Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-9b74x Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-22T14:22:39.551Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cultural impacts on shared decision-making: A cross-European study of psychiatrist preferences in 38 countries

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2025

Yasuhiro Kotera*
Affiliation:
School of Health Sciences, https://ror.org/01ee9ar58University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK Center for Infectious Disease Education and Research, Osaka University, Suita, Japan Department of Social Sciences, Azerbaijan University, Baku, Azerbaijan
Christopher Newby
Affiliation:
School of Medicine, University of Nottingham, Queen’s Medical Centre, Nottingham, UK
Martina Rojnic Kuzman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Zagreb University Hospital Centre, Zagreb, Croatia
Philip Gorwood
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry and Neuroscience of Paris (IPNP), Université Paris Cité, Paris, France GHU-Paris Psychiatrie et Neurosciences, Hôpital Sainte Anne, Paris, France
Andrea Fiorillo
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, University of Campania “L. Vanvitelli”, Napoli, Italy
Mike Slade
Affiliation:
School of Health Sciences, https://ror.org/01ee9ar58University of Nottingham, Nottingham, UK Faculty of Nursing and Health Sciences, Health and Community Participation Division, Nord University, Namsos, Norway
*
Corresponding author: Yasuhiro Kotera; Email: yasuhiro.kotera@nottingham.ac.uk

Abstract

Background

Shared decision-making (SDM) is a collaborative process between clinicians and service users to select treatment, guided by evidence and service user preferences. SDM has clinical, economic, and ethical benefits compared to clinician-led decision-making; yet, implementation remains challenging. An important knowledge gap is the influence of culture on implementation. Cultural impacts on service user decision-making preferences have been documented, but little is known about how culture impacts clinician preferences. This study examined associations between country-level cultural characteristics and decision-making preferences of psychiatrists in routine care settings across Europe.

Methods

We analysed data from 751 psychiatrists and trainees in 38 European countries, who completed the Clinical Decision-Making Style–Staff (CDMS-S) scale. Country-level Hofstede cultural dimensions were linked to CDMS-S scores using univariate and multivariate regression models. Mixed-effects models were used to account for country-level clustering and controlling for professional and economic variables.

Results

In univariate analyses, all six dimensions were associated with SDM preferences. However, only three remained significant in mixed-effects models. Higher levels of Indulgence and Individualism were associated with stronger preferences for SDM, while higher Power Distance was associated with more clinician-led decision-making. These associations did not remain significant in fully adjusted multivariate models, suggesting professional and systematic factors mediate cultural influences.

Conclusions

Indulgence, Individualism, and Power Distance are associated with psychiatrists’ decision-making preferences across Europe. Culturally sensitive SDM interventions should address not only clinician attitudes but also healthcare structures and patient expectations. Findings offer an empirical foundation for tailoring SDM training and policy to diverse cultural contexts within European psychiatry.

Information

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of European Psychiatric Association

Introduction

Clinical shared decision-making (SDM) occurs in most encounters between clinicians and service users. Involvement can range from clinician-led decision-making, through SDM in which decisions are jointly made, to active participation in which the service user leads the decision-making process. SDM is “a process in which clinicians and patients work together to select tests, treatments, management or support packages, based on clinical evidence and the patient’s informed preferences” [Reference Coulter and Collins1]. Clinicians in this context include different professional groups of clinicians, including psychiatrists. SDM preferences reflect the desired level of active service user participation in the decision-making process, which can vary significantly across psychiatrists and cultures.2 Patients, more commonly now described as service users, include their informal carers [Reference Slade2]. SDM empowers service users to engage with the care they receive, helping them make informed choices aligned with their values and preferences. The human rights of service users can be more protected in SDM, and increasingly, service users prefer SDM rather than clinicians making decisions for them [Reference Slade2]. SDM involves service users as experts in their own experiences, requiring their active involvement in treatment [Reference Coulter and Collins1]. Higher service user involvement is associated with multiple positive outcomes, including improved efficiency and quality of health services, enhanced quality of life and satisfaction in service users, and reduced hospital admissions [Reference Bombard3, Reference Coronado-Vázquez, Canet-Fajas, Delgado-Marroquín, Magallón-Botaya, Romero-Martín and Gómez-Salgado4]. SDM is advantageous clinically, economically, and ethically.

SDM research is increasing: 229 publications in 2009 compared with 1,199 in 2018 [Reference Waddell, Lennox, Spassova and Bragge5]. SDM research in mental health has been growing at a similar pace: 4 publications in 2003 compared with 33 in 2015 [Reference James and Quirk6]. A systematic review of 14 randomised controlled trials for service users with mood disorders identified that all SDM interventions improved depression or medication adherence [Reference Samalin, Genty, Boyer, Lopez-Castroman, Abbar and Llorca7]. A systematic review on SDM for adolescent mental healthcare reported positive effects, including treatment satisfaction, better mental health understanding, and reduced depression symptoms [Reference Viksveen8]. A meta-analysis for digital interventions supporting SDM found a significant effect on patient engagement, symptom reduction, and decision process [Reference Vitger, Korsbek, Austin, Petersen, Nordentoft and Hjorthøj9]. Weaknesses of SDM research have also been identified, including poor study quality [Reference Hayes, Edbrooke-Childs, Town, Wolpert and Midgley10] arising in part from poor implementation of SDM interventions [Reference Couët11].

One reason for poor implementation of SDM is an underdeveloped understanding of cultural impacts [Reference López, Kaplan, Nápoles, Hwang, Livaudais and Karliner12]. Culture is understood as “the sum of attitudes, customs and beliefs that distinguishes one group of people from another” [Reference Hawley and Morris13]. There are cultural challenges in SDM: SDM is derived from highly individualistic cultural values; therefore, it can cause harm if used by or for people oriented to other cultural values [Reference Blumenthal-Barby14]. What is considered important in SDM varies significantly across cultures. As an example, service users from a White background often value the belief that doing everything possible is crucial, while those from an Asian background tend to prioritise avoiding worry in SDM [Reference Hawley and Morris13]. Service users in minority cultural groups often experience systemic barriers that reduce trust in healthcare systems, which can impact communication with healthcare professionals and impede SDM [Reference Lin15]. Moreover, SDM expectations can vary by culture. Service users in some minority cultural groups may feel more comfortable with a less active role in SDM compared to those in the dominant cultural group [Reference Gordon, Street, Sharf and Souchek16]. Expectations about other factors, such as involvement of family members, comfortable speed of decision-making, degrees of loss aversion (sensitivity to possible loss), and spread of the alternatives (justifying their decision by perceiving their own choice positively and the other unchosen choices negatively), can also differ by cultures [Reference Yates and de Oliveira17].

The impact of culture on psychiatrist SDM preferences is under-researched. A systematic review identified 24 components of SDM; however, almost all components focus on understanding service users, such as “Patient preferences,” “Tailor information,” and “Learn about the patient” [Reference Hanna, Fania, Anne and Arwen18]. Only one component, “Healthcare professional preferences,” is related to psychiatrists [Reference Hanna, Fania, Anne and Arwen18]. To understand and address cultural impacts on SDM implementation, it is essential to have a better understanding of how culture impacts psychiatrists’ SDM style [Reference Shiraev and Levy19]. Our previous study found regional variations in psychiatrists’ preferences for SDM across Europe [Reference Kuzman20]. Psychiatrists in Northern and Western Europe showed stronger preferences for active service user participation in SDM compared to their counterparts in other European regions. However, beyond these broad regional categorisations, the impact of culture on psychiatrist SDM preferences remains unexplored.

The importance of understanding cultural impact on psychiatrist SDM preferences has been increasingly recognised. In an SDM process, psychiatrists collaboratively communicate with service users and other staff. Psychiatrists today work with staff and service users from different cultural backgrounds more frequently than before, facilitated by the increasing migrant populations and integration of social and health services, both of which are salient in Europe [Reference Cox21]. A lack of shared approaches to mental illness among staff and service users from diverse cultural backgrounds can impede SDM [Reference Elwyn22]. Additionally, more and more psychiatry trainees are considering moving to another country. Two-thirds of psychiatrist trainees in Europe (n = 2,281 from 33 countries) have ever considered leaving the country in which they currently live, and over a quarter of them have taken practical steps towards leaving [Reference da Costa23]. These findings indicate an increasing need for understanding cultural impact on psychiatrists’ SDM preferences.

Hofstede’s cultural dimension theory is the most established quantitative framework in cross-cultural research, with more than 46,000 citations [Reference Gerlach and Eriksson24]. The cultural dimension theory proposes six dimensions (Table 1).

Table 1. Six dimensions in the cultural dimension theory

Cultural impacts become even more important given the trend towards global mobility. The setting of this study was in Europe, which is a multi-cultural region: as of 1 January 2021, 24 million people from non-European Union (EU) countries were living in one of the EU countries, accounting for 5.3% of the EU population. Additionally, 14 million people were living in one of the EU countries with the citizenship of another EU country [25]. These figures are on an increasing trend, for example, an 18% increase for non-EU citizens moving to an EU country, and a 17% increase for EU citizens moving to another EU country, comparing 2020 with 2021 [25]. Non-EU countries, such as the United Kingdom and Switzerland, also have high rates of residents who were born outside the countries (UK 14% and Switzerland 30%) [26]. Moreover, the quality and process of psychiatric practice, and the socio-economic status are relatively similar across countries [Reference Turnpenny, Petri, Finn, Beadle-brown and Nyman27]. These aspects support this cross-cultural research to be conducted in this area.

Study aim

This study aimed to examine the association between Hofstede’s six cultural dimensions and psychiatrists’ preferences for active service user participation in SDM across Europe.

Methods

A cross-sectional study using the data collected in the European Psychiatric Association (EPA) Ambassadors Survey and Hofstede’s metrics. The EPA data were reported to show the preference for SDM among psychiatrists in the three regions [Reference Kuzman20]. The Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines were followed (Supplementary Material 1).

Procedures

The EPA invited psychiatrists across Europe who were part of the EPA community to participate, including individual EPA members, members of its affiliated associations, and the ~5,000 attendees of the last 10 EPA congresses. In 2020, this group was invited to become EPA Ambassadors and to participate in EPA surveys. An invitation email was sent to EPA Ambassadors, the Council of National Psychiatric Associations, the EPA Board, and the EPA Sections, with a request to further distribute the invitation among their associated members. Responses were gathered from April to December 2021 via an online questionnaire. The study was open to all mental health professionals (psychiatrists, psychiatry trainees, psychologists, social workers, and nurses) working in Europe. Only responses from psychiatrists were included in the current study. The authors confirm that all procedures followed in this research align with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation, as well as the 1975 Helsinki Declaration, revised in 2008 and 2013. The study was approved by the Ethical Committee of the Zagreb University Hospital Centre (Ref. 02/013-JG).

Measures

The outcome variable was SDM preference, assessed using the Clinical Decision-Making Style–Staff (CDMS-S) questionnaire [Reference Puschner28], which measures “Participation in decision-making” through two subscales (Sections A and B), with all items rated on 5-point Likert scales. Section A consists of six items (rated from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree”) that reflect general preferences for decision-making in routine mental health services. Section B includes nine items (rated from “service user” to “me”) that measure specific preferences for decision-making based on three clinical vignettes (focusing on work, medication side effects, and general medication use). Items 1, 2, 3, and 5 are reverse-scored. The “Participation in decision-making” subscale is calculated as the prorated mean of all items in Sections A and B, provided at least 12 out of the 15 items have been completed. The score ranges from 0 to 4, with higher scores indicating a greater preference by the clinician for active involvement of service users in decision-making.

The predictor variables were collected in two sets. The first set was cultural dimensions, obtained from Hofstede (https://geerthofstede.com/research-and-vsm/vsm-2013/). Hofstede’s dataset provides scores for the six cultural dimensions of 111 countries. The data were collected using the Value Survey Module 2013, a 24-item self-report measure, responded on a 5-point Likert scale from 1 to 5 [Reference Hofstede and Minkov29]. Each dimension score is calculated using index formulas in the manual. The scores are presented from 0 (low compared to other countries) to 100 [Reference Hofstede and Minkov29]. Data for all 38 countries are appended (Supplementary Material 2). The second set was collected in the EPA survey, comprising socio-demographics (age, sex, and city size) and professional characteristics: expertise, training, and practice (time since becoming consultant psychiatrists, subspecialty, job position, type of practice, clinical setting, and duration of appointments).

Two confounder variables were included in the fully adjusted analyses, chosen for their relevance to Hofstede’s metrics and mental health care resources [Reference Kotera30]. The percentage of gross domestic product (GDP) spent on health represents the proportion of a country’s economy allocated to healthcare, calculated by dividing total health expenditure by GDP (https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SH.XPD.CHEX.GD.ZS). The Gini coefficient, sourced from the World Bank (https://data.worldbank.org/indicator/SI.POV.GINI), measures income inequality within a country, ranging from 0 (indicating perfect equality) to 1 (representing maximum inequality).

Statistical methodology

The survey data [Reference Kuzman20] was linked by country with the country-specific Hofstede metrics, and all analyses were carried out using the R language package. The outcome variable used was the CDMS-S score.

First, we performed univariate complete-case weighted regression analyses (Model 1), applying the same country weights as used in our previous study [Reference Kuzman20]. In parallel, we ran a univariate mixed-effects regression model (Model 2) to account for clustering of cultural dimensions within countries, again using the complete-case dataset.

Second, we conducted multivariate analyses for both Models 1 and 2, incorporating the cultural dimensions and EPA variables from the final model in Kuzman et al. [Reference Kuzman20]. These variables included European region, sex, age, city size, profession type, practice type, patient type, specialised condition, specific specialist conditions, provision of psychotherapy, time since obtaining specialist psychiatric qualification, and frequency of appointments. In addition, we included two confounding variables: the Gini coefficient and GDP expenditure on health.

Third, we addressed missing data through multiple imputation using the MICE package in R. Cultural dimensions, EPA variables, and the two confounders (Gini coefficient and GDP on health) were used as predictors in the imputation model. We generated 50 imputed datasets and applied both univariate and multivariate versions of Models 1 and 2 to each. Final estimates were pooled using Rubin’s rules.

We report β-coefficients, along with 95% confidence intervals (CIs) and corresponding p-values.

Results

Participant information

The online survey was completed by 919 participants across 38 European countries. After excluding 27 (3.0%) psychologists, 14 (1.5%) other mental health professionals, and 10 (1.1%) participants with unspecified professions, the final sample comprised 738 (81.2%) psychiatrists and 130 (14.3%) psychiatry trainees, totalling 868 participants from the target population. Complete data on all 15 CDMS-S items were missing for 112 out of 868 (12.9%) respondents. Additionally, five participants with incomplete data on 14 CDMS-S items (n = 2), 13 items (n = 1), and 10 items (n = 2) were excluded. This resulted in a final sample of 751 (86.5%) out of 868 participants for analysis. In the final sample, 322 (42.9%) participants were from Central and Eastern Europe, 273 (36.4%) from Northern and Western Europe, and 156 (20.8%) from Southern Europe (Table 2; the countries categorised in each region are listed in Supplementary Material 2).

Table 2. Participant characteristics

Abbreviation: IQR, interquartile range.

Note: Data in this table have been published in Table 1 in Kuzman et al. [Reference Kuzman20].

Note: Data were missing for sex in 4 (0.5%), age in 1 (0.1%), city size in 3 (0.4%), practice in 3 (0.4%), patients in 4 (0.5%), subspecialty in 1 (0.1%), time since specialist psychiatry qualification in 10 (1.3%), cost of visit in 271 (36.1%), and average frequency of clinical appointments with each patient in 12 (1.6%) participants.

Culture and SDM

Univariate analysis results for complete case analysis using Models 1 and 2, and multiple imputation estimates for Model 1, can be found in Table 3. It was not possible to pool the multiple imputed datasets for Model 2 for univariate or multivariate results due to the clustered nature of the data. All of the cultural dimension variables were significant in the univariate weighted linear regression (Model 1) for both complete and multiple imputed datasets. However, when adjusted for the EPA variables and covariates (Gini coefficient and GDP on health), none of the cultural dimension variables were significant, suggesting that the more specific variables within each country predicted the CDMS-S score better than the more general country cultural dimensions, which is to be expected.

Table 3. Predictors of SDM

Note: Significant values (p < 0.05) are bold. Ββ-coefficient; CI, confidence interval.

The weighted linear regression model, however, did not capture the within- and between-country cluster variation. By using a univariate mixed-effect model (Model 2), we adjusted for country as a random effect. In this model for the complete case dataset, we found that the cultural dimension variables Indulgence (β = 0.005, 95% CI = 0.002 to 0.009, p = 0.005) and Individualism (β = 0.007, 95% CI = 0.003 to 0.011, p = 0.003) were significantly positively associated with CDMS-S score. Power Distance (β = −0.005, CI = −0.008 to −0.003, p = 0.001) was significantly negatively associated with CDMS-S scores. Although statistically significant, the effect sizes were modest, with standardised β-coefficients of 0.19, 0.24, and 0.25 for Indulgence, Individualism, and Power Distance, respectively. All cultural dimension variables were not significant after adjusting for the EPA variables and confounding variables in a multivariate complete case mixed-effect model.

In sum, all six cultural dimensions were associated with SDM in univariate models. However, these associations disappeared in multivariate models that included professional and system-level variables (EPA, Gini, and GDP on health). Mixed-effects modelling, which accounted for country-level clustering, showed that higher Indulgence and Individualism and lower Power Distance were linked to greater SDM.

Discussion

This cross-cultural study investigated the association between national cultural dimensions and shared SDM among psychiatrists and psychiatry trainees across 38 European countries. Using Hofstede’s cultural framework, we examined how six cultural dimensions are related to clinicians’ preference for active involvement from patients in SDM, as measured by CDMS-S. Our analyses revealed significant associations in univariate models, with all six dimensions showing a relationship with SDM scores. However, these associations did not persist in multivariate models that included clinician-level variables and health system indicators. Notably, in mixed-effects modelling accounting for country-level clustering, Indulgence and Individualism were positively associated with SDM, while Power Distance showed a negative association.

The finding that all six cultural dimensions were associated with SDM in univariate models suggests that broad national cultural characteristics play a role in shaping clinicians’ general orientation toward patient involvement. This aligns with prior literature suggesting that macro-cultural values influence health professionals’ communication styles, attitudes towards hierarchy, and views on patient autonomy [Reference Kotera30, Reference Goyal31]. For instance, in cultures with high Individualism, autonomy and personal agency are more strongly emphasised [Reference Kotera32], which may encourage clinicians to pay close attention to each individual’s needs in clinical decision-making. Conversely, high Power Distance cultures may reinforce hierarchical relationships [Reference Kotera33] that discourages shared approaches to care.

However, the disappearance of these associations in multivariate models implies that cultural values alone are insufficient to explain SDM practices once more immediate contextual variables, such as professional characteristics (EPA) and economic indicators (Gini and GDP on health), are accounted for. This highlights the importance of distinguishing between distal cultural norms (macro-level influences) and proximal systemic or organisational factors (micro-level influences) [Reference Kotera34]. The findings suggest that while national culture sets a backdrop for attitudes and expectations, the actual implementation of SDM may also need to consider professional education, institutional policies, and healthcare infrastructure.

The mixed-effects model further refined our understanding by accounting for country-level clustering. Here, three cultural dimensions remained significant: Indulgence, Individualism, and Power Distance. These findings are consistent with expectations and previous work.

Indulgence, which reflects a cultural orientation towards enjoyment, freedom, and self-expression, was also associated with stronger SDM preferences. Conversely, its opposite, Restraint, was linked to lower preferences for SDM. In Restraint cultures, social norms emphasise self-discipline and the suppression of gratification, which may lead to more conservative, clinician-led approaches to decision-making and limit the perceived legitimacy of patient preferences in clinical interactions [Reference Kotera35]. Countries higher in Indulgence may promote clinical environments where patients’ subjective experiences and personal values are welcomed and legitimised [Reference Yasuhiro36]. This orientation could foster more open, empathetic, and participatory consultation styles, aligning well with the principles of SDM.

High Individualism was associated with greater SDM, suggesting that clinicians in countries where autonomy and self-determination are emphasised are more likely to involve patients in decision-making processes. The most Individualistic countries in our sample, based on Hofstede, were the United Kingdom, Hungary, and the Netherlands. This finding aligns with previous research that associates individualistic values with patient-centred communication and greater emphasis on equality in professional relationships [Reference Narapaka, Singh, Ramesh and Dhingra37]. In contrast, countries with lower levels of Individualism, such as Serbia, Portugal, and Slovenia, may place more value on relational or collective processes, which can also support SDM but in different culturally situated ways. These patterns highlight the need to interpret SDM preferences through the lens of local cultural norms, rather than assuming a universal model tied exclusively to Western ideals.

Higher Power Distance was associated with more clinician-led decision-making. Slovakia, Serbia, and Russia were the highest Power Distance participating countries, while Austria, Denmark, and Ireland were the lowest. High Power Distance countries are characterised by hierarchical structures where authority figures, including psychiatrists, are expected to lead and direct decisions [Reference Kotera, Aledeh, Barnes, Rushforth, Adam and Riswani38]. In these contexts, SDM might be seen as challenging established norms of authority and trust in the expertise of clinicians. Conversely, psychiatrists working in low Power Distance cultures (e.g., Austria) may perceive SDM as aligning with broader societal values of democracy, fairness, and patient autonomy [Reference Cheng39]. These findings underscore the nuanced ways cultural dimensions shape professional attitudes and clinical practices.

Additionally, SDM in high Power Distance settings may be further influenced by patient expectations. In hierarchical cultures, patients might prefer a more directive approach, reinforcing the clinician-led decision-making norm [Reference Miyatake40]. This contrasts with low Power Distance contexts, where patients may actively expect to collaborate and share responsibility in decision-making. Such differences highlight how both clinician and patient perspectives, shaped by cultural norms, contribute to the enactment of SDM. This adds depth to our previous study findings [Reference Kuzman20] based on geographical categories, demonstrating the value of incorporating cultural dimensions like Power Distance into models of decision-making in psychiatry. Together, these observations highlight how Power Distance impacts both psychiatrists’ decision-making preferences and the cultural alignment of SDM with societal expectations.

The lack of significance in the multivariate mixed-effects model again reinforces the notion that cultural values may exert an indirect rather than direct influence on clinical practice. This is in line with theoretical models that position culture as a distal factor, operating through more immediate social and structural mechanisms [Reference Betancourt, Green, Carrillo and Ananeh-Firempong41]. Our findings also suggest that interventions aimed at enhancing SDM should not rely solely on cultural awareness but must also address local structural conditions, including training, workload, and institutional norms.

From a policy perspective, this study provides useful insights for international efforts to promote SDM in psychiatry. While cultural competence remains important, it must be complemented by context-sensitive strategies that recognise the influence of systemic variables [Reference Sa and Ferreira42]. For instance, countries with high Power Distance might benefit from institutional reforms that empower patients and restructure clinician–patient hierarchies, alongside cultural education. Similarly, in countries with lower Indulgence scores, promoting patient-centredness may require efforts to legitimise the expression of personal preferences in clinical encounters. These results also have implications for training and education. Our findings support the idea that while cultural context influences attitudes towards SDM, these attitudes are modifiable and interact with professional development. Embedding SDM principles into psychiatric training across Europe, tailored to local cultural and systemic contexts, may help bridge the gap between global best practices and local realities [Reference Bhugra43].

Several limitations should be noted. First, while Hofstede’s framework provides a useful heuristic, it may oversimplify the complexity of national cultures and does not account for intracultural variation. Second, the cross-sectional design limits causal inference. Third, while we included both psychiatrists and trainees, responses were self-reported and may reflect social desirability bias. Fourth, although our models included economic indicators, other factors such as legal frameworks or specific national mental health policies were not captured. Lastly, we were unable to pool imputed data for mixed-effects models due to technical constraints, which limited the robustness of those estimates. Despite these limitations, this study makes a novel and timely contribution to understanding how cultural and systemic factors jointly shape SDM in psychiatry. It represents one of the first large-scale, empirical investigations to apply a validated cross-cultural framework to SDM practices in mental health across Europe. The combination of national cultural indicators with clinician-level and structural variables provides a unique, multilevel perspective that bridges gaps between global cultural theory and real-world clinical behaviour. These findings reinforce the importance of contextualising SDM promotion efforts not only within professional education and policy reform but also within the socio-cultural realities of different regions.

In conclusion, while national cultural dimensions are associated with SDM attitudes among psychiatrists in Europe, their influence appears to be mediated or moderated by professional and systemic factors. Mixed-effects analysis identified three key cultural dimensions – Individualism, Indulgence, and Power Distance – as significant predictors, but these associations weakened when adjusting for clinical attitudes and economic conditions. Efforts to promote SDM should, therefore, adopt a multilevel approach that incorporates cultural sensitivity alongside institutional and educational reform. Future research should explore these interactions further, ideally through longitudinal and mixed-method designs that can unpack the pathways linking culture, structure, and clinical behaviour.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at http://doi.org/10.1192/j.eurpsy.2025.10082.

Data availability statement

The data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Lara Grosso, an EPA employee, for the efficient and kind technical support of the study. We are grateful for the contributions of the EPA Ambassadors Study Group, which supported data collection.

Author contribution

YK, PG, and MS contributed to the conception and design of the study. MRK contributed to the acquisition of data. CN conducted the data analysis. YK contributed to the data interpretation. YK wrote the initial draft, and all authors contributed to the critical review of the manuscript and approved the final version. CN accessed and verified the data. All authors were able to access the raw data and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.

Financial support

This research received no grant funding from any external agency, commercial, or not-for-profit sector, but was financially supported by the European Psychiatry Association. MS acknowledges the support of the NIHR Nottingham Biomedical Research Centre.

Competing interests

The authors declare none.

References

Coulter, A, Collins, A. Making shared decision-making a reality. 621. London: King’s Fund; 2011.Google Scholar
Slade, M. Implementing shared decision making in routine mental health care. World Psychiatry. 2017;16(2):146–53. doi:10.1002/wps.20412.Google Scholar
Bombard, Y et al. Engaging patients to improve quality of care: A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2018;13(1):98. doi:10.1186/s13012-018-0784-z.Google Scholar
Coronado-Vázquez, V, Canet-Fajas, C, Delgado-Marroquín, MT, Magallón-Botaya, R, Romero-Martín, M, Gómez-Salgado, J. Interventions to facilitate shared decision-making using decision aids with patients in primary health care: A systematic review. Medicine (Baltimore). 2020;99(32):e21389. doi:10.1097/md.0000000000021389.Google Scholar
Waddell, A, Lennox, A, Spassova, G, Bragge, P. Barriers and facilitators to shared decision-making in hospitals from policy to practice: A systematic review. Implement Sci. 2021;16(1):74. doi:10.1186/s13012-021-01142-y.Google Scholar
James, K, Quirk, A. The rationale for shared decision making in mental health care: A systematic review of academic discourse. Ment Health Rev J. 2017;22(3):152–65. doi:10.1108/MHRJ-01-2017-0009.Google Scholar
Samalin, L, Genty, J-B, Boyer, L, Lopez-Castroman, J, Abbar, M, Llorca, P-M. Shared decision-making: A systematic review focusing on mood disorders. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2018;20(4):23 doi:10.1007/s11920-018-0892-0.Google Scholar
Viksveen, P et al. User involvement in adolescents’ mental healthcare: A systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2022;31(11):1765–88. doi:10.1007/s00787-021-01818-2.Google Scholar
Vitger, T, Korsbek, L, Austin, SF, Petersen, L, Nordentoft, M, Hjorthøj, C. Digital shared decision-making interventions in mental healthcare: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psych. 2021;12. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2021.691251.Google Scholar
Hayes, D, Edbrooke-Childs, J, Town, R, Wolpert, M, Midgley, N. A systematic review of shared decision making interventions in child and youth mental health: Synthesising the use of theory, intervention functions, and behaviour change techniques. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023;32(2):209–22. doi:10.1007/s00787-021-01782-x.Google Scholar
Couët, N et al. Assessments of the extent to which health-care providers involve patients in decision making: A systematic review of studies using the OPTION instrument. Health Expect. 2015;18(4):542–61.Google Scholar
López, ME, Kaplan, CP, Nápoles, AM, Hwang, ES, Livaudais, JC, Karliner, LS. Satisfaction with treatment decision-making and treatment regret among Latinas and non-Latina whites with DCIS. Patient Educ Couns. 2014;94(1):83–9.Google Scholar
Hawley, ST, Morris, AM. Cultural challenges to engaging patients in shared decision making. Patient Educ Couns. 2017;100(1):1824. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2016.07.008.Google Scholar
Blumenthal-Barby, J et al. Potential unintended consequences of recent shared decision making policy initiatives. Health Aff. 2019;38(11):1876–81. doi:10.1377/hlthaff.2019.00243.Google Scholar
Lin, JJ et al. Cultural factors associated with racial disparities in lung cancer care. Ann Am Thorac Soc. 2014;11(4):489–95.Google Scholar
Gordon, HS, Street, RL Jr, Sharf, BF, Souchek, J. Racial differences in doctors’ information-giving and patients’ participation. Cancer. 2006;107(6):1313–20. doi: 10.1002/cncr.22122.Google Scholar
Yates, JF, de Oliveira, S. Culture and decision making. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 2016;136:106–18. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.05.003.Google Scholar
Hanna, B-R, Fania, RG, Anne, MS, Arwen, HP. Key components of shared decision making models: A systematic review. BMJ Open. 2019;9(12):e031763. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2019-031763.Google Scholar
Shiraev, EB, Levy, DA. Cross-cultural psychology. 8th ed. London: Routledge; 2024, p. 546.Google Scholar
Kuzman, MR et al. Clinical decision-making style preferences of European psychiatrists: Results from the ambassadors survey in 38 countries. Eur Psychiatry. 2022;65(1):e75. doi:10.1192/j.eurpsy.2022.2330.Google Scholar
Cox, JL. European psychiatry: Moving towards integration and harmony. World Psychiatry. 2007;6(1):54–6.Google Scholar
Elwyn, G et al. Shared decision making: A model for clinical practice. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27(10):1361–7. doi:10.1007/s11606-012-2077-6.Google Scholar
da Costa, MP et al. To which countries do European psychiatric trainees want to move to and why? Eur Psychiatry. 2017;45:174–81. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.06.010.Google Scholar
Gerlach, P, Eriksson, K. Measuring cultural dimensions: External validity and internal consistency of Hofstede’s VSM 2013 scales. Front Psychol. 2021;12: doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2021.662604.Google Scholar
Eurostat. Non-EU citizens make up 5.3% of the EU population. Eurostat. Available from https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/products-eurostat-news/-/ddn-20220330-2 (Accessed 9 June 2023).Google Scholar
European Commission. Overall figures of immigrants in European society. European Commission. Available from https://commission.europa.eu/strategy-and-policy/priorities-2019-2024/promoting-our-european-way-life/statistics-migration-europe_en (Accessed 9 June 2023).Google Scholar
Turnpenny, A, Petri, G, Finn, A, Beadle-brown, J, Nyman, M. Mapping and Understanding Exclusion: Institutional, coercive and community-based services and practices across Europe. Project report. Mental Health Europe, Brussels, Belgium, 2017. doi: 10.22024/UniKent/01.02/64970.Google Scholar
Puschner, B et al. Clinical decision making and outcome in the routine care of people with severe mental illness across Europe (CEDAR). Epidemiol Psychiatr Sci. 2016;25(1):6979. doi:10.1017/S204579601400078X.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G, Minkov, M. Values survey module 2013 manual. Geert Hofstede BV; 2013. Available from https://geerthofstede.com/wp-content/uploads/2016/07/Manual-VSM-2013.pdf (Accessed 25 January 2023).Google Scholar
Kotera, Y et al. 28-country global study on associations between cultural characteristics and recovery college fidelity. npj Mental Health Res. 2024;3(1):46 doi:10.1038/s44184-024-00092-9.Google Scholar
Goyal, S et al. Analyzing the relationship between cultural identity and health professional perspectives. Evol Stud Imaginative Cult. 2024;8(2):880–94. doi:10.70082/esiculture.vi.1086.Google Scholar
Kotera, Y et al. Cross-cultural comparison of recovery college implementation between Japan and England: Corpus-based discourse analysis. Int J Ment Heal Addict. 2024; doi:10.1007/s11469-024-01356-3.Google Scholar
Kotera, Y et al. Mental wellbeing of Indonesian students: Mean comparison with UK students and relationships with self-compassion and academic engagement. Healthcare. 2022;10(8):1439.Google Scholar
Kotera, Y et al. Cross-cultural insights from two global mental health studies: Self-enhancement and Ingroup biases. Int J Ment Heal Addict. 2024; doi:10.1007/s11469-024-01307-y.Google Scholar
Kotera, Y et al. Development of the Japanese version of the original and short work extrinsic and intrinsic motivation scale. Jpn Psychol Res. 2024; doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jpr.12551.Google Scholar
Yasuhiro, K et al. Cultural influences on fidelity components in recovery colleges: A study across 28 countries and territories. General Psychiatry. 2025;38(3):e102010. doi:10.1136/gpsych-2024-102010.Google Scholar
Narapaka, P, Singh, M, Ramesh, M, Dhingra, S. Patient-reported involvement in shared decision-making during medical consultations: A systematic literature review. J Public Health. 2023; doi:10.1007/s10389-023-02153-3.Google Scholar
Kotera, Y, Aledeh, M, Barnes, K, Rushforth, A, Adam, H, Riswani, R. Academic motivation of Indonesian university students: Relationship with self-compassion and resilience. Healthcare. 10(10). doi:10.3390/healthcare10102092.Google Scholar
Cheng, S. The positive impacts on employees and corporates’ benefits of the egalitarian corporation culture. Interdiscip Human Commun Stud. 2024;1:13. doi:10.61173/02fg7w46.Google Scholar
Miyatake, H et al. Case report on the legal assurance of advance care planning in collective culture. Clin Case Rep. 2022;10(4):e05759. doi:10.1002/ccr3.5759.Google Scholar
Betancourt, JR, Green, AR, Carrillo, JE, Ananeh-Firempong, O. Defining cultural competence: A practical framework for addressing racial/ethnic disparities in health and health care. Public Health Rep. 2003;118(4): 293302. doi:10.1093/phr/118.4.293.Google Scholar
Sa, M, Ferreira, J. The importance of recognition, validation and certification of competences system in promoting mental health. Eur Psychiatry. 2011;26(S2):1127–7. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(11)72832-2.Google Scholar
Bhugra, D. Excellence in training – What makes a good psychiatrist? Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30(S1):1. doi:10.1016/S0924-9338(15)30001-8.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Table 1. Six dimensions in the cultural dimension theory

Figure 1

Table 2. Participant characteristics

Figure 2

Table 3. Predictors of SDM

Supplementary material: File

Kotera et al. supplementary material

Kotera et al. supplementary material
Download Kotera et al. supplementary material(File)
File 38.8 KB
Submit a response

Comments

No Comments have been published for this article.