Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-kl2l2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-20T12:57:55.888Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A register approach to modal (non-)concord in English: an experimental study of linguistic and social meaning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 July 2025

Stephanie Rotter*
Affiliation:
Department of English and American Studies, https://ror.org/01hcx6992 Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin, Germany
Mingya Liu
Affiliation:
Department of English and American Studies, https://ror.org/01hcx6992 Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin , Berlin, Germany
*
Corresponding author: Stephanie Rotter; Email: rotterst@hu-berlin.de
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Modal concord refers to the phenomenon where the co-occurrence of two modal elements with the same flavor and force (e.g. may possibly, must certainly) gives rise to the interpretation of single modality. Given their (arguably) equivalent semantics, constructions with modal concord and single modal (e.g. may, must) can function as alternative choices in different contexts of use – how do speakers choose between them, and how is the choice perceived? In this article, we take a ‘Register’ approach and report an experimental study of MC in US English, addressing their linguistic and social meanings with versus without situational context. The results show that (i) modal concord constructions differ from single modal ones in linguistic meanings, which casts doubt on the concord assumption, and (ii) modal concord has distinct social meanings from those of single modal constructions. Our findings suggest a correlation between the meaning strength of a linguistic expression and the social perception about the speaker. Context, manipulated via the single situational parameter of interlocutor relation (close vs. distant), did not interact with the linguistic or social meaning of modal concord constructions, the implications of which are discussed in relation to the multidimensional nature of conversational situations and the method applied.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

1. Introduction

Recent research in formal and experimental linguistics shows growing interest in the social meaning of variation in language users’ choices among functionally equivalent or similar variants, integrating formal grammar with methods of sociolinguistics, language comprehension and perception (e.g. Beltrama Reference Beltrama2020; Hall-Lew et al. Reference Hall-Lew, Moore, Podesva, Hall-Lew, Moore and Podesva2021; Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023). Doubling phenomena – constructions in which multiple elements with similar semantics co-occur (Barbiers et al. Reference Barbiers, Koeneman, Lekakou, van der Ham, Barbiers, Koeneman, Lekakou and van der Ham2008) – are particularly useful for investigating variation as social behavior, since their semantics is – arguably – equivalent to that of their single element counterparts. In English, several such phenomena, including negative concord, multiple modals and double comparatives (see (1a), (1b) and (1c)), are commonly associated with dialects, though their distribution varies considerably across different varieties. For example, negative concord, where two negative elements co-occur (not…nothing), and double comparative, where the synthetic and analytical comparative merge (more friendlier), are among the eleven most frequently attested morphosyntactic features in at least 34 out of 46 English varieties worldwide (Szmrecsanyi & Kortmann Reference Szmrecsanyi, Kortmann, Kortmann and Schneider2004). Given their wide distribution, understanding how language comprehenders perceive these constructions is key to exploring their social meaning and how linguistic variation is interpreted in different conversational contexts.

This article deals with the co-occurrence of modal verbs and adverbs in English – a phenomenon unlike the other ones listed in that it is not typically limited to dialectal use. As illustrated in (2), modality can be expressed through a single modal element (SM), either an auxiliary verb (2a) or adverb (2b), or with both co-occurring in the same clause (2c), known as modal concord (MC).

According to some analyses (Geurts & Huitink Reference Geurts, Huitink, Dekker and Zeijlstra2006; Zeijlstra Reference Zeijlstra2007), when both modal elements share the same modal flavor (e.g. epistemic modality) and force (e.g. existential or universal), MC is interpreted equivalent to single modality, thus aligning with the concept of concord (Dekker & Zeijlstra Reference Dekker and Zeijlstra2012).

Similar to other concord phenomena – like negative concord – MC may be subject to extralinguistic restrictions, such as register, defined as recurrent intra- and interindividual variation influenced by situational-functional parameters (Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023; Lüdeling et al. Reference Lüdeling, Szucsich, Zeige, Adli, Alexiadou, Belz, Bouzouita, Bunk, Dreyer, Egg, Feulner, Fleischer, Gagarina, Hirsch, Jannedy, Knoeferle, Krause, Kutscher, Liu, Lütke, Machicao, Priemer, Hernandez, Meyer, Mooshammer, Müller, Sauerland, Sauermann, Schmitt, Schumacher, Serova, Solt, Klok, Verhoeven, Waltereit, Weirich and Wiese2024). Language users are assumed to adjust their speech according to aspects of the situation, e.g. the interlocutor they talk to (boss vs. mother, Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter and Liu2023). Such choices can signal different social meanings, which is ‘the set of inferences that can be drawn on the basis of how language is used in specific interactions’ (Hall-Lew et al. Reference Hall-Lew, Moore, Podesva, Hall-Lew, Moore and Podesva2021: 3).

The distribution and social meaning of negative concord has been extensively studied, whereas comparable work on the co-occurrence of modal verbs and adverbs is scarce. Unlike negative concord and double comparatives, which are frequently considered as stigmatized (see e.g. González-Díaz Reference González-Díaz and van Ostade2008; Eckert Reference Eckert2019), MC does not carry such a stigma. Recent experimental approaches have explored the influence of register on doubling phenomena and their counterparts, as well as their perceived linguistic and social meanings. The results revealed differences in register sensitivity (see Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter and Liu2024 for negative concord; Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Fagen, Quain, Reyes and Tang2023 for MC; Alexiadou et al. Reference Alexiadou, Oikonomou and Rotter2025 for double comparatives), social meaning and perceived grammaticality in the absence of situational contexts (see Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025 for MC; Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter, Liu, van der Auwera and Gianollo2025 for negative concord). Crucially, the studies found interpretive differences between the doubling phenomena and their (single element) counterparts – challenging semantic equivalence as the core assumption of the concord-analysis (Dekker & Zeijlstra Reference Dekker and Zeijlstra2012).

Offering a unique lens on modality across interpretation, sociolinguistic variation and register, this article takes an experimental approach to investigate MC in US English – focusing on its linguistic and social meanings, grammaticality, and register sensitivity. Building on the experimental set-up (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025), we extended the rating study with situational context, tackling the following research questions (RQ):

(RQ1-linguistic meaning) What is the interpretation of MC constructions? Our results show that they differ from SM in terms of speaker commitment.

(RQ2-social meaning) What is the perceived social meaning of MC constructions? MC and SM showed distinct social meanings.

(RQ3-grammaticality) How are MC constructions perceived in terms of grammaticality? We found that MC is rated less grammatical than SM.

(RQ4-register sensitivity) Are MC constructions sensitive to the register, i.e. situational contexts? There was no evidence for register sensitivity with respect to the single parameter interlocutor relationship.

This article is structured as follows: section 2 reviews theoretical work on MC, register and social meaning. Section 3 details the rationale, method and results of the experimental approach. Section 4 discusses the results in relation to the research questions and section 5 concludes the article.

2. Background

The following two sections provide further theoretical background on previous analyses of modal doubling in section 2.1 and on register and social meaning in section 2.2.

2.1. Previous analyses: modal concord vs. spread

In terms of grammar, the discussion of co-occurring modal verb and adverb dates back to at least the 1970s, see the quote below:

In most dialects of English not more than one modal verb can occur within the same clause. But both a modal verb and a modal adverb may be combined. When this happens a distinction is to be drawn between modally harmonic and modally non-harmonic combinations. For example, ‘possibly’ and ‘may’, if each is being used epistemically, are harmonic, in that they both express the same degree of modality, whereas ‘certainly’ and ‘may’ are, in this sense, modally non-harmonic. It has been pointed out by Halliday ([Reference Halliday1970]: 331) that the adverb and the modal verb may, and normally do, “reinforce each other” in a modally harmonic combination; so that, … there is a kind of concord running through the clause, which results in the double realization of a single modality. (Lyons Reference Lyons1977: 807–8)

More recent literature labels constructions like may possibly as MC (Geurts & Huitink Reference Geurts, Huitink, Dekker and Zeijlstra2006; Zeijlstra Reference Zeijlstra2007), associating them with other concord (or agreement) phenomena under the central idea that doubling adds no effects to semantic interpretations. Following this approach, the – optional – modal adverb possibly would be semantically vacuous.

Giannakidou & Mari (Reference Giannakidou and Mari2018) propose an alternative analysis of such phenomena under the label of ‘modal spread’ (see also Huitink Reference Huitink2012 for a similar proposal). They focus on the universal must vs. existential modal verb might with the three modal adverbs definitely, probably and maybe in English, Italian and Greek. Putting aside the technical details, universal modals – the weak necessity modal verb must – are inherently positively biased, whereas possibility modals, such as might, are neutral in terms of speaker commitment, i.e. the likelihood of the modified proposition being true or false to be equal for the speaker. Furthermore, the co-occurrence of must with the adverb definitely strengthens the default positive bias, probably merely maintains the default and maybe weakens it. In contrast, when the adverbs co-occur with might, the adverbs definitely and probably both introduce positive bias, whereas maybe merely maintains the default; see examples (77) and (78) in Giannakidou & Mari (Reference Giannakidou and Mari2018: 648) for details.

In our study, we investigate the MC constructions must certainly and may possibly, which appeared as more frequent collocations than must definitely or might maybe in a preliminary corpus analysis of US American English. Both selected combinations are also attested in English, as reported in the literature (see e.g. Halliday Reference Halliday1970; Geurts & Huitink Reference Geurts, Huitink, Dekker and Zeijlstra2006; Zeijlstra Reference Zeijlstra2007). Assuming (i) certainly and definitely, (ii) may and might in epistemic readings, and (iii) possibly and maybe to be similar, we derive the preliminary hypotheses in (3), based on the concord and modal spread analyses: the predictions diverge for certainly with universal modals, where the concord analysis assumes equal speaker commitment for must certainly and must, while the spread analysis predicts a strengthening effect of must certainly in comparison to must; see (3a). In contrast, the predictions align for existential modals, as both analyses interpret may possibly and may with equivalent speaker commitment; see (3b).

Despite such attempts to integrate MC constructions into semantic theories, the empirical picture remains unclear. One of our goals is to experimentally investigate whether modal doubling of the same force and flavor has the same semantics as their single element counterparts.

2.2. Register and social meaning

The term register has been used in various ways (see Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023 for an overview and references therein). Following recent work, we define register as recurrent intra- and inter-individual variation in linguistic behavior shaped by situational-functional parameters (Lüdeling et al. Reference Lüdeling, Szucsich, Zeige, Adli, Alexiadou, Belz, Bouzouita, Bunk, Dreyer, Egg, Feulner, Fleischer, Gagarina, Hirsch, Jannedy, Knoeferle, Krause, Kutscher, Liu, Lütke, Machicao, Priemer, Hernandez, Meyer, Mooshammer, Müller, Sauerland, Sauermann, Schmitt, Schumacher, Serova, Solt, Klok, Verhoeven, Waltereit, Weirich and Wiese2024; Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023). Linguistic variation is typically described in terms of concrete instantiations – variants – of an abstract linguistic variable (Labov Reference Labov1972), e.g. the [-ING] variable with its variants -ing and -in’ (Campbell-Kibler Reference Campbell-Kibler2010). Traditional definitions require variants to be semantically equivalent (Labov Reference Labov1972: 188), which poses challenges for non-phonological variables (Lavandera Reference Lavandera1978; Tagliamonte Reference Tagliamonte2006), e.g. dinner and supper refer to the same activity (meal in the evening) but have different meanings. To address this, semantic sameness was replaced with functional equivalence (Lavandera Reference Lavandera1978; Dines Reference Dines1980), requiring variants to perform a similar function on some linguistic level. This, however, demands clear formal criteria to quantify all possible variants, and their actual and potential appearances (see a detailed discussion in Pichler Reference Pichler2010). For the present article, we assume that functions are multilayered and adopt the concept of structural sameness (Christensen & Jensen Reference Christensen, Jensen, Christensen and Jensen2022). Thus, we look at syntactic variation, which is functionally similar on some level and structurally comparable, e.g. presence vs. absence of words. Furthermore, we use the broader term alternatives (Lüdeling et al. Reference Lüdeling, Szucsich, Zeige, Adli, Alexiadou, Belz, Bouzouita, Bunk, Dreyer, Egg, Feulner, Fleischer, Gagarina, Hirsch, Jannedy, Knoeferle, Krause, Kutscher, Liu, Lütke, Machicao, Priemer, Hernandez, Meyer, Mooshammer, Müller, Sauerland, Sauermann, Schmitt, Schumacher, Serova, Solt, Klok, Verhoeven, Waltereit, Weirich and Wiese2024) which allows us to compare MC and SM without assuming semantic or pragmatic sameness.

The concept of register presupposes that language users adjust their speech on all linguistic levels, with these adjustments being (culturally) conventionalized within a speech community (Agha Reference Agha2007; Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023). Situational-functional parameters, shaping this variation, are multidimensional. For instance, the situational context varies depending on, among others, the location (e.g. school vs. youth club, Creber & Giles Reference Creber and Giles1983), or clothing style (cf. Slepian et al. Reference Slepian, Ferber, Gold and Rutchick2015). Functional parameters include the communicative purpose; speech can be used, among other things, to narrate a story adhering to specific rules (e.g. sequential reporting, see Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023) or to construct a specific persona (Eckert Reference Eckert2024). In such cases, linguistic choices are infused with social meanings (Eckert & Labov Reference Eckert and Labov2017). These choices, whether conscious or subconscious, become performative acts that convey supplementary, context-dependent meanings related to the speaker’s stances, identities and group affiliations (Beltrama Reference Beltrama2020; Eckert Reference Eckert2024). For instance, small semantic differences between the modal phrases you need to and you have/got to give rise to a unique social meaning – one that hinges on whether the speaker is authorized to advise the hearer on what is in their best interest (Glass Reference Glass2015).

To approach linguistic phenomena from a register view, we need to include such extralinguistic parameters in experimental work (see Pescuma et al. Reference Pescuma, Serova, Lukassek, Sauermann, Schäfer, Adli, Bildhauer, Egg, Hülk, Ito, Jannedy, Kordoni, Kühnast, Kutscher, Lange, Lehmann, Liu, Lütke, Maquate, Mooshammer, Mortezapour, Müller, Norde, Pankratz, Patarroyo, Plesca, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Rotter, Sauerland, Schnelle, Schulte, Schüppenhauer, Sell, Solt, Terada, Tsiapou, Verhoeven, Weirich, Wiese, Zaruba, Zeige, Lüdeling and Knoeferle2023 for discussion). One option is to embed linguistic material into enriched situational context. For instance, a rating study assessed the influence of interlocutor relation on the perceived level of formality as a situational manipulation (Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter and Liu2023). The study used labels indicating social relations in the distantFootnote 1 (e.g. like boss or judge) compared to the private sector (e.g. family and friends); see (4). Participants native to US American English judged the expected degree of formality of the upcoming speech (see Q1) on a seven-point Likert scale.

Results showed a clear distinction: more formal speeches were expected with distant relations than with close relations.

Recent experimental studies have used interlocutor relationships as a parameter to investigate the influence of register on doubling phenomena. However, few studies have examined MC from a register perspective, a gap we aim to fill with the experiment in section 3.2 by integrating the interlocutor relation manipulation as a contextual parameter.

3. Experiments

There is, to our knowledge, limited experimental research on the co-occurrence of modal verb and adverb in English. A previously conducted and published study – referred to here as Experiment 1 – investigated both the linguistic and social meanings of MC and its grammaticality (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025) but did not include the potential influence of the situational context; section 3.1 briefly summarizes the design and key findings. Building on this, the current article extends the approach by incorporating register as central factor in Experiment 2. Section 3.2 outlines the rationale and reports on the method and section 3.3 details the results of Experiment 2.

3.1. Experiment 1: modal concord without context (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025)

One recently published study investigated the perception of MC in comparison to its SM alternative in terms of linguistic and social meanings (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025),Footnote 2 addressing the research questions on the linguistic (RQ1) and social meaning (RQ2), and the grammaticality (RQ3).

The rating study was employed in a 2×2-factorial design with the factors NUMBER and its levels MC and SM, as well as FORCE and its levels necessity and possibility. In total 104 native speakers of US American English participated. The items followed the structure as exemplified in (5): (S1) was a static sentence across all items. (S2) introduced the critical sentences with both NUMBER and FORCE manipulation. (Q1) used the speaker commitment (Liu et al. Reference Liu, Rotter and Giannakidou2021) to access the linguistic meaning and (Q2) asked about the grammaticality.

The social meaning was assessed via antonyms on the end-points of a seven-point Likert scale as speaker perception in terms on the social background (low/high socio-economic status, low/high education level) and the persona (in/formal, im/polite, un/confident, un/friendly, cold/ warm, un/cool and obedient/rebellious), the midpoint of the scale was labeled (undecided). Participants first rated the social meaning in random sets of three scales before rating the linguistic meaning (Q1) on the speaker commitment and finally, (Q2) on the grammaticality.

The results are summarized in table 1 and figure 1. MC was rated less grammatical than its SM alternative, reflecting its more restricted distribution. Regarding (RQ1-linguistic meaning), the interaction effect showed a weakening effect in possibility and strengthening effect in necessity of MC in comparison to SM. For (RQ2), MC received lower ratings for grammaticality than SM. Regarding (RQ3-social meaning), MC was rated as less friendly, warm and cool, and more specifically ‘in possibility modals, MC was rated as significantly lower than SM in SES [socio-economic status], education and confidence levels and in necessity modals, MC was rated as more formal and more confident than SM’ (9–10).

Table 1. Summary of the significant results from Experiment 1 (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025: 9, table 5). The abbreviation ‘poss’ stands for possibility and ‘ness’ for necessity conditions. MC stands for modal concord and SM for single modal. 2-int abbreviates the two-way interaction FORCE×NUMBER. The symbol < indicates that the left entity is smaller than the right one; > represents the reverse. The symbol ‘–’ indicates the lack of a significant effect

Figure 1. By-subject means (transparent dots) of the rating measures in comparison to overall means (opaque dots with error bars) of Experiment 1 (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025: 5, figure 1). Panel A depicts the ratings of the speaker commitment (sp commitment) and grammaticality, panel B the social background measures, and panel C the persona measures. The x-axis indicates the specific measures. The top scale shows the ratings from the necessity conditions (nece), the one below those of the possibility conditions (poss). The colors indicate the factor NUMBER with MC (modal concord) in blue and SM (single modal) in yellow. The y-axis depicts the ratings on a seven-point Likert scale. SES abbreviates socioeconomic status.

3.2. Experiment 2: modal concord in context

3.2.1. Rationale

Building on previous research, we adopted a register-based approach to MC and expanded the design of Experiment 1 by introducing the situational parameter interlocutor relation (see section 2.2) in Experiment 2. By enriching the context through the manipulation, we were also able to extend our investigation to include judgments of appropriateness (Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter and Liu2024; Alexiadou et al. Reference Alexiadou, Oikonomou and Rotter2025), thereby tapping into potential register sensitivities in a more nuanced – though still simplified – approximation of natural language use. We address the following extended research questions (RQ):

(RQ1-linguistic meaning) What is the interpretation of MC constructions in situational context?

(RQ2-social meaning) What is the perceived social meaning of MC constructions?

(RQ3-grammaticality) How are MC constructions perceived in terms of grammaticality?

(RQ4-register sensitivity) Are MC constructions sensitive to the register, i.e., situational contexts?

Furthermore, we formulate the following hypotheses (H):

(H1) MC constructions would receive higher speaker commitment ratings for necessity modals and lower speaker commitment ratings for possibility modals than SM constructions for necessity modals. (We expect a significant interaction NUMBER×FORCE: MC nece > SM nece , MC poss < SM poss )

(H2) MC constructions would receive lower friendliness, warmth and coolness ratings in the persona than SM constructions. (MC < SM)

(H3) MC constructions would be rated as less grammatical than SM constructions. (MC < SM)

(H4) MC constructions would be rated as less appropriate than SM constructions. (MC < SM)

(H5) Context (in terms of distant vs. close social relations) has an influence on the perception of MC vs. SM. (two-way interaction CONTEXT×NUMBER)

Our predictions rely on findings of Experiment 1 and would replicate them ((H1), (H2) and (H3)). Furthermore we formulate a non-directional hypothesis (H5) on the influence of context. Since we examine several measures not previously tested in context with MC, we take an exploratory approach and make no directional predictions. This marks a first step toward understanding how extra-linguistic factors shape perceptions of speakers based on their linguistic choices.

3.2.2. Method

The experiment was divided into four parts, conducted in the following sequence: (P1) an information sheet and consent form, (P2) a rating study assessing the appropriateness, speaker commitment and perception of the speaker, (P3) the autism-spectrum quotient questionnaire, not included in this report, and (P4) a brief survey on demographic and linguistic backgrounds (e.g. a force-choice task which is not included in this report).

Participants. We recruited 306 native speakers of US English through the crowd-sourcing platform Prolific (www.prolific.co/). The experiment lasted approximately 45 minutes, and participants were monetarily compensated for their participation. We received from all participants informed consent as approved by the Ethics Committee of the Deutsche Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft (DGfS) in the context of SFB 1412 ‘Register’. All participants met the inclusion criteria of being native English speakers and aged between 18 and 65 years (mean age = 38.4 (SD = 11.3), range = [18, 64]; female N = 152, male N = 153, nonbinary N = 1). The participants came from 45 different states across the United States.

The majority have completed college (46.7%) or high school (30.4%), 22.6% hold a graduate degree, and below 1% did not finish high school. Roughly half of the participants grew up in a suburban environment (47.7%), one-third (34%) grew up in an urban and 18.3% in a rural environment.

Overall, 6.5% of participants reported speaking one of six dialects: Hawaiian Pidgin (N = 1), Michigan (N = 1), Midwestern (N = 1), New York (N = 1), Southern (N = 6) and West American English dialect (N = 2), with an additional four participants naming more than one dialect (dialect information missing [N = 2]). In addition, several participants identified themselves as speakers of American (N = 14), British (N = 1), informal (N = 1) and standard English (N = 1), thereby showing register knowledge concerning both regional varieties and different levels of formality.

Material and design. The experiment was conducted using PCIbex (Zehr & Schwarz Reference Zehr and Schwarz2018) and hosted on the PCIbex Farm platform (https://farm.pcibex.net/).

The rating study in (P2) employed a 2×2×2-factorial design with the factors NUMBERFootnote 3 (modal concord/MC vs. single modal/SM), FORCE (necessity vs. possibility) and CONTEXT (distant vs. close). We utilized 24 critical items, along with 16 filler items of similar structure. CONTEXT was manipulated in (S1) as pairs of interlocutor relation labels that aligned with the story (see (6)). The agents in the short stories alternated between ‘a woman’ and ‘a man’, avoiding personal names to eliminate uncontrolled social meanings. The NUMBER and FORCE factors were manipulated in (S2). Participants responded to three questions in the indicated order: (Q1) speaker commitment, (Q2) appropriateness and (Q3) grammaticality of the sentence. Participants were assigned to one of eight lists following a Latin Square design.

Procedure. Participants completed (P1–4) in the specified sequence.

In (P2), participants used the space bar to advance and the mouse to click on the options. The experiment started with a practice item to help participants familiarize themselves with the setup. Each trial started with a fixation cross centered on the screen (see panel A of figure 2). (S1) was displayed in the center of the screen, followed by (S2) on a separate screen (see panels B and C of figure 2). (S2) remained on the screen while participants rated the perceived social background (low/high socioeconomic status and low/high education) and the persona (in/formal, im/polite, un/confident, un/friendly, cold/warm, un/cool and obedient/rebellious) of the speaker. These measures appeared in two groups (five and four traits, respectively) in randomized order (see panel D of figure 2). In the final screens of the trail, the participants separately rated (Certainly no/Certainly yes) the speaker commitment (Q1), appropriateness (Q2) and the grammaticality (Q3) of the sentence. This order was chosen to reduce potential bias, ensuring that previous ratings about the appropriateness and grammaticality do not influence the speaker commitment of the sentence. Additionally, appropriateness was rated before grammaticality for similar reasons. These measures all used a seven-point Likert scale in which end and midpoints were labels (e.g. 1:informal - 4:undecided - 7:formal).

Figure 2. Screenshots of the experimental design. Panel A shows the fixation cross. Panel B shows the presentation of (S1), and panel C the presentation of (S2). Panel D shows (S2) together with the randomized Likert scale ratings.

In (P4), participants took part in a short survey on their demographic and language backgrounds. This information was used to verify the inclusion criteria and to gain insights into the sample population. No inferential analysis were conducted based on these self-reports.

Data analysis. We used the open source software ‘R’ (version 4.1.2, R Core Team 2024) in the RStudio environment to process and analyze the data. Since the study did not include attention checks, we visualized each participant’s rating patterns for critical and filler items separately prior to analysis. Since the ratings across the items did not seem monotonic, we included all participants in the analysis.

The study comprised of 12 judgment ratings (i.e., appropriateness, grammaticality, speaker commitment, socio-economic status, education level, formality, politeness, confidence, friendliness, warmth, coolness and rebelliousness). Each measure was used separately as dependent variable for a model in the cumulative link function model framework (Liddell & Kruschke Reference Liddell and Kruschke2018) using the package ‘ordinal’ (cf. Christensen Reference Christensen2015).

We determined the link function of each model as the highest log-likelihood value of the five link functions (i.e. probit, logit, cauchit, loglog and cloglog) (Christensen Reference Christensen2015). The main factors were sum-coded in the following way: NUMBER (MC: 0.5, SM: 0.5), FORCE (necessity: 0.5, possibility: 0.5) and CONTEXT (distant: 0.5, close: 0.5). The models contained the three main factors as well as all possible two-way (i.e. NUMBER×FORCE, NUMBER×CONTEXT, FORCE×CONTEXT) and three-way interactions (i.e. NUMBER× FORCE×CONTEXT). Since the experimental design includes two sub-studies based on the FORCE factor, we conducted sub-analyses by splitting the data into necessity and possibility conditions whenever a significant interaction involving the FORCE factor was observed. In such cases, we only report effects from this sub-analysis.

We determined the random effect structure by using the most parsimonious model approach, the structures used are indicated in the respective result section. If the correlation turned out to be exactly 1 or 1, we regressed to the simpler model. The p-values were obtained with the help of log-likelihood ratio test comparisons of nested models (Bates et al. Reference Bates, Kliegl, Vasishth and Baayen2018). We report them as significant at a value below 0.05. All statistical values of means, estimates and the like are rounded to the second decimals except for p-values smaller than 0.01.

3.3. Results

Figure 3 depicts the overall and subject means of the ratings. In the following sections, we detail the results of the descriptive statistics and inferential analysis of each of the twelve dependent measurements. The results are indexed with continuous numbers in relation to the RQs (see section 3.2.1), e.g. RQ1.1 for the first result in relation to (RQ1-linguistic meaning).

Figure 3. By-subject means (transparent dots) of the rating measures in comparison to overall means (squares). Panel A depicts the ratings of the speaker commitment (Sp. commitment), grammaticality and appropriateness, panel B the social background measures, and panel C the persona measures. The x-axis indicates the CONTEXT factor. The top scale shows the ratings from the necessity conditions (nece), the one below the possibility conditions (poss). The colors indicate the factor NUMBER: MC (i.e. modal concord) in blue and SM (i.e. single modal) in yellow. The y-axis depicts the ratings on a seven-point Likert scale. SES abbreviates socioeconomic status.

This section continues as follows: in section 3.3.1, we detail the results from the speaker commitment ratings. In section 3.3.2, we report the results from the social meaning measures. Section 3.3.3 focuses on the results from the grammaticality and section 3.3.4 on the appropriateness ratings.

3.3.1. Linguistic meaning: speaker commitment

For the speaker commitment ratings, the descriptive statistics are shown in table 2 and the output of the related model in table 3.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the speaker commitment ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Table 3. Output of the models using the speaker commitment measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

The logit link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE and their two-way interaction (NUMBER×FORCE), as well as intercepts for item intercepts with slopes for FORCE and CONTEXT provided the best fit to the data. The results showed a significant two-way interaction NUMBER×FORCE ( $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 1.60, χ2(1) = 136.06, p < 0.0001). No other interaction turned out significant.

Two models were fit to subsets of the data based on the FORCE levels. The necessity model showed the best fit using a cloglog link function, with random intercepts for subjects and items, and by-subject random slopes for NUMBER. The possibility-model used the logit link function with random subject and item intercepts. The results revealed a cross-over effect in that MC was rated higher than SM in necessity conditions (RQ1.1, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.87, χ2(1) = 113.64, p < 0.0001), while in possibility conditions the reverse effect was found (RQ1.2, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.43, χ2(1) = 46.23, p < 0.0001). No further effect turned out significant.

3.3.2 Social meaning

For the social background ratings, the descriptive statistics are shown in table 4 and the output of the related models are shown in table 5.

Table 4. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the social background ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Table 5. Output of the models using the social background measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

For the socio-economic status ratings, the probit link model that included random inter-cepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE and their two-way interaction (NUMBER ×FORCE), as well as intercepts for items with slopes for FORCE and CONTEXT fit the data best. The results showed no significant main or interaction effect.

For the education level ratings, the loglog link model that included only random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE, CONTEXT, as well as the three two-way interactions. The results showed a significant main effect of NUMBER in that MC conditions were rated with lower education levels than SM conditions (RQ2.1, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.14, χ2(1) = 8.16, p = 0.004). The main effect CONTEXT showed significant levels in that distant conditions were rated with lower education levels than close conditions (RQ2.2, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.08, χ2(1) = 6.07, p = 0.01). No other main or interaction effects turned out significant.

The descriptive statistics of the persona ratings are shown in table 6 and the output of the related models are shown in table 7 and 8.

Table 6. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the persona ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Table 7. Output of the models using the first part of the persona measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Table 8. Output of the models using the second part of the persona measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

For the formality ratings, the loglog link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE, CONTEXT and the two-way interaction NUMBER×FORCE, along with intercepts for items fit the data best. The results showed a significant NUMBER×FORCE interaction ( $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.58, χ2(1) = 29.77, p < 0.0001). No other interaction turned out significant.

For the sub-analyses, the necessity-model was fit using the logit and the possibility model using the loglog link function, both used NUMBER and CONTEXT slopes for subject intercepts, along with item intercepts. The results revealed that in only the necessity conditions, MC was rated as more formal than SM (RQ2.3, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.60, χ2(1) = 31.51, p < 0.0001). Both models showed significant CONTEXT effects in that close conditions were rated higher than distant ones (RQ2.4, necessity: $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.66, χ2(1) = 40.15, p < 0.0001; RQ2.5, possibility: $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.42, χ2(1) = 46.81, p < 0.0001). No further interaction effect turned out significant.

For the politeness ratings, the loglog link model that included only random intercepts for subjects fit the data best. The results showed a significant two-way interaction NUMBER×FORCE (RQ2.6, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.18, χ2(1) = 9.73, p = 0.002). No other interaction effect turned out significant.

For the sub-analyses, the necessity-model was fit using the loglog link function and only subject intercepts, while the possibility-model used the logit link function along with intercepts for subjects and slopes for NUMBER, as well as item intercepts. The results for both models revealed no significant results.

For the confidence ratings, the logit link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE and CONTEXT, as well as intercepts for item with slopes for FORCE and CONTEXT fit the data best. The results showed a significant two-way interaction NUMBER×FORCE ( $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.58, χ2(1) = 130.88, p < 0.0001). No other interaction turned out significant.

The necessity-model was fit using the cloglog link function together with NUMBER slopes for subject intercepts, and item intercepts. The possibility-model used the loglog link function, NUMBER slopes for subject and CONTEXT slopes for item intercepts. The results showed a cross-over effect in that MC was rated more confident than SM in necessity conditions (RQ2.7, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = 0.35, χ2(1) = 32.01, p < 0.0001) and the reverse pattern occurred for possibility conditions (RQ2.8, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.34, χ2(1) = 39.52, p < 0.0001). No further effect turned out significant.

For the friendliness ratings, the logit link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER and CONTEXT as well as random intercepts for items provided the best fit to the data. The result showed a significant main effect of NUMBER in that MC was rated as less friendly than SM (RQ2.9, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.17, χ2(1) = 26.21, p < 0.0001). The main effect of FORCE turned out significant in that necessity conditions were rated as less friendly than possibility conditions (RQ2.10, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.12, χ2(1) = 22.09, p < 0.0001). There was a significant main effect of CONTEXT in that distant conditions were rated as less friendly than close ones (RQ2.11, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.19, χ2(1) = 41.29, p < 0.0001). No interaction turned out significant.

For the warmth ratings, the loglog link model that included only subject intercepts fit the data best. The results showed a significant main effect of NUMBER in that MC was rated as less warm than SM (RQ2.12, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.18, χ2(1) = 43.53, p < 0.0001). The main effect FORCE turned out significant in that necessity conditions were rated as less warm than possibility conditions (RQ2.13, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.07, χ2(1) = 7.19, p = 0.007). There was a significant main effect of CONTEXT in that distant conditions were rated as less warm than close ones (RQ2.14, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.11, χ2(1) = 16.81, p < 0.0001). None of the interactions turned out significant.

For the coolness ratings, the loglog link model including only subject fit the data best. The results showed a significant two-way interaction NUMBER×FORCE ( $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.10, χ2(1) = 3.76, p = 0.05). No other interaction turned out significant.

For the sub-analyses, the necessity-model was fit with only subject intercepts and the possibility-model with item intercepts and NUMBER slopes for subject intercepts. Both models used the probit link function. The results showed that in both necessity (RQ2.15, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.25, χ2(1) = 49.74, p < 0.0001) and possibility (RQ2.16, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.15, χ2(1) = 12.26, p = 0.0005) conditions, NUMBER turned out significant in that MC was rated less cool than SM. No other effect of interaction turned out significant

For the rebelliousness ratings, the logit link model that included intercepts for subjects and items provided the best fit to the data. The results showed a significant main effect of CONTEXT in that distant conditions were rated as less rebellious than close ones (R.Q2.17, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.17, χ2(1) = 14.15, p = 0.0002). No other main or interaction effect turned out significant.

3.3.3. Grammaticality

For the grammaticality ratings, the descriptive statistics are shown in table 9 and the output of the related model is shown in table 10.

Table 9. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the grammaticality and appropriateness ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Table 10. Output of the models using the grammaticality measure as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

For the grammaticality ratings, the probit link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE and their two-way interaction (NUMBER×FORCE), as well as intercepts for items with slopes for NUMBER and FORCE fit the data best. The results showed a significant main effect of NUMBER in that MC was rated as less grammatical than SM conditions (RQ3.1, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -1.49, χ2(1) = 90.44, p < 0.0001). No other main effect or interaction turned out significant.

3.3.4. Register sensitivity: Appropriateness

For the appropriateness ratings, the descriptive statistics are shown in table 9 and the output of the related models is shown in table 11.

Table 11. Output of the models using the appropriateness measure as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

For the appropriateness ratings, the probit link model that included random intercepts for subjects with slopes for NUMBER, FORCE and the two-way interactions NUMBER×FORCE and NUMBER×CONTEXT, along with intercepts for items with slopes for FORCE fit the data best. The results showed a significant NUMBER×FORCE interaction ( $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.13, χ2(1) = 3.95, p < 0.05). No other interactions were significant.

For the sub-analyses, the necessity-model fit best using the probit link function and NUMBER and the two-way interaction NUMBER×CONTEXT as slopes for subject, and NUMBER and CONTEXT slopes for item intercepts. The possibility-model used the probit link function with NUMBER slopes for subject intercepts, along with item intercepts. The results showed that in both necessity (RQ4.1, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.79, χ2(1) = 56.26, p < 0.0001) and possibility (RQ4.2, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.67, χ2(1) = 85.38, p < 0.0001) conditions, MC was rated as less appropriate than SM, indicating a general NUMBER difference. Furthermore, there was CONTEXT effect in the possibility subset in that distant conditions received lower ratings than distant conditions (RQ4.3, $ \hat{\unicode{x03B2}} $ = -0.12, χ2(1) = 9.91, p < 0.002). No other interaction effect turned out significant.

4. Discussion

The current study takes a first step to MC from a register approach by exploring the linguistic and social meaning of modal concord in US English. We extended a previously published design – Experiment 1 investigating the social meaning of MC and SM without situational contexts (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025) – using interlocutor relation to address the following research questions: in section 4.1, (RQ1-linguistic meaning) What is the interpretation of MC constructions? In section 4.2, (RQ2-social meaning) What is the perceived social meaning of MC constructions in? In section 4.3, (RQ3-grammaticality) How are MC constructions perceived in terms of grammaticality? Lastly, in section 4.4, (RQ4-register sensitivity) Are MC constructions sensitive to the register, i.e. situational contexts?

The results of the current study (Experiment 2) are summarized in table 12, which we discuss in the following sections comparing them with the results from Experiment 1.

Table 12. Summary of the results from Experiment 2. The abbreviation ‘p’ stands for possibility and for ‘n’ necessity conditions. MC abbreviates modal concord and SM single modal. The symbol ⋆ indicates the significance of an interaction effect, – marks the lack of a significant effect, the two-way N×C, F×C and three-way interactions showed no significant results. Replicated results from Experiment 1 are in bold font

4.1. Linguistic meaning (RQ1)

To access the linguistic meaning of MC and SM relating to (RQ1), we used the speaker commitment as measure (Liu et al. Reference Liu, Rotter and Giannakidou2021). We found evidence that MC strengthens the statement in necessity (must certainly, RQ1.1) and a weakening effect in possibility conditions (may possibly) in comparison to their SM alternatives (RQ1.2). This is a replication of the finding from Experiment 1 and further supports (H1: MC nece > SM nece; MC poss < SM poss) – co-occurrences of modal adverb and verb in the same flavor do not have a concord interpretation.

Whereas the strengthening effect was predicted by Giannakidou & Mari’s (Reference Giannakidou and Mari2018) ‘modal spread’ analysis, the weakening effect in may possibly was not predicted by either analysis. Going back to the assumptions we made in (3), that (ii) the modal verbs may and might in epistemic readings, and (iii) the modal adverbs possibly and maybe are similar, we provide some tentative answers for the finding: it is possible that may and might in epistemic readings differ in terms of speaker commitment. This is not implausible as may can be coerced to have a stronger reading, e.g. when the speaker uses it to convey a necessity (e.g. This may happen) or obligation (e.g. You may leave now). It is also possible that the modal adverbs possibly and maybe also differ in terms of speaker commitment. Both are empirical questions that we cannot answer with the current study but will leave for future experimentation.

The current study did not test the speaker commitment of SM involving a single adverb (possibly/certainly), which may differ from that of the modal verbs. This also needs to be further addressed in future studies.

4.2. Social meaning (RQ2)

Relating to (R2) and the social meaning of MC and SM, we found mostly converging evidence in line with those of Experiment 1. The most differences compared to Experiment 1 were revealed in the speaker’s social background: there was no replication of modal use effects in socio-economic status, i.e. must certainly/may possibly vs. must/may, or interactions. In Experiment 1, there was a force difference and the association with higher socio-economic status in SM may in comparison to MC may possibly use, which we attribute to the weakening effect influencing the social perception of the speaker. In the education measures, we found MC to be associated with lower levels than SM in Experiment 2 (RQ2.1). This finding is different from the Experiment 1, where there were only differences between possibility and necessity condition. These differences between the studies can be due to the inclusion of the context and show the influence of social hierarchy on the judgments, i.e. the influence of situational functional parameters.

Relating to the persona measures, we found that MC conditions were rated as less friendly (RQ2.9), warm (RQ2.12) and cool (RQ2.15/16) than SM conditions, which replicated results of Experiment 1 and confirm (H2: MC < SM). Moreover, relating to the differences between MC vs. SM in necessity and possibility conditions, we found some replications: formality (RQ2.3) and confidence (RQ2.7) were stronger for necessity MCs (must certainly) compared to SM (must) along with the opposite effect (RQ2.8) where confidence was weaker for possibility MCs (may possibly) compared to SM (may). In addition, there were some general differences between necessity and possibility conditions: compared to possibility, necessity conditions were rated as less friendly (RQ2.10) and warm (RQ2.13), which also mirrors results from Experiment 1. Lastly, as in experiment 1, we did not find differences between MC vs. SM in necessity or possibility conditions in terms of politeness (RQ2.6). These replications may hint at how the context did not further enrich the social meanings. In Experiment 1, no concrete situational context was given, however, participants might have contextualized the statements while rating. In the current experiment, the concrete situational parameter may have not been one that impacts the doubling of modal verb and adverb – indicated by many replications of results. New results were found in both necessity and possibility conditions: MC was rated as less cool than SM (RQ2.15/16). The main effects in socio-economic status and education, and formality from Experiment 1 were missing in Experiment 2. These findings may be influenced by how interlocutor relationships interact with the semantics of the modal expressions. As shown by Glass (Reference Glass2015) for universal force modals, subtle semantic distinctions can give rise to social meanings, particularly depending on whether the speaker is perceived as having authority or access to the relevant domain of knowledge. The details have to be further addressed in future experimental work.

4.3. Grammaticality (RQ3)

Relating to the grammaticality judgments, we found that MC was overall perceived as less grammatical than SM (RQ3.1), which replicates the finding from Experiment 1 and confirms (H3: MC < SM). However, for all eight conditions, the mean ratings were above 4.8 with medians above 5, in this set-up 4 marks undecided and the shift from ungrammatical to grammatical. Moreover, the visual inspection of figure 3 shows that the participants’ means are scattered across the entire scale for both necessity (must certainly) and possibility (may possibility) MC, while both SM conditions (must/may) received rather uniform ratings mostly above the value 5. Thus, participants were less certain about the grammaticality of MC than SM with great intra-individual variation.

4.4 Register sensitivity (RQ4) and influence of the context

Relating to (RQ4), we found the following evidence: MC shows lower appropriateness ratings than SM in both modal force conditions (RQ4.1/2), providing evidence for (H4: MC < SM).

Crucially, both MC constructions were rated as less grammatical (RQ3.1) and appropriate (RQ4.1/RQ4.2). Necessity MC (must certainly) was perceived as more formal (RQ2.3) and confident (RQ2.7) than (must), while possibility MC (may possibly) received no formality difference but its use indicated lower confidence levels than SM (may) (RQ2.8). Moreover, figure 3 shows that the appropriateness ratings of both necessity (must certainly) and possibility (may possibly) MC are more scattered across the plot than SM constructions, but that they are more focused on values above 4 – more so than the grammaticality judgments, which is even more spread out. This represents an interesting mismatch: alternatives can be perceived as rather appropriate but as not entirely grammatical and English speakers have different intuitions about the degrees. This indicates that the measures we used are distinct but inter-related concepts and that crucially, necessity and possibility MC constructions have distinct situational-functional requirements and effects.

Lastly, in terms of the influence of the situational context, which was a major extension to the design of Experiment 2, justifying a register approach. We only found main effects but no interactions, thus, (H5: two-way interaction CONTEXT×NUMBER) has to be rejected. Distant conditions are rated as more formal (RQ2.4/5), but lower in educational level (RQ2.2), less friendly (RQ2.11), warm (RQ2.14) and rebellious (RQ2.17). In addition, we found lower appropriateness ratings for distant than for close conditions but only for possibility modal verbs and adverbs (RQ4.3). While especially the formality differences show that the context manipulation was perceived by the participants, the results may show expectations of language behavior in the social space, e.g. when someone talks to their boss, they are less warm and more formal than when they talk to their sister. But also that less certain statements like those with possibility modals are disfavored in specific social spaces. Additionally, the topic of the sentences – as another situational-functional parameter – could have also an influence on these ratings. All of the critical items were first person singular statements about everyday events, which might be less suitable in situations involving distant social relations.

Overall, these effects of the context indicate that participants were sensitive to the manipulation we introduced, namely, interlocutor relationship. However, this specific situational parameter may not be a primary factor influencing the perception of modal verbs, adverbs and their co-occurrences. Since registers are multi-dimensional, the influence of specific situational factors and interrelated communicative requirements must be considered. Future research should therefore consider additional parameters to gain a more comprehensive understanding of registers in general, as well as the use and co-occurrence of modal verbs and adverbs.

5. Conclusion

The research presented in this article illustrates a register approach to the interpretation and social meaning of a doubling phenomenon – modal concord (MC) – in American English. Our results support that necessity MC increased the speaker commitment ratings, but possibility MC did the opposite, suggesting asymmetries not fully captured by existing theories. While MC was rated as less grammatical and appropriate than the single modal constructions (SM), it was perceived as less friendly, warm and cool – yet also more formal in necessity conditions. These results largely replicate those from our previous study without context manipulation (Liu & Rotter Reference Liu, Rotter, Knowlton, Schwarz and Papafragou2025), while the current experiment revealed limited interaction with context, suggesting that the chosen situational parameter – interlocutor relation – does not impact the perception on modal verbs, adverbs and their co-occurrences. What remains clear is that MC has distinct linguistic and social meanings from that of SM, but the concrete situational-functional parameters driving this variation remain uncertain.

Notably, while MC is not typically marked as a dialectal feature, yet our constructions may possibly and must certainly are still perceived as less grammatical and appropriate than their single modal alternatives may and must. Given the overall – rather high – ratings for the tested constructions (see table 9), we attribute these to distributional differences, that is, the lower frequency of MC than SM. Crucially, necessity MC is also rated as more formal than SM, setting it apart from the doubling phenomena negative concord and double comparatives (cf. Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter and Liu2024, Reference Rotter, Liu, van der Auwera and Gianollo2025; Alexiadou et al. Reference Alexiadou, Oikonomou and Rotter2025). Moreover, similar to findings on negative concord (Rotter & Liu Reference Rotter, Liu, van der Auwera and Gianollo2025), our results indicate that the interpretation of concord constructions differs from that of their single element alternatives, challenging the core assumption of semantic equivalence underlying the concord analysis. This contrast highlights a unique perceptual profile for MC and underscores how doubling and concord constructions more broadly offer rich ground for investigating register and social meaning.

Before concluding, we briefly address the study’s limitations. First, we did not compare modal adverbs in isolation with MC or modal verb constructions – an important next step for clarifying their respective semantic contributions. Additionally, future research should examine a broader range of modal verb–adverb combinations to capture the full scope of variability and interpretation (see, e.g., Glass Reference Glass2015). Second, context was manipulated solely via interlocutor relation, oversimplifying the multidimensional nature of conversations. Future work should incorporate more contextual parameters. Additionally, while we included several social meaning measures, only some (e.g. formality, confidence) were strongly theory-driven, highlighting the need for more principled selection. Despite these limitations, our study offers a first step toward exploring MC as a doubling phenomenon and as a case of modality from a register perspective, with implications for both semantic theory and experimental methodology.

Data repository

The data sets for this study can be found in the online repository: https://osf.io/v6sjt/

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) – SFB 1412, 416591334. We would like to thank the organizers and audience of the Workshop on ‘Variation, contact, and modal constructions in English 2022’ at the Université Paris Cité and on ‘Social meaning Berlin 2023’ at the Leibniz-Zentrum Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft (ZAS) for their helpful discussions and comments. We are also grateful to the anonymous reviewers for their critical but constructive feedback on previous versions of the article.

Author contribution

S.R. and M.L. designed the study together. S.R. conducted the experiments and the data analyses; both authors were responsible for the interpretation of the data. S.R. prepared the first draft of the manuscript, with revisions from M.L. M.L. provided funding, project administration and resources.

Footnotes

1 For conformity, we adjust the level names to close for the original label public and distant for private.

2 In the study, the term Interpretation was used to address what is called speaker commitment in the current study (Experiment 2); see section 3.2. For conformity with Experiment 2, we use speaker commitment in Experiment 1.

3 Hereafter, factors are capitalized.

References

Agha, Asif. 2007. Language and social relations. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Alexiadou, Artemis, Oikonomou, Despina & Rotter, Stephanie. 2025. Comparing comparatives: Appropriateness ratings of synthetic, analytic and double comparatives in American and British English. English Language and Linguistics. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1360674324000546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbiers, Sjef, Koeneman, Olaf, Lekakou, Marika & van der Ham, Margreet. 2008. Microvariation in syntactic doubling – An introduction. In Barbiers, Sjef, Koeneman, Olaf, Lekakou, Marika & van der Ham, Margreet (eds.), Microvariation in syntactic doubling (Syntax and Semantics 36), 134. Leiden: Brill.10.1163/9781848550216CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, Douglas, Kliegl, Reinhold, Vasishth, Shravan & Baayen, Harald. 2018. Parsimonious mixed models. arXiv 1506.Google Scholar
Beltrama, Andrea. 2020. Social meaning in semantics and pragmatics. Language and Linguistics Compass 14(9), 120.10.1111/lnc3.12398CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn. 2010. The sociolinguistic variant as a carrier of social meaning. Language Variation and Change 22(3), 423–41.10.1017/S0954394510000177CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Rune Haubo Bojesen. 2015. Analysis of ordinal data with cumulative link models – estimation with the R-package ordinal. https://people.vcu.edu/˜dbandyop/ BIOS625/CLM_R.pdfGoogle Scholar
Christensen, Tanya Karoli & Jensen, Torben Juel. 2022. When variants lack semantic equivalence: Adverbial subclause word order. In Christensen, Tanya Karoli & Jensen, Torben Juel (eds.), Explanations in sociosyntactic variation, 171206. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108674942.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Creber, Clare & Giles, Howard. 1983. Social context and language attitudes: The role of formality-informality of the setting. Language Sciences 5(2), 155–61.10.1016/S0388-0001(83)80020-5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dekker, Paul & Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2012. Concord and doubling phenomena: An introduction. Journal of Semantics 29(3), 295303.10.1093/jos/ffs004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dines, Elizabeth R. 1980. Variation in discourse – ‘and stuff like that’. Language in Society 9(1), 1331.10.1017/S0047404500007764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2019. The limits of meaning: Social indexicality, variation, and the cline of interiority. Language 95(4), 751–76.10.1353/lan.2019.0072CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope. 2024. Social meaning. Theoretical Linguistics 50(1–2), 6370.10.1515/tl-2024-2004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eckert, Penelope & Labov, William. 2017. Phonetics, phonology and social meaning. Journal of Sociolinguistics 21(4), 467–96.10.1111/josl.12244CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geurts, Bart & Huitink, Janneke. 2006. Modal concord. In Dekker, Paul & Zeijlstra, Hedde (eds.), Proceedings of the ESSLLI 2006 workshop concord phenomena at the syntax semantics interface, 1520. Malaga: University of Malaga.Google Scholar
Giannakidou, Anastasia & Mari, Alda. 2018. The semantic roots of positive polarity: Epistemic modal verbs and adverbs in English, Greek and Italian. Linguistics and Philosophy 41, 623–64.10.1007/s10988-018-9235-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glass, Lelia. 2015. Strong necessity modals: Four socio-pragmatic corpus studies. Selected Papers from New Ways of Analyzing Variation (NWAV) 43, vol. 21, 2nd edn. https://repository.upenn.edu/handle/20.500.14332/45055 (accessed 18 December 2023).Google Scholar
González-Díaz, Victorina. 2008. On normative grammarians and the double marking of degree. In van Ostade, Ingrid Tieken-Boon (ed.), Grammars, grammarians and grammar-writing in eighteenth-century England, 289310. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.10.1515/9783110199185.4.289CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall-Lew, Lauren, Moore, Emma & Podesva, Robert J.. 2021. Social meaning and linguistic variation: Theoretical foundations. In Hall-Lew, Lauren, Moore, Emma & Podesva, Robert J. (eds.), Social meaning and linguistic variation, 124. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/9781108578684CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halliday, Michael A. K. 1970. Functional diversity in language as seen from a consideration of mood and modality in English. Foundations of Language 6(3), 322–61.Google Scholar
Huitink, Janneke. 2012. Modal concord: A case study of Dutch. Journal of Semantics 29(3), 403–37.10.1093/jos/ffr012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William. 1972. Sociolinguistic patterns. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press.Google Scholar
Lavandera, Beatriz R. 1978. Where does the sociolinguistic variable stop? Language in Society 7(2), 171–82.10.1017/S0047404500005510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, Torrin M. & Kruschke, John K.. 2018. Analyzing ordinal data with metric models: What could possibly go wrong? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 79, 328–48.10.1016/j.jesp.2018.08.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liu, Mingya & Rotter, Stephanie. 2023. Modal concord in American and British English: A register-based experimental study. In Fagen, Lucas, Quain, Sam Gray, Reyes, Stephanie & Tang, Irene (eds.), Proceedings of the fifty-eighth annual meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society (CLS58), 277–88. Chicago: University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Liu, Mingya & Rotter, Stephanie. 2025. Linguistic and social meaning match: An experiment on modal concord in English. In Knowlton, Tyler, Schwarz, Florian & Papafragou, Anna (eds.), Proceedings of ELM 3, vol. 3, 224–35. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. https://doi.org/10.3765/elm.3.5817 (accessed 14 May 2025).Google Scholar
Liu, Mingya, Rotter, Stephanie & Giannakidou, Anasatasia. 2021. Bias and modality in conditionals: Experimental evidence and theoretical implications. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 50, 1369–99.10.1007/s10936-021-09813-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdeling, Anke, Szucsich, Luka, Zeige, Lars Erik, Adli, Aria, Alexiadou, Artemis, Belz, Malte, Bouzouita, Miriam, Bunk, Oliver, Dreyer, Malte, Egg, Markus, Feulner, Anna Helene, Fleischer, Jörg, Gagarina, Natalia, Hirsch, Aron, Jannedy, Stefanie, Knoeferle, Pia, Krause, Thomas, Kutscher, Silvia, Liu, Mingya, Lütke, Beate, Machicao, Antonio y Priemer, Katja Maquate, Hernandez, Laura Merino, Meyer, Roland, Mooshammer, Christine, Müller, Stefan, Sauerland, Uli, Sauermann, Antje, Schmitt, Viola, Schumacher, Nicole, Serova, Dina, Solt, Stephanie, Klok, Jozina Vander, Verhoeven, Elisabeth, Waltereit, Richard, Weirich, Melanie & Wiese, Heike. 2024. Register: Language users’ knowledge of situational-functional variation. Register Aspects of Language in Situation (REALIS) 3, 153.Google Scholar
Lyons, John. 1977. Semantics, vol. 2. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mishoe, Margaret & Montgomery, Michael. 1994. The pragmatics of multiple modal variation in North and South Carolina. American Speech 69(1), 329. www.jstor.org/stable/455947 10.2307/455947CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pescuma, Valentina N., Serova, Diana, Lukassek, Julia, Sauermann, Antje, Schäfer, Roland, Adli, Aria, Bildhauer, Felix, Egg, Markus, Hülk, Kristina, Ito, Aine, Jannedy, Stefanie, Kordoni, Valia, Kühnast, Milena, Kutscher, Silvia, Lange, Robert, Lehmann, Nico, Liu, Mingya, Lütke, Beate, Maquate, Katja, Mooshammer, Christine, Mortezapour, Vahid, Müller, Stefan, Norde, Muriel, Pankratz, Elizabeth, Patarroyo, Angela G., Plesca, Ana-Maria, Rodríguez-Ronderos, Camilo, Rotter, Stephanie, Sauerland, Uli, Schnelle, Gohar, Schulte, Britta, Schüppenhauer, Gediminas, Sell, Bianca Maria, Solt, Stephanie, Terada, Megumi, Tsiapou, Dimitra, Verhoeven, Elisabeth, Weirich, Melanie, Wiese, Heike, Zaruba, Kathy, Zeige, Lars Erik, Lüdeling, Anke & Knoeferle, Pia. 2023. Situating language register across the ages, languages, modalities, and cultural aspects: Evidence from complementary methods. Frontiers in Psychology 13, 131.10.3389/fpsyg.2022.964658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pichler, Heike. 2010. Methods in discourse variation analysis: Reflections on the way forward. Journal of Sociolinguistics 14(5), 581608. https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/abs/10.1111/j.1467-9841.2010.00455.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pullum, Geoffrey K. 1999. African American vernacular English is not standard English with mistakes. In Wheeler, Rebecca S. (ed.), The workings of language: From prescriptions to perspectives, 3958. Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
R Core Team. 2024. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.Google Scholar
Rotter, Stephanie & Liu, Mingya. 2023. Interlocutor relation predicts the formality of the conversation. Register Aspects of Language in Situation (REALIS) 2(2), 127.Google Scholar
Rotter, Stephanie & Liu, Mingya. 2024. A register approach to negative concord versus negative polarity items in English. Linguistics 63(3), 129.Google Scholar
Rotter, Stephanie & Liu, Mingya. 2025. Less formal and more rebellious – an experiment on the social meaning of negative concord in American English. In van der Auwera, Johan & Gianollo, Chiara (eds.), Negative concord: A hundred years on, 302–32. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Slepian, Michael L., Ferber, Simon N., Gold, Joshua M. & Rutchick, Abraham M.. 2015. The cognitive consequences of formal clothing. Social Psychological and Personality Science 6(6), 661–68.10.1177/1948550615579462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt & Kortmann, Bernd. 2004. Global synopsis: Morphological and syntactic variation in English. In Kortmann, Bernd & Schneider, Edgar W. (eds.), A multimedia reference tool, vol. 2: Morphology and syntax, 11421202. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A. 2006. The linguistic variable. In Analysing sociolinguistic variation (Key Topics in Sociolinguistics), chap. 5, 7098. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511801624.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zehr, Jérémy & Schwarz, Florian. 2018. Penncontroller for internet based experiments (IBEX). https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/MD832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeijlstra, Hedde. 2007. Modal concord. Proceedings of Semantics and Linguistic Theory 17, 317–32.10.3765/salt.v17i0.2961CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Summary of the significant results from Experiment 1 (Liu & Rotter 2025: 9, table 5). The abbreviation ‘poss’ stands for possibility and ‘ness’ for necessity conditions. MC stands for modal concord and SM for single modal. 2-int abbreviates the two-way interaction FORCE×NUMBER. The symbol < indicates that the left entity is smaller than the right one; > represents the reverse. The symbol ‘–’ indicates the lack of a significant effect

Figure 1

Figure 1. By-subject means (transparent dots) of the rating measures in comparison to overall means (opaque dots with error bars) of Experiment 1 (Liu & Rotter 2025: 5, figure 1). Panel A depicts the ratings of the speaker commitment (sp commitment) and grammaticality, panel B the social background measures, and panel C the persona measures. The x-axis indicates the specific measures. The top scale shows the ratings from the necessity conditions (nece), the one below those of the possibility conditions (poss). The colors indicate the factor NUMBER with MC (modal concord) in blue and SM (single modal) in yellow. The y-axis depicts the ratings on a seven-point Likert scale. SES abbreviates socioeconomic status.

Figure 2

Figure 2. Screenshots of the experimental design. Panel A shows the fixation cross. Panel B shows the presentation of (S1), and panel C the presentation of (S2). Panel D shows (S2) together with the randomized Likert scale ratings.

Figure 3

Figure 3. By-subject means (transparent dots) of the rating measures in comparison to overall means (squares). Panel A depicts the ratings of the speaker commitment (Sp. commitment), grammaticality and appropriateness, panel B the social background measures, and panel C the persona measures. The x-axis indicates the CONTEXT factor. The top scale shows the ratings from the necessity conditions (nece), the one below the possibility conditions (poss). The colors indicate the factor NUMBER: MC (i.e. modal concord) in blue and SM (i.e. single modal) in yellow. The y-axis depicts the ratings on a seven-point Likert scale. SES abbreviates socioeconomic status.

Figure 4

Table 2. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the speaker commitment ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Figure 5

Table 3. Output of the models using the speaker commitment measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Figure 6

Table 4. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the social background ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Figure 7

Table 5. Output of the models using the social background measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Figure 8

Table 6. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the persona ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Figure 9

Table 7. Output of the models using the first part of the persona measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Figure 10

Table 8. Output of the models using the second part of the persona measures as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Figure 11

Table 9. Descriptive statistics (n = 918) of the grammaticality and appropriateness ratings. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Figure 12

Table 10. Output of the models using the grammaticality measure as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results

Figure 13

Table 11. Output of the models using the appropriateness measure as dependent variable. The symbol ‘⋆’ indicates significant results. Nece abbreviates necessity and poss possibility conditions

Figure 14

Table 12. Summary of the results from Experiment 2. The abbreviation ‘p’ stands for possibility and for ‘n’ necessity conditions. MC abbreviates modal concord and SM single modal. The symbol ⋆ indicates the significance of an interaction effect, – marks the lack of a significant effect, the two-way N×C, F×C and three-way interactions showed no significant results. Replicated results from Experiment 1 are in bold font