Hostname: page-component-cb9f654ff-65tv2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-08-27T19:06:10.087Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Placebo and nocebo phenomena in schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a narrative review on current knowledge and potential future directions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 July 2025

James A. Waltz*
Affiliation:
Maryland Psychiatric Research Center (MPRC), Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/055yg0521 University of Maryland School of Medicine , Baltimore, MD, USA
Sherry D. Pujji
Affiliation:
Maryland Psychiatric Research Center (MPRC), Department of Psychiatry, https://ror.org/055yg0521 University of Maryland School of Medicine , Baltimore, MD, USA
Luana Colloca
Affiliation:
Department of Pain and Translational Symptom Science, Placebo Beyond Opinions (PBO) Center, https://ror.org/04rq5mt64 University of Maryland School of Nursing , Baltimore, MD, USA
*
Corresponding author: James A. Waltz; Email: jwaltz@som.umaryland.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

The experience of psychosis in schizophrenia spectrum disorders involves significant distress and functional impairment, contributing to immense social and economic costs. Current gold standard treatment guidelines emphasize the use of antipsychotic medications, though responses to these treatments vary widely, with the potential for detrimental side effects. However, increasing placebo responses in randomized controlled trials since the 1960s complicate the development of new medications. Elevated placebo responses are common in psychiatric populations, including those with psychosis, and are influenced by individual beliefs and prior experiences. Despite extensive research on placebo mechanisms in conditions such as depression and pain, little is known about mechanisms of these effects in psychosis. This narrative review examines the predictors and belief formation processes underlying placebo and nocebo phenomena in psychosis. We discuss features of randomized controlled trials for antipsychotic medications, individual symptom heterogeneity, and contextual factors. Findings related to placebo effects for motivation and cognition-enhancing drugs are also discussed. We then consider the possibility that theories of predictive coding and aberrant salience provide explanation for aspects of both placebo effects and schizophrenia spectrum symptoms. The role of outcome expectations broadly and in the context of reward processing is considered. We conclude with some recommendations for future placebo research in psychosis, emphasizing the diversity of placebo effects, assessment concerns, cultural considerations, and methodological aspects. Future multidisciplinary research is required to further elucidate placebo effects in schizophrenia spectrum disorders.

Information

Type
Review Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

The experience of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (SSDs) involves symptoms and impairments associated with enormous social and financial costs (Ali, Santomauro, Ferrari, & Charlson, Reference Ali, Santomauro, Ferrari and Charlson2023; American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2022; Kadakia et al., Reference Kadakia, Catillon, Fan, Williams, Marden, Anderson, Kirson and Dembek2022; Oh, Waldman, Stickley, DeVylder, & Koyanagi, Reference Oh, Waldman, Stickley, DeVylder and Koyanagi2019; Stubbs et al., Reference Stubbs, Koyanagi, Veronese, Vancampfort, Solmi, Gaughran, Carvalho, Lally, Mitchell, Mugisha and Correll2016). While current psychosis treatment guidelines often prioritize the use of antipsychotic medications, the literature on treatment responses is mixed. There appears to be evidence for both broad symptom improvements (McCutcheon et al., Reference McCutcheon, Pillinger, Mizuno, Montgomery, Pandian, Vano, Marques and Howes2021) and variations of antipsychotic medication responsiveness (Case et al., Reference Case, Stauffer, Ascher-Svanum, Conley, Kapur, Kane, Kollack-Walker, Jacob and Kinon2011; Stroup, Reference Stroup2007). Furthermore, antipsychotic medications also often have detrimental side effects, including movement disorders and weight gain (Ceraso et al., Reference Ceraso, Lin, Schneider-Thoma, Siafis, Tardy, Komossa, Heres, Kissling, Davis and Leucht2020; Doane et al., Reference Doane, Sajatovic, Weiden, O’Sullivan, Maher, Bjorner, Sikora Kessler, Carpenter-Conlin, Bessonova and Velligan2020). Importantly – in addition to multitude of other treatment development challenges, such as the presence of immense heterogeneity in psychosis symptom profiles (APA, 2022; Tsuang & Faraone, Reference Tsuang and Faraone1995) – the use and development of medications to manage psychosis has been complicated by trends pointing toward increasing placebo responses and decreasing responses to active treatments in randomized controlled trials (RCTs; Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen, & McGuire, Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023; Leucht et al., Reference Leucht, Leucht, Huhn, Chaimani, Mavridis, Helfer, Samara, Rabaioli, Bächer, Cipriani, Geddes, Salanti and Davis2017). The reasons for increasing placebo responses – apparent improvements on the relevant outcome measure in the placebo group of a clinical trial – are not fully understood. While much of the magnitudes of placebo responses observed in RCTs with psychiatric medications can be attributed to genuine placebo effects in individuals (defined as advantageous outcomes in response to inactive treatments that can influence an individual’s beliefs beyond that of any treatment effects; Neogi & Colloca, Reference Neogi and Colloca2023), many other factors influence the size of these responses. Furthermore, the degree to which reports of medication side effects reflect nocebo effects (defined as subjective experiences of aversive outcomes in response to inactive treatments) is not known (Atlas, Reference Atlas2023; Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023). There is some evidence to suggest that the perception of adverse events, consequent to nocebo effects, is related to elevated levels of psychiatric symptoms and may contribute to treatment noncompliance and attrition, in the context of RCTs (Blasini et al., Reference Blasini, Corsi, Klinger and Colloca2017; Wartolowska, Reference Wartolowska2019). In short, while treatment responses clearly exceed placebo responses in many clinical trials (Taylor, Smith, Gee, & Nielsen, Reference Taylor, Smith, Gee and Nielsen2012), the last six decades have seen a reduction in differences between drug and placebo response effects (Agid et al., Reference Agid, Siu, Potkin, Kapur, Watsky, Vanderburg, Zipursky and Remington2013; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023; Leucht et al., Reference Leucht, Leucht, Huhn, Chaimani, Mavridis, Helfer, Samara, Rabaioli, Bächer, Cipriani, Geddes, Salanti and Davis2017).

While much of the literature focuses on profound placebo effects under depressive and pain conditions (Atlas, Reference Atlas2023; Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023; Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013), placebo effects appear to vary considerably within and across psychiatric disorders and drug classes (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021; Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). While placebo effect estimates for antipsychotic medications range anywhere from 20 to 70% (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021; Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013), those estimates pale in comparison to placebo effect estimates for antidepressant medications, which range from 70 to 90% (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021). However, it is not known whether specific predictors and belief processes that maintain placebo and nocebo effects in psychosis are the same in other populations. In a recent review, Hird et al. (Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023) have suggested that neurocognitive mechanisms, formalized in theories of predictive coding, may provide a useful framework to understand the unique qualities of placebo and nocebo responses, in terms of both the alleviation of psychotic symptoms such as delusions (which are themselves beliefs) and believing that a medicine is actually reducing symptoms (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). If we are to isolate treatment effects in future RCTs for psychiatric medications, there is an urgent need to increase our understanding of the origins of placebo and nocebo responses in patients with psychosis (Alphs, Benedetti, Fleischhacker, & Kane, Reference Alphs, Benedetti, Fleischhacker and Kane2012; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). That is, developing a better understanding of placebo effects and their predictors may lead to improvements in health care.

In this narrative review, we extend the observations, recommendations, and guidance provided by Hird et al. (Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023) by considering additional factors that may modulate placebo effects in psychosis. These include the unique features of antipsychotic medications and RCTs, symptom heterogeneity, and the roles of expectations, insight, reward sensitivity, and other contextual factors. We conclude with specific recommendations for research in psychosis focused on different types of placebo effects, assessment concerns, and cultural considerations.

Predictors of placebo responses in RCTs for antipsychotic medications in people with SSDs

Although the specific predictors of placebo responses in antipsychotic medication trials have yet to be purposefully and clearly examined, a number of potential predictors that may guide RCT design and augment clinical practice have been identified (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). These predictors have been grouped into study-centered factors (number of study sites, trial duration, sample size, industry sponsorship, and number of individuals in the drug versus placebo group) and patient-centered factors (duration of illness, younger age, prior treatment experiences; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023; see Figure 1).

Figure 1. Summary of study- and patient-centered factors influencing placebo effects in SSDs as identified by Hird et al. (Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). This meta-analysis identified several study- and patient-centered factors that may play a role in elevated placebo effects for some individuals with schizophrenia.

In terms of study-centered factors, greater numbers of participants in the placebo arm of a trial (relative to the treatment arm) have contributed to increased drug-placebo differences in both antipsychotic and antidepressant drug trials (Mallinckrodt, Zhang, Prucka, & Millen, Reference Mallinckrodt, Zhang, Prucka and Millen2010). The quality, sponsorship, and location of study sites may also partially contribute to the magnitudes of placebo responses (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). Study sites for RCTs are subject to a variety of regulations, guidance, and pressures (Bhide, Shah, & Acharya, Reference Bhide, Shah and Acharya2018). Of note, trends of increased placebo responses and decreased treatment effects are especially prevalent in North America (Chen, Wang, Khin, Hung, & Laughren, Reference Chen, Wang, Khin, Hung and Laughren2010; Khin, Chen, Yang, Yang, & Laughren, Reference Khin, Chen, Yang, Yang and Laughren2012). Industry sponsorship may also increase pressure to obtain desired results and reduce the rigor of specific study arms. Fraguas et al. (Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019) argued that industry-sponsored sites may recruit ‘professional patients’ who tend to have less severe baseline symptoms (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Pott, Tandler, Wall, Roose and Lieberman2014). Yet, research has also found that purposely recruiting individuals with elevated symptom profiles does not add advantages in quality and may create disadvantages (Welge & Keck, Reference Welge and Keck2003). Nonetheless, industry-sponsored trials tend to have elevated placebo responses, relative to non-industry-sponsored trials (Haddad & Correll, Reference Haddad and Correll2018; Leucht et al., Reference Leucht, Leucht, Huhn, Chaimani, Mavridis, Helfer, Samara, Rabaioli, Bächer, Cipriani, Geddes, Salanti and Davis2017).

With regard to patient-centered factors, there is mixed support for symptom severity being a predictor of the magnitude of placebo responses (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023), as some meta-analyses have found support for stronger placebo effects being related to higher baseline symptoms (Agid et al., Reference Agid, Siu, Potkin, Kapur, Watsky, Vanderburg, Zipursky and Remington2013), and others indicate that stronger placebo effects are associated with milder baseline symptoms (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023; Weimer, Colloca, & Enck, Reference Weimer, Colloca and Enck2015). Furthermore, individuals with higher positive and total SSD symptoms may find the attention received in RCTs or through hospital stabilization to reinforce positive expectancies and thus have greater placebo responses (Agid et al., Reference Agid, Siu, Potkin, Kapur, Watsky, Vanderburg, Zipursky and Remington2013). Individuals recruited to participate in RCTs may also be subject to practices geared toward altering their expectations of study-related outcomes (Rutherford et al., Reference Rutherford, Pott, Tandler, Wall, Roose and Lieberman2014). Specifically, it has been suggested that placebo research may be subject to the ‘halo effect’ where participants may allow their positive views of research-related staff to influence their expectations (Andrade, Reference Andrade2012; Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). Other potential influential factors include the Hawthorne effect (i.e. knowledge of being observed influences observations), Rosenthal effect (i.e. inaccurate perception of improvement over time by raters), and contextual factors such as social support (see Andrade [Reference Andrade2012] for detailed discussion).

Predictors of placebo and nocebo effects in patients with SSDs

In contrast to placebo responses observed in RCTs, placebo effects are mechanistically built on expectations and beliefs regarding the effectiveness of an inactive intervention (Colloca & Barsky, Reference Colloca and Barsky2020; Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023). Expectations (priors), specifically outcome-related expectations, appear to be central to placebo effects across psychiatric conditions (Rief & Petrie, Reference Rief and Petrie2016). Expectations of improvement can be deduced from study-related factors such as an individual believing that they are assigned to an active RCT treatment group because they know there are many treatment arms and likely only one placebo group, which has been shown to mediate placebo responses (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019). The fact that expectations, consequent to conditioning, may wane over time, due to decay or extinction (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023), may partially explain why RCTs of longer duration tend to show smaller placebo effects. Agid et al. (Reference Agid, Siu, Potkin, Kapur, Watsky, Vanderburg, Zipursky and Remington2013) noted that individuals may also be aware of intrinsic factors, such as the presence or absence of extrapyramidal side effects, which may influence their group membership expectations, and subsequently, outcome expectations. Research groups have suggested to try to account for individual differences when designing and recruiting for clinical trials to better account for and address placebo responders in RCTs (Agid et al., Reference Agid, Siu, Potkin, Kapur, Watsky, Vanderburg, Zipursky and Remington2013; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). Of note, antipsychotic RCTs rarely, if ever, directly query participants’ expectations (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019); doing so would contribute greatly to our understanding of placebo responses in psychiatric patients.

The Placebo-Reward Hypothesis posits that expectations of benefit may actually cause improvement in RCTs with medications for Parkinson’s disease (de la Fuente-Fernández, Reference de la Fuente-Fernández2009). That is, the anticipation of successful treatment (i.e., reward) may inherently boost dopamine and thereby mood (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). The expectation of improvement may itself be antidepressant. Recent research has also found that treatment uncertainty and reward-related learning may modulate placebo effects (Augustat, Endres, & Mueller, Reference Augustat, Endres and Mueller2024). Regarding large placebo effects for antidepressant medications, research has emphasized maximizing positive associations through conditioning to increase reward anticipation (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013).

While placebo responses and effects have been linked to psychological and pathophysiological mechanisms (Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023), the neural and psychological origins of nocebo effects are less-well understood (Atlas, Reference Atlas2023; Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023). Recent research outside of SSDs has suggested that a range of biological and neurophysiological factors surrounding adverse experiences influences susceptibility to nocebo effects (Grosso et al., Reference Grosso, Barbiani, Cavalera, Volpato and Pagnini2024). The valences of the expectations (positive/optimistic or negative/pessimistic) are likely to influence whether the effects of inactive compounds are falsely perceived as positive or negative. That is, positive expectations may be linked to placebo effects, while negative expectations may be linked to nocebo effects (Rief & Petrie, Reference Rief and Petrie2016). Coupled with existing priors, sensitivity to reward may bolster positive expectancies associated with placebo effects, as has been observed in placebo analgesia (De Pascalis & Scacchia, Reference De Pascalis and Scacchia2019). Outcome expectations are inherently linked to reward and punishment sensitivity (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). Finally, while not consistently examined in the literature, it may be possible that individuals with SSDs are particularly sensitive to nocebo effects where they might believe that inactive interventions could be causing them harm. This is especially salient when considering symptoms such as specific types of delusional ideation where individuals may be prone to believing that innocuous stimuli or individuals mean them harm (APA, 2022). Additionally, it is plausible that comorbid mood symptoms increase the likelihood that outcome expectations are negative (Palermo et al., Reference Palermo, Giovannelli, Bartoli and Amanzio2019). In any case, much research is needed to further identify the nocebo effects across SSDs and understand their sources.

Individuals with comorbid depressive and psychotic symptoms tend to be less responsive to placebo antidepressants, when compared with individuals who only had depression (Glassman & Roose, Reference Glassman and Roose1981). While this suggests that there may be unique processes associated with psychosis that influence the likelihood of placebo responses (and effects, mechanistically speaking), these processes have not yet been clearly identified. For example, reduced levels of insight into symptoms and diagnoses in SSDs may be one potential differentiator (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021), while differences in outcome expectations may be another (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). However, it is unknown if shared mechanisms that underlie placebo effects are consistent across SSDs and other diagnoses. The ease of identifying reward-related placebo effects in SSDs might be hampered by individuals experiencing symptoms such as delusions as rewarding experiences (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). A better understanding of the importance and value of beliefs and expectations to the individual, as they relate to reward prediction and reward processing, may greatly contribute to placebo response research in SSDs (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). Individuals with SSDs may have impairments in multiple aspects of belief formation, including prediction error signaling in reward and value-based decision-making (Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Voss, Schlagenhauf and Heinz2019) that may generate differential susceptibility to placebo and nocebo effects.

Predictive coding theory accounts of placebo effects

Given that placebo effects are mechanistically built on expectations and beliefs (Colloca & Barsky, Reference Colloca and Barsky2020; Colloca et al., Reference Colloca, Noel, Franklin and Seneviratne2023), understanding the origins of placebo effects requires the consideration of theories of belief formation. One of the most detailed theories of belief formation, at the present time, is predictive coding (PC) theory, the central tenets of which are tied closely to Bayesian theory (Büchel et al., Reference Büchel, Geuter, Sprenger and Eippert2014; Kaptchuk, Hemond, & Miller, Reference Kaptchuk, Hemond and Miller2020). Broadly, PC predicting coding frameworks are built on the idea that perception involves a combination of bottom-up and top-down processing, and consequently, perception can be strongly influenced by expectations (Büchel, Geuter, Sprenger, & Eippert, Reference Büchel, Geuter, Sprenger and Eippert2014; Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Adams, Fletcher, Frith, Lawrie, Muckli, Petrovic, Uhlhaas, Voss and Corlett2018). That is, PC theory posits that top-down prior beliefs and experiences (priors), coupled with bottom-up sensory information (likelihoods), lead individuals to make inferences about how to navigate their environment (Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Adams, Fletcher, Frith, Lawrie, Muckli, Petrovic, Uhlhaas, Voss and Corlett2018). Neurocomputationally, the brain combines priors and likelihoods into posteriors, or an interpretation of current experiences (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). Any mismatches between priors and likelihoods result in prediction errors, which may subsequently contribute to changes in one’s internal model of priors to reduce prediction-error-related dissonance (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). Predictive coding theory suggests that discrepancies in the weighting of priors and likelihoods are responsible for both SSD symptoms, such as delusions, and placebo effects, perhaps highlighting similarities between the two constructs (Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Adams, Fletcher, Frith, Lawrie, Muckli, Petrovic, Uhlhaas, Voss and Corlett2018; Sterzer, Voss, Schlagenhauf, & Heinz, Reference Sterzer, Voss, Schlagenhauf and Heinz2019). Computational modeling analyses also suggest that the lower- and higher-order beliefs and expectations inherent in delusions may generate somewhat analogous placebo effects to those of the opioid network when considering pain (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023).

Several aspects of psychosis may emerge from a reduced ability to integrate bottom-up and top-down signals, including auditory hallucinations, delusions, reinforcement learning impairments, and an altered sense of agency (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023; Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Adams, Fletcher, Frith, Lawrie, Muckli, Petrovic, Uhlhaas, Voss and Corlett2018). Computational PC frameworks may further assist in understanding processes critical to learning and inference, such as the perception of volatility, precision/uncertainty, and the amplification of prediction error signals through learning rates (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). There is a large body of work that links hallucinations to overly strong prior beliefs (Corlett et al., Reference Corlett, Horga, Fletcher, Alderson-Day, Schmack and Powers2019), whereas delusions have been associated with the presence of imprecise priors that may or may not be strong in nature (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). For example, delusions have been associated with overly strong priors (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Konova, Daw and Horga2019) and more broadly, overly strong priors might also influence beliefs about treatment effects and lead to placebo or nocebo effects (Grosso et al., Reference Grosso, Barbiani, Cavalera, Volpato and Pagnini2024; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). Conversely, the presence of weaker low-level priors and increased emphasis on higher-level priors may also contribute to delusions and placebo responses, as individuals may be attempting to detect patterns to form expectations (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). The valence of beliefs about treatment effects might further be influenced by a subject’s degree of paranoia, which has been linked (through PC theory) to beliefs about environmental volatility (Sheffield et al., Reference Sheffield, Suthaharan, Leptourgos and Corlett2022).

Predictions and certainty appear to influence expectations at different levels, thus lending itself to specific symptom profiles that may be more susceptible to placebo and nocebo effects. For example, delusional beliefs have been explained in terms of an increased weighting of priors (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023; Sterzer et al., Reference Sterzer, Adams, Fletcher, Frith, Lawrie, Muckli, Petrovic, Uhlhaas, Voss and Corlett2018), which may potentially signal susceptibility to placebo effects. Delusional beliefs have also been explained in terms of stronger priors related to higher-level beliefs, which compensate for weaker, ambiguity-driven priors at lower levels (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). This suggests that in the case of delusions, beliefs about environmental contingencies may be more imprecise, while beliefs about environmental volatility may be more rigid. Preliminary evidence in one experimental study supports the notion that placebo-like beliefs tend to rely on stronger priors than new information in the context of sensory processing (Baker, Gamer, Rauh, & Brassen, Reference Baker, Gamer, Rauh and Brassen2022). On the other hand, some researchers argue that delusionality emerges from a lack of strong priors – that is, priors that are overly flexible, plastic, and updatable, such that they are easily overridden through new experiences. Some (Haarsma et al., Reference Haarsma, Knolle, Griffin, Taverne, Mada, Goodyer, Fletcher and Murray2020; Powers et al., Reference Powers, Angelos, Bond, Farina, Fredericks, Gandhi, Greenwald, Hernandez-Busot, Hosein, Kelley, Mourgues, Palmer, Rodriguez-Sanchez, Seabury, Toribio, Vin, Weleff, Woods and Benrimoh2025) have postulated that weak priors are characteristic of earlier phases of psychosis, including psychosis-risk syndromes, when beliefs are malleable, allowing new delusions to emerge (a state sometimes called ‘delusional mood’). Furthermore, certain theoretical accounts suggest that hallucinations are linked to greater weighting of sensory evidence and lower emphasis on prior beliefs (reduced prior-to-likelihood ratio; Weilnhammer et al., Reference Weilnhammer, Röd, Eckert, Stuke, Heinz and Sterzer2020).

Per PC theory, paranoia can also be explained in terms of strongly weighted priors and weaker likelihoods that theoretically result in individuals taking more time to integrate new information and develop beliefs related to placebo and nocebo responses (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023). Paranoia is also thought to relate closely to expectations of volatility, which have a strong influence on learning rates and the updating of priors, especially in emotionally salient experiences (Sheffield et al., Reference Sheffield, Suthaharan, Leptourgos and Corlett2022). Thus, an individual experiencing these or similar traits may consistently overestimate environmental volatility in part due to worry and anxiety stemming from the stimuli (Sheffield et al., Reference Sheffield, Suthaharan, Leptourgos and Corlett2022). With regard to antipsychotic medications, an individual with paranoid cognitions may be less likely to have positive expectations about medication or placebo, which may produce a nocebo effect and, subsequently, treatment noncompliance. Over time, however, the weight of likelihoods may slowly increase if the individual experiences benefits from the medication. As learning mechanisms work to reduce prediction errors, placebo beliefs may become further integrated into an individual’s model of the world (Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023).

Aberrant salience attribution and placebo effects

Another critical aspect of belief formation thought to play a role in delusion formation is salience attribution. According to the aberrant salience hypothesis, individuals with psychosis inadvertently assign undue value or importance to seemingly mundane stimuli or experiences (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). In essence, this hypothesis considers how aberrant learning and conditioning contribute to salience attribution, laying the foundation for the development of delusions or other odd beliefs. As aberrant salience influences where an individual places a focus or an emphasis on incoming stimuli, this process may be representative of an attempt to detect a signal in noisy input. In the absence of strong priors, this can lead to maladaptive salience attribution and the formation of delusional beliefs, as individuals are attempting to fit perceptions and events to specific prior beliefs (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023). The aberrant salience hypothesis is grounded in anomalous learning, resulting in mundane stimuli and events being labeled as biologically significant (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003). These aberrant attributions of salience to experience can, through conditioning, influence expectations and contribute to the development of odd or delusional beliefs (Petrovic & Sterzer, Reference Petrovic and Sterzer2023) and by extension, placebo and nocebo effects. In other words, experiences of aberrant salience influence expectations and learning experiences (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003; Waltz & Gold, Reference Waltz and Gold2016). Critically, the concept of salience attribution is closely tied to ideas regarding the functional roles of dopamine systems in the brain, with excessive/erratic dopamine system activity either causing or resulting from maladaptive attributions of salience (Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagali, Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015). Different SSD symptoms (positive versus negative) may be associated with different abnormalities in salience attribution (excessive versus insufficient), brought on by different disruptions of dopamine system function (hyperactivity versus hypoactivity; Whitton, Treadway, & Pizzagalli, Reference Whitton, Treadway and Pizzagalli2015). According to the aberrant salience hypothesis, antipsychotic medications are ‘antipsychotic’ because they reduce the salience attributed to mundane or insignificant stimuli and thereby attenuate positive symptoms (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003).

Placebo responses in RCTs for adjunctive medication for SSDs

Treatments for symptoms that are grounded in beliefs and expectations may be more subject to placebo effects than treatments for negative (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019) and cognitive symptoms (Keefe et al., Reference Keefe, Davis, Harvey, Atkins, Haig, Hagino, Marder, Hilt and Umbricht2017). In fact, Czobor et al. (Reference Czobor, Kakuszi and Bitter2022) have suggested that negative symptom placebo effect estimates are smaller than previously reported, due to the stability and lack of variability of negative symptoms over time. This may also be highlighted in Keefe et al.’s (Reference Keefe, Davis, Harvey, Atkins, Haig, Hagino, Marder, Hilt and Umbricht2017) findings of no strong placebo effects for cognition in those with schizophrenia. The adaptive attribution of salience is also critical to the refinement of goal-directed behavior, as motivational deficits are thought to arise from a blunted ability to properly assign incentive value to stimuli and events (Kapur, Reference Kapur2003; Waltz & Gold, Reference Waltz and Gold2016).

The influence of placebo responses in SSDs has also been examined in the context of clinical trials for medications designed to enhance motivation and cognition, though not all have used placebo groups. Findings from these studies generally indicate that placebo responses in such trials are considerably smaller than they are in trials of antipsychotic medications. In a meta-analysis of 12 randomized placebo-controlled RCT studies, involving 813 patients with schizophrenia, Keefe et al. (Reference Keefe, Davis, Harvey, Atkins, Haig, Hagino, Marder, Hilt and Umbricht2017) found that, in clinical trials for candidate compounds for cognition enhancement, placebo responses were very small, suggesting that placebo effects for cognition in schizophrenia are virtually nonexistent. Clinical trials of the add-on use of arginine vasopressin for learning and memory in SSDs revealed treatment responses that exceed placebo responses (Geng et al., Reference Geng, Wang, Yang, Wang, Ma, Liu and Wang2017). The use of modafinil for cognition appears to have neither a strong placebo nor treatment effect in people with SSDs (Kumar, Reference Kumar2008), although Strzelecki et al. (Reference Strzelecki, Kałużyńska, Szyburska and Wysokiński2016) observed improvements in cognition in patients with SSD administered modafinil, but not in the placebo group. Regarding social functioning in patients with schizophrenia, both placebo and treatment responses were observed in an RCT for paliperidone ER (Patrick et al., Reference Patrick, Burns, Morosini, Gagnon, Rothman and Adriaenssen2010). Finally, placebo effects associated with the use of various other adjunctive or repurposed medications remain to be clearly elucidated (Cao et al., Reference Cao, Liu, Selvitella, Librenza-Garcia, Passos, Sawalha, Ballester, Chen, Dong, Wang, Kapczinski, Dursun, Li, Greiner and Greenshaw2021; Correll et al., Reference Correll, Solmi, Cortese, Fava, Højlund, Kraemer, McIntyre, Pine, Schneider and Kane2023).

Future directions

Because the placebo effects of antipsychotic and other medications in SSDs have rarely been examined in a mechanistic manner, their underlying physiology remains unknown. Based on the theories, concepts, and gaps in the literature relevant to understanding the placebo effect in SSDs, identified by us and others, there are several possible future directions that the field may take (Table 1).

Table 1. Future directions for placebo research in SSDs

When examining placebo responses in clinical trials involving individuals with psychosis, it is essential to consider contextual and methodological differences compared to other clinical populations (Atlas, Reference Atlas2023). Expectations shaped by prior learning, beliefs, uncertainty, and reward sensitivity should be assessed in both pharmacological and nonpharmacological RCTs. These variables, alongside insight, symptoms, and cognitive functioning, should be included as covariates to better understand the magnitude of placebo and nocebo effects. However, expectations are often symptom-dependent in psychosis, making it difficult to control for them independent of symptom severity (Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). Disparities related to culture, sex, gender, and socioeconomic status also influence expectations and treatment engagement, potentially leading to underrepresentation of certain groups in placebo research (Cundiff-O’Sullivan et al., Reference Cundiff-O’Sullivan, Oula, Shafir and Colloca2023; Jarvis et al., Reference Jarvis, Iyer, Andermann and Fung2020). Sex/gender differences in SSDs, such as earlier age of onset and more severe negative symptoms in men, further justify examining biological sex as a moderator of placebo effects (Giordano, Bucci, Mucci, Pezzella, & Galderisi, Reference Giordano, Bucci, Mucci, Pezzella and Galderisi2021; Shafir, Olson, & Colloca, Reference Shafir, Olson and Colloca2022). While antipsychotic placebo effects in adults have been explored (Kalaria et al., Reference Kalaria, Zhu, Farchione, Mathis, Gopalakrishnan, Uppoor, Mehta and Younis2019, Reference Kalaria, Farchione, Mathis, Gopalakrishnan, Younis, Uppoor, Mehta, Wang and Zhu2020), there is limited research in children and adolescents.

Applying methods common in non-industry-sponsored trials, such as prioritizing knowledge of participants’ histories, attention to quality and detail, and the reduction of assessment bias, may be possible options to reduce industry pressures in future trials (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019). One recommendation is to create networks of academic centers to match the recruitment and output of industry. For example, there is now a large network of academic centers and sites in Europe that work together to promote, centralize, and coordinate schizophrenia research called the European Group for Research in Schizophrenia (Galderisi, Reference Galderisi2023). The creation of such a network in North America would similarly benefit future research and reduce industry pressures.

One important factor to consider is differences in outcome measures (i.e., subjective measurements of symptom ratings versus objective cognitive performance scores). Although objective markers of positive and negative symptoms are lacking, emerging tools such as digital phenotyping and neuroimaging (e.g. conditioned hallucinations [Powers et al., Reference Powers, Mathys and Corlett2017] and geolocation [Horan et al., Reference Horan, Sachs, Velligan, Davis, Keefe, Khin, Butlen-Ducuing and Harvey2024]) may help to quantify constructs such as paranoia, hallucinations, anhedonia, and avolition. In examining the convergence and divergence of subjective and objective measurements, we may bolster our understanding of placebo responses in people with SSDs. Experimental studies designed specifically for people with SSDs and medications of interest are needed, given that most placebo findings stem from RCTs primarily focused on treatment efficacy (Kinon et al., Reference Kinon and Watson2011; Murray & Stoessl, Reference Murray and Stoessl2013). It remains unclear whether placebo responses for psychiatric symptoms generalize to other conditions, such as pain. For example, predictions regarding placebo analgesia in psychosis may not be straightforward, as certain individuals with schizophrenia experience higher sensitivity to acute pain and lower sensitivity to prolonged and chronic pain (called the ‘Pain Threshold Paradox’; Lévesque et al., Reference Lévesque, Potvin, Marchand, Stip, Grignon, Pierre, Lipp and Goffaux2012; Nagamine, Reference Nagamine2023).

Designs involving the manipulation of expectations may clarify the role of belief valence. Interventions targeting appraisals and expectations have been shown to reduce negative symptoms (Fraguas et al., Reference Fraguas, Díaz-Caneja, Pina-Camacho, Umbricht and Arango2019). Mechanisms such as social learning and verbal suggestion can shape placebo responses, with evidence from pain modulation and emotion regulation studies (e.g., Schenk et al., Reference Schenk, Krimmel and Colloca2017; Shafir, Israel, & Colloca, Reference Shafir, Israel and Colloca2023). Eliciting positive expectations through social interactions has also improved mood in placebo-controlled trials (Baker et al., Reference Baker, Gamer, Rauh and Brassen2022) and brief psychoeducation via a Placebo-Control Reminder Script reduced depressive symptoms in individuals with psychosis or depression diagnoses (Cohen et al., Reference Cohen, Hassman, Ereshefsky, Walling, Grindell, Keefe, Wyka and Horan2021). Theoretical models, such as predictive coding, have been successfully applied to placebo analgesia (Büchel et al., Reference Büchel, Geuter, Sprenger and Eippert2014; Ongaro & Kaptchuk, Reference Ongaro and Kaptchuk2019) but rarely to psychotic or depressive symptoms. Investigating placebo and nocebo effects in SSDs may yield insights into alternative mechanisms for symptom relief, especially considering the side effects of psychiatric medications.

Future research should employ novel designs and analytic methods – including virtual health tools, digital phenotyping, expectation conditioning, and emotion regulation-based paradigms – to isolate placebo mechanisms. Technology such as artificial intelligence-based machine learning algorithms, remote assessments, and virtual reality can be applied to identify characteristics of placebo responders, though future research to confirm proof of concept is needed (Horan et al., Reference Horan, Sachs, Velligan, Davis, Keefe, Khin, Butlen-Ducuing and Harvey2024). Utilizing advanced statistics such as mixed-effects models, stimulations, dropout models, and growth mixture modeling with maximum-likelihood estimations can refine our understanding of placebo-drug differences (Kozielska et al., Reference Kozielska, Pilla Reddy, Johnson, de Ridder, Vermeulen, Liu, Groothuis, Danhof and Proost2013; Reddy et al., Reference Reddy, Kozielska, Johnson, Vermeulen, de Greef, Liu, Groothuis, Danhof and Proost2011, Reference Reddy, Kozielska, Johnson, Suleiman, Vermeulen, Liu, de Greef, Groothuis, Danhof and Proost2012, Reference Reddy, Kozielska, de Greef, Vermeulen and Proost2013). Additionally, nonpharmacological interventions such as psychosis therapies and transcranial magnetic stimulation show promise and warrant further placebo-related investigation (Paillère‐Martinot et al., Reference Paillère‐Martinot, Galinowski, Plaze, Andoh, Bartrés‐Faz, Bellivier, Lefaucheur, Rivière, Gallarda, Martinot and Artiges2017; Turkington et al., Reference Turkington, Spencer, Lebert and Dudley2017), as do repurposed medications with potential placebo effects (Correll et al., Reference Correll, Solmi, Cortese, Fava, Højlund, Kraemer, McIntyre, Pine, Schneider and Kane2023).

Questions and concerns regarding placebo responses in RCTs involving antipsychotic medications

Despite the clinical relevance (Alphs et al., Reference Alphs, Benedetti, Fleischhacker and Kane2012; Hird et al., Reference Hird, Diederen, Leucht, Jensen and McGuire2023), longitudinal trends in antipsychotic placebo effects are rarely evaluated. Evidence suggests that placebo participants tend to relapse over time, reverting to baseline symptom levels (Ceraso et al., Reference Ceraso, Lin, Schneider-Thoma, Siafis, Tardy, Komossa, Heres, Kissling, Davis and Leucht2020; van de Laar et al., Reference van de Laar, Henter, Bartko and Wyatt2001). It is also unknown whether placebo responses in SSDs generalize or vary across treatment types, such as antipsychotic, analgesic, or antihypertensive medications. Given the chronic nature of SSDs and the side effects of antipsychotic medications that contribute to elevated morbidity (Peritogiannis et al., Reference Peritogiannis, Ninou and Samakouri2022), understanding the sustainability of placebo responses in SSDs and their relation to treatments is valuable (Ali et al., Reference Ali, Santomauro, Ferrari and Charlson2023; Ceraso et al., Reference Ceraso, Lin, Schneider-Thoma, Siafis, Tardy, Komossa, Heres, Kissling, Davis and Leucht2020). Ethical concerns are especially salient in placebo trials for psychosis, given the vulnerability present in this population (Melamed et al., Reference Melamed, Doron, Stein-Reisner and Bleich2009). For example, the duration of placebo arms in first-episode psychosis trials should be minimized to avoid extended untreated psychosis (Haddad & Correll, Reference Haddad and Correll2018), although placebo controls may help avoid exposing patients to the adverse effects of ineffective medications (Oliveira et al., Reference Oliveira, Nunes, Coutinho and Sena2009). Ultimately, focusing on theoretical constructs such as predictive coding and aberrant salience provides testable frameworks for advancing placebo research in SSDs.

Funding statement

This work was supported by NIH grants T32 MH067533 (PI: G. Elmer) and R01 MH115031 (PI: J. Waltz).

Competing interests

Dr. Pujji has no conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise.

Dr. Colloca has no conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise.

Dr. Waltz has no conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise.

References

Agid, O., Siu, C. O., Potkin, S. G., Kapur, S., Watsky, E., Vanderburg, D., Zipursky, R. B., & Remington, G. (2013). Meta-regression analysis of placebo response in antipsychotic trials, 1970-2010. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 170(11), 13351344. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12030315CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ali, S., Santomauro, D., Ferrari, A. J., & Charlson, F. (2023). Schizophrenia as a risk factor for cardiovascular and metabolic health outcomes: A comparative risk assessment. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 32, e8. https://doi.org/10.1017/S2045796023000045CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alphs, L., Benedetti, F., Fleischhacker, W. W., & Kane, J. M. (2012). Placebo-related effects in clinical trials in schizophrenia: What is driving this phenomenon and what can be done to minimize it?. The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(7), 10031014. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1461145711001738CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders : DSM-5-TR/American Psychiatric Association (Fifth edition, text revision. ed.). American Psychiatric Association Publishing.Google Scholar
Andrade, C. (2012). There’s more to placebo-related improvement than the placebo effect alone. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73(10), 13221325.10.4088/JCP.12f08124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Atlas, L. Y. (2023). Computational psychiatry and the placebo effect in psychosis. Biological Psychiatry Global Open Science 3(4), 585586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2023.08.006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Augustat, N., Endres, D., & Mueller, E. M. (2024). Uncertainty of treatment efficacy moderates placebo effects on reinforcement learning. Scientific Reports, 14(1), 14421–14417. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-024-64240-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, J., Gamer, M., Rauh, J., & Brassen, S. (2022). Placebo induced expectations of mood enhancement generate a positivity effect in emotional processing. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 53455345. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-09342-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baker, S. C., Konova, A. B., Daw, N. D., & Horga, G. (2019). A distinct inferential mechanism for delusions in schizophreniaBrain: A Journal of Neurology142(6), 17971812. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz051CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bhide, A., Shah, P. S., & Acharya, G. (2018). A simplified guide to randomized controlled trials. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 97(4), 380387. https://doi.org/10.1111/aogs.13309CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasini, M., Corsi, N., Klinger, R., & Colloca, L. (2017). Nocebo and pain: An overview of the psychoneurobiological mechanisms. Pain Reports, 2(2), e585. https://doi.org/10.1097/PR9.0000000000000585CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Büchel, C., Geuter, S., Sprenger, C., & Eippert, F. (2014). Placebo analgesia: A predictive coding perspective. Neuron (Cambridge, Mass.), 81(6), 12231239. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.02.042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, B., Liu, Y. S., Selvitella, A., Librenza-Garcia, D., Passos, I. C., Sawalha, J., Ballester, P., Chen, J., Dong, S., Wang, F., Kapczinski, F., Dursun, S. M., Li, X.-M., Greiner, R., & Greenshaw, A. (2021). Differential power of placebo across major psychiatric disorders: A preliminary meta-analysis and machine learning study. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 21301. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-021-99534-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Case, M., Stauffer, V. L., Ascher-Svanum, H., Conley, R., Kapur, S., Kane, J. M., Kollack-Walker, S., Jacob, J., & Kinon, B. J. (2011). The heterogeneity of antipsychotic response in the treatment of schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 41(6), 12911300. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291710001893CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceraso, A., Lin, J. J., Schneider-Thoma, J., Siafis, S., Tardy, M., Komossa, K., Heres, S., Kissling, W., Davis, J. M., & Leucht, S. (2020). Maintenance treatment with antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, 8(8), CD008016. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD008016.pub3Google ScholarPubMed
Chen, Y.-F., Wang, S.-J., Khin, N. A., Hung, H. M. J., & Laughren, T. P. (2010). Trial design issues and treatment effect modeling in multi-regional schizophrenia trials. Pharmaceutical Statistics: The Journal of the Pharmaceutical Industry, 9(3), 217229. https://doi.org/10.1002/pst.439CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, E. A., Hassman, H. H., Ereshefsky, L., Walling, D. P., Grindell, V. M., Keefe, R. S. E., Wyka, K., & Horan, W. P. (2021). Placebo response mitigation with a participant-focused psychoeducational procedure: A randomized, single-blind, all placebo study in major depressive and psychotic disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology: Official Publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(4), 844850. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-020-00911-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colloca, L., & Barsky, A. J. (2020). Placebo and Nocebo effects. The New England Journal of Medicine, 382, 554561. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMra1907805CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colloca, L., Noel, J., Franklin, P.D., & Seneviratne, C. (2023) Placebo effects through the lens of translational research. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780197645444.001.0001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corlett, P. R., Horga, G., Fletcher, P. C., Alderson-Day, B., Schmack, K., & Powers, A. R., 3rd (2019). Hallucinations and strong priorsTrends in Cognitive Sciences23(2), 114127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.12.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Correll, C. U., Solmi, M., Cortese, S., Fava, M., Højlund, M., Kraemer, H. C., McIntyre, R. S., Pine, D. S., Schneider, L. S., & Kane, J. M. (2023). The future of psychopharmacology: A critical appraisal of ongoing phase 2/3 trials, and of some current trends aiming to de-risk trial programmes of novel agents. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 22, 4874. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.21056CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cundiff-O’Sullivan, R. L., Oula, D., Shafir, R., & Colloca, L. (2023). 38C1.4Cultural influences on placebo and nocebo effects. In Placebo effects through the lens of translational research. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/med/9780197645444.003.0004Google Scholar
Czobor, P., Kakuszi, B., & Bitter, I. (2022). Placebo response in trials of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A critical reassessment of the evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 48(6), 12281240. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbac061CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de la Fuente-Fernández, R. (2009). The placebo-reward hypothesis: Dopamine and the placebo effect. Parkinsonism & Related Disorders, 15, S72S74. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1353-8020(09)70785-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Pascalis, V., & Scacchia, P. (2019). The influence of reward sensitivity, heart rate dynamics and EEG-delta activity on placebo analgesia. Behavioural Brain Research, 359, 320332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2018.11.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doane, M. J., Sajatovic, M., Weiden, P. J., O’Sullivan, A. K., Maher, S., Bjorner, J. B., Sikora Kessler, A., Carpenter-Conlin, J., Bessonova, L., & Velligan, D. I. (2020). Antipsychotic treatment experiences of people with schizophrenia: patient perspectives from an online survey. Patient Preference and Adherence, 14, 20432054. https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S270020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fraguas, D., Díaz-Caneja, C. M., Pina-Camacho, L., Umbricht, D., & Arango, C. (2019). Predictors of placebo response in pharmacological clinical trials of negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A meta-regression analysis. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 45(1), 5768. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbx192.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galderisi, S. (2023). Promoting schizophrenia research in Europe: The contribution of the European Group for Research in Schizophrenia. World Psychiatry: Official Journal of the World Psychiatric Association (WPA), 22(3), 486487. https://doi.org/10.1002/wps.21100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geng, C. H., Wang, C., Yang, J., Wang, H., Ma, R. Q., Liu, X., & Wang, C. H. (2017). Arginine vasopressin improves the memory deficits in Han Chinese patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Peptides, 97, 815. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.peptides.2017.09.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giordano, G. M., Bucci, P., Mucci, A., Pezzella, P., & Galderisi, S. (2021). Gender differences in clinical and psychosocial features among persons with schizophrenia: A mini review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 789179789179. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.789179CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glassman, A. H., & Roose, S. P. (1981). Delusional depression. A distinct clinical entity?. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38(4), 424427. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780290058006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grosso, F., Barbiani, D., Cavalera, C., Volpato, E., & Pagnini, F. (2024). Risk factors associated with nocebo effects: A review of reviews. Brain, Behavior, & Immunity. Health, 38, 100800. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbih.2024.100800CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haarsma, J., Knolle, F., Griffin, J. D., Taverne, H., Mada, M., Goodyer, I. M., The Nspn Consortium, Fletcher, P. C., & Murray, G. K. (2020). Influence of prior beliefs on perception in early psychosis: Effects of illness stage and hierarchical level of belief. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 129(6), 581598. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000494CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haddad, P. M., & Correll, C. U. (2018). The acute efficacy of antipsychotics in schizophrenia: A review of recent meta-analyses. Therapeutic Advances in Psychopharmacology, 8(11), 303318. https://doi.org/10.1177/2045125318781475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hird, E. J., Diederen, K., Leucht, S., Jensen, K. B., & McGuire, P. (2023). The placebo effect in psychosis: Why it matters and how to measure it. Biological Psychiatry: Global Open Science, 3(4), 605613. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsgos.2023.02.008Google Scholar
Horan, W. P., Sachs, G., Velligan, D. I., Davis, M., Keefe, R. S. E., Khin, N. A., Butlen-Ducuing, F., & Harvey, P. D. (2024). Current and emerging technologies to address the placebo response challenge in CNS clinical trials: Promise, pitfalls, and pathways forward. Innovations in Clinical Neuroscience, 21(1–3), 1930.Google ScholarPubMed
Jarvis, G. E., Iyer, S. N., Andermann, L., & Fung, K. P. (2020). Culture and psychosis in clinical practice. . In A clinical introduction to psychosis: Foundations for clinical psychologists and neuropsychologists (pp. 85112). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-815012-2.00004-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kadakia, A., Catillon, M., Fan, Q., Williams, G. R., Marden, J. R., Anderson, A., Kirson, N., & Dembek, C. (2022). The economic burden of schizophrenia in the United States. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 83(6), 22m14458. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.22m14458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalaria, S. N., Farchione, T. R., Mathis, M. V., Gopalakrishnan, M., Younis, I., Uppoor, R., Mehta, M., Wang, Y., & Zhu, H. (2020). Assessment of similarity in antipsychotic exposure‐response relationships in clinical trials between adults and adolescents with acute exacerbation of schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Pharmacology, 60(7), 848859. https://doi.org/10.1002/jcph.1580CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalaria, S. N., Zhu, H., Farchione, T. R., Mathis, M. V., Gopalakrishnan, M., Uppoor, R., Mehta, M., & Younis, I. (2019). A quantitative justification of similarity in placebo response between adults and adolescents with acute exacerbation of schizophrenia in clinical trials. Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics, 106(5), 10461055. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpt.1501CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaptchuk, T. J., Hemond, C. C., & Miller, F. G. (2020). Placebos in chronic pain: Evidence, theory, ethics, and use in clinical practice. BMJ (Online), 370, m1668. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.m1668Google ScholarPubMed
Kapur, S. (2003). Psychosis as a State of Aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(1), 1323. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.160.1.13CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keefe, R. S. E., Davis, V. G., Harvey, P. D., Atkins, A. S., Haig, G. M., Hagino, O., Marder, S., Hilt, D. C., & Umbricht, D. (2017). Placebo response and practice effects in schizophrenia cognition trials. JAMA Psychiatry (Chicago, Ill.), 74(8), 807814. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.1574CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Khin, N. A., Chen, Y. F., Yang, Y., Yang, P., & Laughren, T. P. (2012). Exploratory analyses of efficacy data from schizophrenia trials in support of new drug applications submitted to the US Food and Drug Administration. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 73(6), 856864. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.11r07539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kinon, B. J. J. P. A., & Watson, S. B. (2011). Placebo response in clinical trials with schizophrenia patients. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 24, 107113. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0b013e32834381b0Google ScholarPubMed
Kozielska, M., Pilla Reddy, V., Johnson, M., de Ridder, F., Vermeulen, A., Liu, J., Groothuis, G. M. M., Danhof, M., & Proost, J. H. (2013). Sensitivity of individual items of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) and items subgroups to differentiate between placebo and drug treatment in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 146(1), 5358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.022CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kumar, R. (2008). Approved and investigational uses of Modafinil: An evidence-based review. Drugs (New York, N.Y.), 68(13), 18031839. https://doi.org/10.2165/00003495-200868130-00003Google ScholarPubMed
Leucht, S., Leucht, C., Huhn, M., Chaimani, A., Mavridis, D., Helfer, B., Samara, M., Rabaioli, M., Bächer, S., Cipriani, A., Geddes, J. R., Salanti, G., & Davis, J. M. (2017). Sixty years of placebo-controlled antipsychotic drug trials in acute schizophrenia: Systematic review, Bayesian meta-analysis, and meta-regression of efficacy predictors. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 927942. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.16121358CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lévesque, M., Potvin, S., Marchand, S., Stip, E., Grignon, S., Pierre, L., Lipp, O., & Goffaux, P. (2012). Pain perception in schizophrenia: Evidence of a specific pain response profile. Pain medicine (Malden, Mass.), 13(12), 15711579. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1526-4637.2012.01505.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mallinckrodt, C. H., Zhang, L., Prucka, W. R., & Millen, B. A. (2010). Signal detection and placebo response in schizophrenia: parallels with depression. Psychopharmacology Bulletin, 43(1), 5372.Google ScholarPubMed
McCutcheon, R. A., Pillinger, T., Mizuno, Y., Montgomery, A., Pandian, H., Vano, L., Marques, T. R., & Howes, O. D. (2021). The efficacy and heterogeneity of antipsychotic response in schizophrenia: A meta-analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 26(4), 13101320. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-019-0502-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Melamed, Y., Doron, A., Stein-Reisner, O., & Bleich, A. (2009). Ethical and scientific perspectives of placebo-controlled trials in schizophrenia. Journal of Clinical Medicine Research, 1(3), 132136. https://doi.org/10.4021/jocmr2009.07.1247Google ScholarPubMed
Murray, D., & Stoessl, A. J. (2013). Mechanisms and therapeutic implications of the placebo effect in neurological and psychiatric conditions. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 140(3), 306318. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pharmthera.2013.07.009.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nagamine, T. (2023). Pain threshold paradox in schizophrenia: A narrative review based on the lastest neuroscience. Psychiatria Danubina, 35(2), 174179. https://doi.org/10.24869/psyd.2023.174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neogi, T., & Colloca, L. (2023). Placebo effects in osteoarthritis: Implications for treatment and drug developmentNature Reviews Rheumatology19(10), 613626. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41584-023-01021-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, H., Waldman, K., Stickley, A., DeVylder, J. E., & Koyanagi, A. (2019). Psychotic experiences and physical health conditions in the United States. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 90, 16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.12.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oliveira, I. R. D., Nunes, P. M., Coutinho, D. M., & Sena, E. P. D. (2009). Review of the efficacy of placebo in comparative clinical trials between typical and atypical antipsychotics. Revista Brasileira de Psiquiatria, 31(1), 5256. https://doi.org/10.1590/S1516-44462009000100013CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ongaro, G., & Kaptchuk, T. J. (2019). Symptom perception, placebo effects, and the Bayesian brain. Pain (Amsterdam), 160(1), 14. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001367Google ScholarPubMed
Paillère‐Martinot, M. L., Galinowski, A., Plaze, M., Andoh, J., Bartrés‐Faz, D., Bellivier, F., Lefaucheur, J. P., Rivière, D., Gallarda, T., Martinot, J. L., & Artiges, E. (2017). Active and placebo transcranial magnetic stimulation effects on external and internal auditory hallucinations of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 135(3), 228238. https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12680CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palermo, S., Giovannelli, F., Bartoli, M., & Amanzio, M. (2019). Are patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders more prone to manifest nocebo-like-effects? A meta-analysis of adverse events in placebo groups of double-blind antipsychotic trials. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 10, 502502. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphar.2019.00502CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patrick, D. L. P. M., Burns, T. M. D. D. F., Morosini, P. M. D., Gagnon, D. D. M. A., Rothman, M. P., & Adriaenssen, I. M. (2010). Measuring social functioning with the personal and social performance scale in patients with acute symptoms of schizophrenia: Interpretation of results of a pooled analysis of three Phase III trials of paliperidone extended-release tablets. Clinical Therapeutics, 32(2), 275292. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinthera.2010.02.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peritogiannis, V. A.-O. X., Ninou, A., & Samakouri, M. A.-O. (2022). Mortality in schizophrenia-spectrum disorders: Recent advances in understanding and management. In Healthcare (Vol. 10, No. 12, p. 2366). MDPI. https://doi.org/10.3390/healthcare10122366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petrovic, P., & Sterzer, P. (2023). Resolving the delusion paradox. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 49(6), 14251436. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbad084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, A., Angelos, P. A., Bond, A., Farina, E., Fredericks, C., Gandhi, J., Greenwald, M., Hernandez-Busot, G., Hosein, G., Kelley, M., Mourgues, C., Palmer, W., Rodriguez-Sanchez, J., Seabury, R., Toribio, S., Vin, R., Weleff, J., Woods, S., & Benrimoh, D. (2025). A computational account of the development and evolution of psychotic symptoms. Biological Psychiatry, 97(2), 117127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2024.08.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Powers, A. R., Mathys, C., & Corlett, P. R. (2017). Pavlovian conditioning-induced hallucinations result from overweighting of perceptual priors. Science (New York, N.Y.), 357(6351), 596600. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aan3458CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reddy, V., Kozielska, M., de Greef, R., Vermeulen, A., & Proost, J. H. (2013). Modelling and simulation of placebo effect: Application to drug development in schizophrenia. Journal of Pharmacokinetics and Pharmacodynamics, 40(3), 377388. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10928-012-9296-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V. P., Kozielska, M., Johnson, M., Suleiman, A. A., Vermeulen, A., Liu, J., de Greef, R., Groothuis, G. M. M., Danhof, M., & Proost, J. H. (2012). Modelling and simulation of the positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) time course and dropout hazard in placebo arms of schizophrenia clinical trials. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 51(4), 261275. https://doi.org/10.2165/11598460-000000000-00000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reddy, V. P., Kozielska, M., Johnson, M., Vermeulen, A., de Greef, R., Liu, J., Groothuis, G. M. M., Danhof, M., & Proost, J. H. (2011). Structural models describing placebo treatment effects in schizophrenia and other neuropsychiatric disorders. Clinical Pharmacokinetics, 50(7), 429450. https://doi.org/10.2165/11590590-000000000-00000CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rief, W., & Petrie, K. J. (2016). Can psychological expectation models be adapted for placebo research? Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 18761876. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01876CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutherford, B. R., Pott, E., Tandler, J. M., Wall, M. M., Roose, S. P., & Lieberman, J. A. (2014). Placebo response in antipsychotic clinical trials: A meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 71(12), 14091421. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.1319.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schenk, L. A., Krimmel, S. R., & Colloca, L. (2017). Observe to get pain relief: Current evidence and potential mechanisms of socially learned pain modulation. Pain (Amsterdam), 158(11), 20772081. https://doi.org/10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000943Google ScholarPubMed
Shafir, R., Israel, M., & Colloca, L. (2023). Harnessing the placebo effect to enhance emotion regulation effectiveness and choice. Scientific Reports, 13(1), 23732373. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-023-29045-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shafir, R., Olson, E., & Colloca, L. (2022). The neglect of sex: A call to action for including sex as a biological variable in placebo and nocebo research. Contemporary Clinical Trials, 116, 106734106734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cct.2022.106734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheffield, J. M., Suthaharan, P., Leptourgos, P., & Corlett, P. R. (2022). Belief updating and paranoia in individuals with schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 7(11), 11491157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2022.03.013Google ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P., Adams, R. A., Fletcher, P., Frith, C., Lawrie, S. M., Muckli, L., Petrovic, P., Uhlhaas, P., Voss, M., & Corlett, P. R. (2018). The predictive coding account of psychosis. Biological Psychiatry, 84(9), 634643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.05.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sterzer, P., Voss, M., Schlagenhauf, F., & Heinz, A. (2019). Decision-making in schizophrenia: A predictive-coding perspective. NeuroImage (Orlando, Fla.), 190, 133143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.074Google ScholarPubMed
Stroup, T. S. (2007). Heterogeneity of treatment effects in schizophrenia. The American Journal of Medicine, 120(4 Suppl 1), S2631. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.amjmed.2007.02.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strzelecki, D., Kałużyńska, O., Szyburska, J., & Wysokiński, A. (2016). MMP-9 serum levels in schizophrenic patients during treatment augmentation with sarcosine (results of the PULSAR study). International Journal of Molecular Sciences, 17(7), 10751075. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijms17071075CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stubbs, B., Koyanagi, A., Veronese, N., Vancampfort, D., Solmi, M., Gaughran, F., Carvalho, A. F., Lally, J., Mitchell, A. J., Mugisha, J., & Correll, C. U. (2016). Physical multimorbidity and psychosis: Comprehensive cross sectional analysis including 242,952 people across 48 low- and middle-income countries. BMC Medicine, 14(1), 189. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12916-016-0734-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taylor, D. M., Smith, L., Gee, S. H., & Nielsen, J. (2012). Augmentation of clozapine with a second antipsychotic - a meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 125(1), 1524. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01792.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsuang, M. T., & Faraone, S. V. (1995). The case for heterogeneity in the etiology of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 17(2), 161175. https://doi.org/10.1016/0920-9964(95)00057-SCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turkington, D., Spencer, H., Lebert, L., & Dudley, R. (2017). Befriending: Active placebo or effective psychotherapy? British Journal of Psychiatry, 211(1), 56. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.116.197467CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van de Laar, N., Henter, I., Bartko, J. J., & Wyatt, R. J. (2001). Adverse events during a placebo phase for inpatients with chronic schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 50(7), 487492. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0006-3223(01)01225-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waltz, J. A., & Gold, J. M. (2016). Motivational deficits in schizophrenia and the representation of expected value. Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation, 27, 375410. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2015_385CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wartolowska, K. (2019). The nocebo effect as a source of bias in the assessment of treatment effects. F1000Research, 8, 5. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.17611.2Google ScholarPubMed
Weilnhammer, V., Röd, L., Eckert, A.-L., Stuke, H., Heinz, A., & Sterzer, P. (2020). Psychotic experiences in schizophrenia and sensitivity to sensory evidence. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(4), 927936. https://doi.org/10.1093/schbul/sbaa003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weimer, K., Colloca, L., & Enck, P. (2015). Placebo effects in psychiatry: Mediators and moderators. The Lancet. Psychiatry, 2(3), 246257.10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00092-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Welge, J. A., & Keck, P. E. Jr. (2003). Moderators of placebo response to antipsychotic treatment in patients with schizophrenia: A meta-regression. Psychopharmacology, 166, 110. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00213-002-1299-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitton, A. E., Treadway, M. T., & Pizzagalli, D. A. (2015). Reward processing dysfunction in major depression, bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 28(1), 712. https://doi.org/10.1097/YCO.0000000000000122CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Summary of study- and patient-centered factors influencing placebo effects in SSDs as identified by Hird et al. (2023). This meta-analysis identified several study- and patient-centered factors that may play a role in elevated placebo effects for some individuals with schizophrenia.

Figure 1

Table 1. Future directions for placebo research in SSDs