Hostname: page-component-54dcc4c588-sq2k7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-10-11T12:55:32.990Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The paradox of stress in English

Is it vital or unimportant?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  07 October 2025

Laurie Bauer*
Affiliation:
School of Linguistics and Applied Language Studies, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand

Abstract

Despite comments in the ELT literature on the importance of word-stress for comprehensibility in English, there are many places where native speakers of English appear to pay it little attention, showing systematic variation as well as errors. At the very least, there is a paradox here, in that learners are told to get a feature right that native speakers feel free to ignore. More detailed consideration, though, shows that matters are not as simple as this implies. In this paper, several types of stress variation in English are exemplified, and it is also shown that in everyday usage native English speakers are flexible in what they will accept where stress is concerned. This raises questions about the best model for teaching stress in English as a second or foreign language. A simple right/wrong dichotomy is unlikely to reflect native usage.

Information

Type
Shorter Article
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2025. Published by Cambridge University Press.

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Article purchase

Temporarily unavailable

References

Bauer, Laurie. 1994. Watching English Change. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar
Bauer, Laurie. 2024. English Phonetics, Phonology and Spelling for the English Language Teacher. New York and London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Benrabah, Mohamed. 1997. “Word-Stress – a Source of Unintelligibility in English.” IRAL 35: 157165.10.1515/iral.1997.35.3.157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berg, Thomas. 1999. “Stress Variation in British and American English.” World Englishes 18: 123143.10.1111/1467-971X.00128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Gillian. 1977. Listening to Spoken English. London: Longman.Google Scholar
Cruttenden, Alan. 2001. Gimson’s Pronunciation of English. 6th ed. London: Arnold.Google Scholar
Cutler, Anne. 2015. “Lexical Stress in English Pronunciation.” In The Handbook of English Pronunciation, edited by Reed, Marnie and Levis, John M., 106124. Chichester: Wiley Blackwell.10.1002/9781118346952.ch6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fudge, Erik. 1984. English Word-Stress. London: George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Grabe, Esther, and Warren, Paul. 1995. “Stress Shift: Do Speakers Do It or Listeners Hear It?” In Papers in Laboratory Phonology IV: Phonology and Phonetic Evidence, edited by Connell, Bruce and Arvaniti, Amalia, 95110. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511554315.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Eron. 2024. “Prosodic Dissonance.” M[usic]T[heory]O[nline] 30. https://mtosmt.org/issues/mto.24.30.2/mto.24.30.2.smith.html.Google Scholar
Vogel, Irene, Bunnell, H. Timothy, and Hoskins, Steven. 1995. “The Phonology and Phonetics of the Rhythm Rule.” In Papers in Laboratory Phonology IV: Phonology and Phonetic Evidence, edited by Connell, Bruce and Arvaniti, Amalia, 111127. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511554315.009CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, J.C. 2008. Longman Pronunciation Dictionary. 3rd ed. Harlow: Longman.Google Scholar