, Torheim, & Melinder, Reference Augusti, Torheim and Melinder2014), has been widely documented. Working memory deficits may contribute to poor academic outcomes and result in children’s exposure to negative emotions arising from teachers’ feedback (Lahey et al., Reference Lahey, Rathouz, Keenan, Stepp, Loeber and Hipwell2015), which in turn can lead to externalising problems (Tajik-Parvinchi et al., Reference Tajik-Parvinchi, Farmus, Tablon Modica, Cribbie and Weiss2021).

We found that increased maternal anxiety in toddlerhood was associated with increased externalising problems in offspring, in line with previous findings (Spence et al., Reference Spence, Najman, Bor, O’Callaghan and Williams2002). The precise mechanisms underlying the association between maternal anxiety and children’s outcomes are not well understood. One of the most accepted theories suggests that this influence occurs via the maternal hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, leading to higher cortisol levels, higher placental corticotropin-releasing hormone levels and reduced blood serum levels of placental growth factor (Hobel et al., Reference Hobel, Dunkel-Schetter, Roesch, Castro and Arora1999; Torry et al., Reference Torry, Wang, Wang, Caudle and Torry1998). These stress-related hormones could interfere with the development of the fetal brain and affect offspring socioemotional and behavioural functioning across the lifespan (Hostinar & Gunnar, Reference Hostinar and Gunnar2013). In addition, though most prior studies cannot rule out genetic confounding as an explanation, some evidence suggests that maternal prenatal stress may lead to children’s behavioural problems by influencing the family environment in the postnatal period (Rice et al., Reference Rice, Harold, Boivin, van den Bree, Hay and Thapar2010). We did not find an association between maternal anxiety during toddlerhood and internalising or socio-emotional problems in offspring, which contradicts much of the existing literature (Henrichs et al., Reference Henrichs, van den Heuvel, Witteveen, Wilschut and Van den Bergh2019; Shih et al., Reference Shih, Ahmad, Bush, Roubinov, Tylavsky, Graff, Karr, Sathyanarayana and LeWinn2023). This discrepancy may be partially due to the type of behavioural problems at this stage, manifesting more as externalising rather than internalising symptoms. Additionally, the timing of the assessment and the choice of measure (STAI traits) may confound the interpretation of the effects observed. Finally, the broad nature of the internalizing-socio-emotional problems factor in our study, encompassing a wide range of difficulties beyond internalising symptoms, may explain why maternal anxiety may not directly influence their development in late childhood.

Previous studies building prediction models for psychiatric outcomes during childhood among preterm children usually included only a limited number of predictors and outcomes (Anderson et al., Reference Anderson, de Miranda, Albuquerque, Indredavik, Evensen, Van Lieshout, Saigal, Taylor, Raikkonen, Kajantie, Marlow, Johnson, Woodward, Austin, Nosarti, Jaekel, Wolke, Cheong, Burnett and Doyle2021; Yaari et al., Reference Yaari, Treyvaud, Lee, Doyle and Anderson2019). We applied a mixed approach of double-cross validation and elastic net regularisation to allow a greater number of correlated predictors without overfitting and without losing model interpretability. By incorporating the indicators identified from infancy through early childhood into routine paediatric developmental screenings, clinicians could enhance risk stratification. An updated framework that emphasises transdiagnostic risk factors, rather than symptom-based screening, may enable earlier detection through fewer, yet more precise, indicators. In high-risk populations (e.g., very preterm children), these predictors could also serve as potential indicators for evaluating early interventions.

Several limitations should be considered when interpreting the findings of our study. Firstly, the outcomes of the prediction model were based on the results of a factor analysis. Therefore, the interpretation of the model relies more on the clinical significance of the two outcome factors than on the standardised scales that comprised them. Secondly, our analysis was based on a comparatively small sample size with a rather large set of variables, which limits model performance and the generalisation of our findings. Thirdly, some variables were more affected by missing data than others (see Supplementary Table 1). Although the imputation method we used (MissForest) outperforms other methods for the imputation of missing values in mixed-type data (Stekhoven & Buhlmann, Reference Stekhoven and Buhlmann2012), those predictors with fewer missing values may nevertheless be favoured by prediction models (Moons, Donders, Stijnen, & Harrell, Reference Moons, Donders, Stijnen and Harrell2006). A further limitation is the lack of granular information about family-level contextual factors (e.g., caregiver relationships, parenting practices, or household dynamics) (Fosco & Lydon-Staley, Reference Fosco and Lydon-Staley2020; Hosokawa & Katsura, Reference Hosokawa and Katsura2024), which were not fully captured by our measures. Finally, our study was based on longitudinal cognitive and behavioural measurements and did not include data on children’s brain development, which prevented the study from probing the biological basis of the outcome measures of interest. We consider these two models as baseline frameworks, which we hope future analyses incorporating neuroimaging data will further refine.

Conclusion

We identified two phenotypes of late-childhood behavioural problems, derived from several common mental health outcomes in children born very preterm, and developed two separate prediction models accordingly. Our findings suggest that poorer inhibitory self-control and cognition, along with heightened externalising symptoms in early childhood, predict externalising problems in late childhood. Additionally, poor cognitive flexibility, increased internalising symptoms and autism traits during early childhood predict internalising social-emotional problems in late childhood. Future research should evaluate their clinical utility in guiding interventions designed to mitigate the long-term sequelae of very preterm birth, thereby improving developmental trajectories and overall well-being in very preterm children.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291725001151.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to sincerely thank all the participants and their families. Our gratitude also extends to the professionals, advisors, and colleagues who provided invaluable support and guidance. This work would not have been possible without their collective contributions.

Author contribution

ZS, LH and AOM contributed to data curation and investigation. ZS, AL, LZ and QG contributed to the formal analysis. AL and CN contributed to the study design, methodology and supervision. ZS, AL and CN contributed to first draft writing. CN, ES, SJC, ADE and PD contributed to funding acquisition, project administration, resources and validation. All authors participated in reviewing and editing and shared responsibility for the final version of the work submitted and published.

Funding statement

Source of funding: This work was supported by the Medical Research Council (UK) (grant nos MR/S026460/1 and MR/K006355/1) and by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Programme Grant for Applied Research Programme (RP-PG-0707–10154). The study was also supported by the NIHR Biomedical Research Centre (BRC) at South London & Maudsley National Health Service (NHS) Foundation Trust.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

Ethical standard

The ePrime study was approved by the Hammersmith and Queen Charlotte’s Research Ethics Committee (REC: 09/H0707/98), BIPP by the South East Research Ethics Committee (REC: 19/LO/1940), and the Stanmore Research Ethics Committee (REC: 18/LO/0048). Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participant’s legal guardian/next of kin.

References

Alnaes, D., Kaufmann, T., Doan, N. T., Cordova-Palomera, A., Wang, Y., Bettella, F., Moberget, T., Andreassen, O. A., & Westlye, L. T. (2018). Association of heritable cognitive ability and psychopathology with white matter properties in children and adolescents. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(3), 287295. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.4277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, P. J., de Miranda, D. M., Albuquerque, M. R., Indredavik, M. S., Evensen, K. A. I., Van Lieshout, R., Saigal, S., Taylor, H. G., Raikkonen, K., Kajantie, E., Marlow, N., Johnson, S., Woodward, L. J., Austin, N., Nosarti, C., Jaekel, J., Wolke, D., Cheong, J. L., Burnett, A.,…Doyle, L. W. (2021). Psychiatric disorders in individuals born very preterm / very low-birth weight: An individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis. EClinicalMedicine, 42, 101216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2021.101216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Atherton, O. E., Zheng, L. R., Bleidorn, W., & Robins, R. W. (2019). The codevelopment of effortful control and school behavioral problems. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 117(3), 659673. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspp0000201CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augusti, E. M., Torheim, H. K., & Melinder, A. (2014). The effect of emotional facial expressions on children’s working memory: Associations with age and behavior. Child Neuropsychology, 20(1), 86105. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2012.749225CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayer, J. K., Ukoumunne, O. C., Lucas, N., Wake, M., Scalzo, K., & Nicholson, J. M. (2011). Risk factors for childhood mental health symptoms: National longitudinal study of Australian children. Pediatrics, 128(4), e865879. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2011-0491CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bayley, N. (2006). Bayley scales of infant and toddler development (3rd ed). Harcourt Assessment, a company based in San Antonio, TX.Google Scholar
Behrendt, H. F., Wade, M., Bayet, L., Nelson, C. A., & Bosquet Enlow, M. (2020). Pathways to social-emotional functioning in the preschool period: The role of child temperament and maternal anxiety in boys and girls. Development and Psychopathology, 32(3), 961974. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000853CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behrman, R. E., & Butler, A. S. (2007). Preterm birth: Causes, consequences, and prevention. The National Academies Press, Washington, DC, USA. https://doi.org/10.17226/11622Google Scholar
Blok, E., de Mol, C. L., van der Ende, J., Hillegers, M. H. J., Althoff, R. R., Shaw, P., & White, T. (2022). Stability and change of psychopathology symptoms throughout childhood and adolescence. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 53(6), 13301339. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-021-01212-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cawley, G. C., & Talbot, N. L. C. (2010). On over-fitting in model selection and subsequent selection bias in performance evaluation. Journal of Machine Learning Research, 11, 20792107.Google Scholar
Christodoulou, E., Ma, J., Collins, G. S., Steyerberg, E. W., Verbakel, J. Y., & Van Calster, B. (2019). A systematic review shows no performance benefit of machine learning over logistic regression for clinical prediction models. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 110, 1222. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2019.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (2016). Socioemotional, personality, and biological development: Illustrations from a multilevel developmental psychopathology perspective on child maltreatment. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 187211. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Constantino, J. N., Davis, S. A., Todd, R. D., Schindler, M. K., Gross, M. M., Brophy, S. L., Metzger, L. M., Shoushtari, C. S., Splinter, R., & Reich, W. (2003). Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(4), 427433. https://doi.org/10.1023/a:1025014929212CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Craig, F., Crippa, A., Ruggiero, M., Rizzato, V., Russo, L., Fanizza, I., & Trabacca, A. (2021). Characterization of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) subtypes based on the relationship between motor skills and social communication abilities. Human Movement Science, 77, 102802. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.humov.2021.102802CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damon, W., Lerner, R. M., & Eisenberg, N. (2006). Handbook of child psychology, social, emotional, and personality development. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Delobel-Ayoub, M., Arnaud, C., White-Koning, M., Casper, C., Pierrat, V., Garel, M., Burguet, A., Roze, J. C., Matis, J., Picaud, J. C., Kaminski, M., Larroque, B., & Grp, E. S. (2009). Behavioral problems and cognitive performance at 5 years of age after very preterm birth: The EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics, 123(6), 14851492. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2008-1216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Delobel-Ayoub, M., Kaminski, M., Marret, S., Burguet, A., Marchand, L., N’Guyen, S., Matis, J., Thiriez, G., Fresson, J., Arnaud, C., Poher, M., Larroque, B., & Group, E. S. (2006). Behavioral outcome at 3 years of age in very preterm infants: The EPIPAGE study. Pediatrics, 117(6), 19962005. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2005-2310CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dollar, J. M., & Stifter, C. A. (2012). Temperamental surgency and emotion regulation as predictors of childhood social competence. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(2), 178194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.02.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Donati, G., Meaburn, E., & Dumontheil, I. (2021). Internalising and externalising in early adolescence predict later executive function, not the other way around: A cross-lagged panel analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 35(5), 986998. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2021.1918644CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Donoso, J., Rattray, F., de Bildt, A., Tillmann, J., Williams, P., Absoud, M., & Totsika, V. (2023). Association of cognitive and adaptive skills with internalizing and externalizing problems in autistic children and adolescents. Autism Research, 17(3), 596609. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.3056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, A. D., Redshaw, M. E., Kennea, N., Rivero-Arias, O., Gonzales-Cinca, N., Nongena, P., Ederies, M., Falconer, S., Chew, A., Omar, O., Hardy, P., Harvey, M. E., Eddama, O., Hayward, N., Wurie, J., Azzopardi, D., Rutherford, M. A., Counsell, S., & ePrime, I. (2018). Effect of MRI on preterm infants and their families: A randomised trial with nested diagnostic and economic evaluation. Archives of Disease in Childhood. Fetal and Neonatal Edition, 103(1), F15F21. https://doi.org/10.1136/archdischild-2017-313102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Engle, J. M., & McElwain, N. L. (2011). Parental reactions to toddlers’ negative emotions and child negative emotionality as correlates of problem behavior at the age of three. Social Development, 20(2), 251271. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9507.2010.00583.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisher, A., Thiessen, E., Godwin, K., Kloos, H., & Dickerson, J. (2013). Assessing selective sustained attention in 3- to 5-year-old children: evidence from a new paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 114(2), 275294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.07.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fosco, G. M., & Lydon-Staley, D. M. (2020). Implications of family cohesion and conflict for adolescent mood and well-being: Examining within- and between-family processes on a daily timescale. Family Process, 59(4), 16721689. https://doi.org/10.1111/famp.12515CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 3160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gioia, G. A., Isquith, P. K., Guy, S. C., & Kenworthy, L. (2000). Behavior rating inventory of executive function: BRIEF. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.Google Scholar
Goodman, A., Lamping, D. L., & Ploubidis, G. B. (2010). When to use broader internalising and externalising subscales instead of the hypothesised five subscales on the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ): Data from British parents, teachers and children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(8), 11791191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-010-9434-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38(5), 581586. http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/9255702 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallett, V., Ronald, A., Rijsdijk, F., & Happe, F. (2010). Association of autistic-like and internalizing traits during childhood: A longitudinal twin study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 167(7), 809817. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09070990CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrichs, J., van den Heuvel, M. I., Witteveen, A. B., Wilschut, J., & Van den Bergh, B. R. H. (2019). Does mindful parenting mediate the association between maternal anxiety during pregnancy and child behavioral/emotional problems? Mindfulness, 12(2), 370380. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-019-01115-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobel, C. J., Dunkel-Schetter, C., Roesch, S. C., Castro, L. C., & Arora, C. P. (1999). Maternal plasma corticotropin-releasing hormone associated with stress at 20 weeks’ gestation in pregnancies ending in preterm delivery. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 180(1 Pt 3), S257263. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9378(99)70712-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hollo, A., Wehby, J. H., & Oliver, R. M. (2014). Unidentified language deficits in children with emotional and behavioral disorders: A meta-analysis. Exceptional Children, 80(2), 169186. https://doi.org/10.1177/001440291408000203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30, 179185. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hosokawa, R., & Katsura, T. (2024). Association between parents’ perceived social support and children’s psychological adjustment: A cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatrics, 24(1), 756. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12887-024-05235-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostinar, C. E., & Gunnar, M. R. (2013). Future directions in the study of social relationships as regulators of the HPA axis across development. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 42(4), 564575. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.804387CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang-Pollock, C., Shapiro, Z., Galloway-Long, H., & Weigard, A. (2017). Is poor working memory a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 45(8), 14771490. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-016-0219-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jamshidian, M., & Jalal, S. (2010). Tests of homoscedasticity, normality, and missing completely at random for incomplete multivariate data. Psychometrika, 75(4), 649674. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11336-010-9175-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S., & Marlow, N. (2011). Preterm birth and childhood psychiatric disorders. Pediatric Research, 69(5 Pt 2), 11R18R. https://doi.org/10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212faa0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S., Marlow, N., Wolke, D., Davidson, L., Marston, L., O’Hare, A., Peacock, J., & Schulte, J. (2004). Validation of a parent report measure of cognitive development in very preterm infants. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 46(6), 389397. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162204000635CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, S., O’Reilly, H., Ni, Y., Wolke, D., & Marlow, N. (2019). Psychiatric symptoms and disorders in extremely preterm young adults at 19 years of age and longitudinal findings from middle childhood. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 58(8), 820826.e6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2019.02.020CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahle, S., Utendale, W. T., Widaman, K. F., & Hastings, P. D. (2018). Parasympathetic regulation and inhibitory control predict the development of externalizing problems in early childhood. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 46(2), 237249. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-017-0305-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kanel, D., Vanes, L. D., Pecheva, D., Hadaya, L., Falconer, S., Counsell, S. J., Edwards, D. A., & Nosarti, C. (2021). Neonatal white matter microstructure and emotional development during the preschool years in children who were born very preterm. eNeuro, 8(5), 112. https://doi.org/10.1523/ENEURO.0546-20.2021CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karcher, N. R., Michelini, G., Kotov, R., & Barch, D. M. (2021). Associations between resting-state functional connectivity and a hierarchical dimensional structure of psychopathology in middle childhood. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 6(5), 508517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.09.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khanam, R., & Nghiem, S. (2018). Behavioural and emotional problems in children and educational outcomes: A dynamic panel data analysis. Administration and Policy in Mental Health, 45(3), 472483. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10488-017-0837-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kleine, I., Falconer, S., Roth, S., Counsell, S. J., Redshaw, M., Kennea, N., Edwards, A. D., & Nosarti, C. (2020). Early postnatal maternal trait anxiety is associated with the behavioural outcomes of children born preterm <33 weeks. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 131, 160168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.09.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Komsi, N., Raikkonen, K., Pesonen, A. K., Heinonen, K., Keskivaara, P., Jarvenpaa, A. L., & Strandberg, T. E. (2006). Continuity of temperament from infancy to middle childhood. Infant Behavior & Development, 29(4), 494508. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lahey, B. B., Rathouz, P. J., Keenan, K., Stepp, S. D., Loeber, R., & Hipwell, A. E. (2015). Criterion validity of the general factor of psychopathology in a prospective study of girls. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 56(4), 415422. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lawrence, A. J., Stahl, D., Duan, S., Fennema, D., Jaeckle, T., Young, A. H., Dazzan, P., Moll, J., & Zahn, R. (2022). Neurocognitive measures of self-blame and risk prediction models of recurrence in major depressive disorder. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(3), 256264. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.06.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Linsell, L., Malouf, R., Morris, J., Kurinczuk, J. J., & Marlow, N. (2017). Risk factor models for neurodevelopmental outcomes in children born very preterm or with very low birth weight: A systematic review of methodology and reporting. American Journal of Epidemiology, 185(7), 601612. https://doi.org/10.1093/aje/kww135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maggi, S., Irwin, L. J., Siddiqi, A., & Hertzman, C. (2010). The social determinants of early child development: An overview. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 46(11), 627635. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1754.2010.01817.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandy, W., Midouhas, E., Hosozawa, M., Cable, N., Sacker, A., & Flouri, E. (2022). Mental health and social difficulties of late-diagnosed autistic children, across childhood and adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 63(11), 14051414. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13587CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meehan, K. B., De Panfilis, C., Cain, N. M., & Clarkin, J. F. (2013). Effortful control and externalizing problems in young adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(5), 553558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.04.019CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikolajewski, A. J., Allan, N. P., Hart, S. A., Lonigan, C. J., & Taylor, J. (2013). Negative affect shares genetic and environmental influences with symptoms of childhood internalizing and externalizing disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(3), 411423. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-012-9681-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moberget, T., Alnaes, D., Kaufmann, T., Doan, N. T., Cordova-Palomera, A., Norbom, L. B., Rokicki, J., van der Meer, D., Andreassen, O. A., & Westlye, L. T. (2019). Cerebellar gray matter volume is associated with cognitive function and psychopathology in adolescence. Biological Psychiatry, 86(1), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2019.01.019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moons, K. G., Donders, R. A., Stijnen, T., & Harrell, F. E. Jr. (2006). Using the outcome for imputation of missing predictor values was preferred. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 59(10), 10921101. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jclinepi.2006.01.009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Office for National Statistics (2019). Research report on population estimates by ethnic group and religion. ONS London.Google Scholar
Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., De Winter, A. F., Veenstra, R., & Ormel, J. (2004). Temperament profiles associated with internalizing and externalizing problems in preadolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 16(2), 421440. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579404044591CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldehinkel, A. J., Hartman, C. A., Ferdinand, R. F., Verhulst, F. C., & Ormel, J. (2007). Effortful control as modifier of the association between negative emotionality and adolescents’ mental health problems. Development and Psychopathology, 19(2), 523539. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407070253CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papachristou, E., & Flouri, E. (2020). The codevelopment of internalizing symptoms, externalizing symptoms, and cognitive ability across childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology, 32(4), 13751389. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419001330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patwardhan, I., Nelson, T. D., McClelland, M. M., & Mason, W. A. (2021). Childhood cognitive flexibility and externalizing and internalizing behavior problems: Examination of prospective bidirectional associations. Research on Child and Adolescent Psychopathology, 49(4), 413427. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-020-00757-xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettersson, E., Lahey, B. B., Larsson, H., & Lichtenstein, P. (2018). Criterion validity and utility of the general factor of psychopathology in childhood: Predictive associations with independently measured severe adverse mental health outcomes in adolescence. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(6), 372383. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, S. P., Ellis, L. K., Rothbart, M. K., Eliasz, A., & Angleitner, A. (2001). Advances in research on temperament (pp. 165182). Lengerich: Pabst Science.Google Scholar
Putnam, S. P., Gartstein, M. A., & Rothbart, M. K. (2006). Measurement of fine-grained aspects of toddler temperament: The early childhood behavior questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 29(3), 386401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.01.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rhee, S. H., Friedman, N. P., Smith Watts, A. K., Corley, R. P., Hewitt, J. K., Robinson, J., & Zahn-Waxler, C. (2018). The association between toddlerhood self-control and later externalizing problems. Behavior Genetics, 48(2), 125134. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10519-017-9886-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rice, F., Harold, G. T., Boivin, J., van den Bree, M., Hay, D. F., & Thapar, A. (2010). The links between prenatal stress and offspring development and psychopathology: Disentangling environmental and inherited influences. Psychological Medicine, 40(2), 335345. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291709005911CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieffe, C., Ketelaar, L., & Wiefferink, C. H. (2010). Assessing empathy in young children: Construction and validation of an empathy questionnaire (EmQue). Personality and Individual Differences, 49(5), 362367. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.03.046CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robins, D., Fein, D., & Barton, M. (1999). Modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT) follow-up interview. Storrs, Connecticut, USA.Google Scholar
Romer, A. L., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2021). Is executive dysfunction a risk marker or consequence of psychopathology? A test of executive function as a prospective predictor and outcome of general psychopathology in the adolescent brain cognitive development study(R). Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 51, 100994. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.100994CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ronald, A., Edelson, L. R., Asherson, P., & Saudino, K. J. (2010). Exploring the relationship between autistic-like traits and ADHD behaviors in early childhood: Findings from a community twin study of 2-year-olds. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38(2), 185196. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-009-9366-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosseel, Y. (2012). lavaan: AnRPackage for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software, 48(2), 136. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v048.i02CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rueda, M. R., Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Halparin, J. D., Gruber, D. B., Lercari, L. P., & Posner, M. I. (2004). Development of attentional networks in childhood. Neuropsychologia, 42(8), 10291040. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.12.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sallis, H., Szekely, E., Neumann, A., Jolicoeur-Martineau, A., van, I. M., Hillegers, M., Greenwood, C. M. T., Meaney, M. J., Steiner, M., Tiemeier, H., Wazana, A., Pearson, R. M., & Evans, J. (2019). General psychopathology, internalising and externalising in children and functional outcomes in late adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 60(11), 11831190. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salomone, E., Settanni, M., Ferrara, F., Salandin, A., & Team, C. S. T. I. (2019). The interplay of communication skills, emotional and behavioural problems and parental psychological distress. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(11), 43654374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-04142-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Samuelsson, M., Holsti, A., Adamsson, M., Serenius, F., Hagglof, B., & Farooqi, A. (2017). Behavioral patterns in adolescents born at 23 to 25 weeks of gestation. Pediatrics, 140(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2017-0199CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Secinti, D. D., & Gürbüz, H. G. A. (2021). The relationship between temperamental characteristics and internalizing and externalizing problems in early adolescents between 11–14 years old. Turkish Journal of Child & Adolescent Mental Health, 28(2), 117123.10.4274/tjcamh.galenos.2020.86580CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation checklist. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 906916.10.1037/0012-1649.33.6.906CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shih, E. W., Ahmad, S. I., Bush, N. R., Roubinov, D., Tylavsky, F., Graff, C., Karr, C. J., Sathyanarayana, S., & LeWinn, K. Z. (2023). A path model examination: Maternal anxiety and parenting mediate the association between maternal adverse childhood experiences and children’s internalizing behaviors. Psychological Medicine, 53(1), 112122. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291721001203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonds, J., & Rothbart, M. K. (2004). The temperament in middle childhood questionnaire (TMCQ): A computerized self-report instrument for ages 7–10. In Poster session present occasional temperament conference Athens, GA,10.1037/t70081-000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, S. H. (1998). A measure of anxiety symptoms among children. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 36(5), 545566.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00034-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spence, S. H., Najman, J. M., Bor, W., O’Callaghan, M. J., & Williams, G. M. (2002). Maternal anxiety and depression, poverty and marital relationship factors during early childhood as predictors of anxiety and depressive symptoms in adolescence. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(4), 457469. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spielberger, C. D. (1970). Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory (self-evaluation questionnaire). Consulting Psychologists Press, Palo Alto, California.Google Scholar
Stekhoven, D. J., & Buhlmann, P. (2012). MissForest–non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics, 28(1), 112118. https://doi.org/10.1093/bioinformatics/btr597CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stice, L. V., & Lavner, J. A. (2019). Social connectedness and loneliness mediate the association between autistic traits and internalizing symptoms among young adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 49(3), 10961110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3812-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tajik-Parvinchi, D., Farmus, L., Tablon Modica, P., Cribbie, R. A., & Weiss, J. A. (2021). The role of cognitive control and emotion regulation in predicting mental health problems in children with neurodevelopmental disorders. Child: Care, Health and Development, 47(5), 608617. https://doi.org/10.1111/cch.12868CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tamayo Martinez, N., Tiemeier, H., Luijk, M., Law, J., van der Ende, J., Verhulst, F., & Jansen, P. W. (2021). Aggressive behavior, emotional, and attention problems across childhood and academic attainment at the end of primary school. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 56(5), 837846. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-021-02039-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torry, D. S., Wang, H. S., Wang, T. H., Caudle, M. R., & Torry, R. J. (1998). Preeclampsia is associated with reduced serum levels of placenta growth factor. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 179(6 Pt 1), 15391544. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0002-9378(98)70021-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Treyvaud, K., Ure, A., Doyle, L. W., Lee, K. J., Rogers, C. E., Kidokoro, H., Inder, T. E., & Anderson, P. J. (2013). Psychiatric outcomes at age seven for very preterm children: Rates and predictors. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(7), 772779. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12040CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vanes, L. D., Hadaya, L., Kanel, D., Falconer, S., Ball, G., Batalle, D., Counsell, S. J., Edwards, A. D., & Nosarti, C. (2021). Associations between neonatal brain structure, the home environment, and childhood outcomes following very preterm birth. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science, 1(2), 146155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2021.05.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., & Liu, Y. (2021). The development of internalizing and externalizing problems in primary school: Contributions of executive function and social competence. Child Development, 92(3), 889903. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13462CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wechsler, D. (2012). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological CorporationGoogle Scholar
Wolke, D., Johnson, S., & Mendonça, M. (2019). The life course consequences of very preterm birth. Annual Review of Developmental Psychology, 1(1), 6992. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-084804CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yaari, M., Treyvaud, K., Lee, K. J., Doyle, L. W., & Anderson, P. J. (2019). Preterm birth and maternal mental health: Longitudinal trajectories and predictors. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 44(6), 736747. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jsz019CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, Y., Shields, G. S., Zhang, Y., Wu, H., Chen, H., & Romer, A. L. (2022). Child executive function and future externalizing and internalizing problems: A meta-analysis of prospective longitudinal studies. Clinical Psychology Review, 97, 102194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2022.102194CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zelazo, P. D. (2006). The dimensional change card sort (DCCS): A method of assessing executive function in children. Nature Protocols, 1(1), 297301. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2006.46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zou, H., & Hastie, T. (2005). Regularization and variable selection via the elastic net. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B: Statistical Methodology, 67(2), 301320. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9868.2005.00503.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Recruitment flow chart.

Figure 1

Table 1. Clinical, socio-demographic and parental characteristics of the sample (n = 153)

Figure 2

Figure 2. Heatmap of loadings of each variable on Factor 1 (externalising problems) and Factor 2 (internalising-socio-emotional problems). Positive loadings (>0) are indicated by red and negative loadings (<0) by blue.

Figure 3

Table 2. Model performance measures of cross-validations (k = 10, lambda = 100)

Figure 4

Table 3. Prospective predictors of behavioural and mental health outcomes in late childhood

Supplementary material: File

Sun et al. supplementary material

Sun et al. supplementary material
Download Sun et al. supplementary material(File)
File 26.6 KB